TICS-664; No of Pages 4 Update Research Focus Theory of mind, language and the temporoparietal junction mystery Josef Perner and Markus Aichhorn University of Salzburg and Center for Neurocognitive Research, Hellbrunnerstraße 34, 5020 Salzburg, Austria Brain imaging of adults during false-belief story tasks consistently shows activation of the temporoparietal junction in English-speaking Americans and German-speaking Europeans. Kobayashi et al. find this observation in adult English speakers but not in English-speaking children or in English–Japanese bilingual persons. This finding suggests a cultural or linguistic influence on location of brain function and argues against maturation of innately specified neural substrates. It is reminiscent of effects of linguistic development, bilingualism and cultural differences on theory of mind development. Introduction The paper by Kobayashi et al. [1] caught our attention for about as many reasons as there are words in its title: neural basis, theory of mind (ToM), linguistic/cultural effects, children. A central aspect of ToM is that the mind takes a perspective on the world. Children’s appreciation of this fact has been assessed by their understanding of false beliefs, for example to understand that a person who does not witness an unexpected transfer of an object will then look for the object in its original, but now empty, place. Saxe and Kanwisher [2] adopted this paradigm in the form of short story vignettes for brain imaging with adults to explore the neural correlates of ToM reasoning (Studies 1– 6, M, P and A in Figure 1). For contrast, they used structurally very similar photo vignettes, also adopted from work with normally developing children and children with autism. Because of the structural similarity between the false-belief and photo task, this contrast is considered to be a good way to filter out the specific mental aspects of false beliefs. Most earlier studies used Happé’s Strange Stories [3], in which a burglar mistakenly thinks that a policeman has caught him when the policeman merely wants to return the burglar’s lost glove, in contrast to physical event stories – a cat triggering the burglar alarm (F, G, H and V in Figure 1). One recent study contrasted false beliefs with true beliefs (S). Brain imaging of false-belief reasoning shows remarkable consistency Activation differences were consistently reported [2,4] within the anterior cingulate/paracingulate area in the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC), the temporoparietal junction (TPJ)/posterior superior temporal sulcus (pSTS) area, areas along the middle temporal gyrus down to the Corresponding author: Perner, J. ([email protected]). temporal pole (TP) and the posterior cingulate cortex/precuneus region (PC). In particular, bilateral activation in the TPJ was found across all 15 studies (with only three exceptions in Figure 1: H only left, M only right, and V neither). The importance of the TPJ for reasoning about beliefs is also highlighted by lesion studies showing that a patient with extensive bilateral damage to the mPFC (including an area activated by most ToM studies [4]) could still solve false-belief tasks [5], and patients with damage in the left TPJ showed specific impairment on false-belief tasks, whereas damage to the mPFC resulted in a wider impairment including executive problems [6]. Figure 1 illustrates the consistency of imaging results for the right hemisphere showing the peak voxels reported in these studies on adults from Western cultures speaking IndoGermanic languages: English-speaking Americans (Studies 1–6 and M in Figure 1) or British (F, G) or German-speaking Europeans (A, P, S and V). Surprising results from brain images of bilingual children Kobayashi et al. [1] tested 12 bilingual Japanese–Englishspeaking and 12 monolingual 9-year-old children from the New York Metropolitan area on second-order belief stories (what one person thinks another person thinks) presented verbally (either in English or Japanese) or as nonverbal cartoons. Non-ToM control stories were modelled after physical-event stories used in earlier studies [3]. Only bilingual children tested with Japanese stories showed activation in one of the typical four areas (mPFC). Most surprisingly, even monolingual English children (‘ ’ in Figure 1) did not show any activation in the TPJ. Why did this study produce different findings from those of previous studies? The investigation by Kobayashi et al. differs from earlier studies In the study by Kobayashi et al. [1] children rather than adults and second-order beliefs instead of first-order beliefs were tested, and some children were bilingual and had, therefore, a somewhat different linguistic/cultural background. Any effect of materials can be checked by looking at the earlier study by Kobayashi et al. [7] on adults. Reassuringly, here there was activation in or near three of the typical areas in the right hemisphere: TPJ, PC and mPFC. This finding suggests that the lack of activation reported for children is not due to different story materials but to the age and background of the participants. In particular, the hitherto unanimously reported activation of the TPJ in (presumably) monolingual adults 1 TICS-664; No of Pages 4 Update Trends in Cognitive Sciences Vol.xxx No.x Figure 1. Location of peak voxels in y- and z-space coordinates (outlines of cerebral structures are only approximate) in three lateral and medial areas reported in imaging studies of false-belief (FB) processing in comparison with data reported by Kobayashi et al. [1,7] for monolingual English speakers. Abbreviations: mPFC, medial prefrontal cortex; MTG, middle temporal gyrus; PC, precuneus; TP, temporal pole; TPJ, temporoparietal junction. seems to be absent in Japanese–English bilingual children as well as in 9-year-old monolingual children. This is potentially an important finding. The difference in findings has potentially important implications We focus on TPJ-R (TPJ in the right hemisphere), which Saxe and Kanwisher [2] suggested was specifically associated with ToM and false-belief understanding. There are signs in the data by Kobayashi et al. [1] that activation in the TPJ when dealing with false beliefs might be modulated by development and environmental factors, such as cultural or linguistic background or bilingualism – which of these factors accounts for the observed differences is not known. Nevertheless, the data of Kobayashi et al. suggest that the cerebral location for ToM cannot be prespecified in a genetic code awaiting maturation (as assumed by modularity theorists [8]). The specificity claim of Saxe and Kanwisher is still highly contested Mitchell [9] pointed out that basic attentional processes (as tested by invalid cueing and flanker paradigm) activate the same region of the TPJ-R as does the false-belief–photo contrast, as well as other processes [10]. Mitchell argues that it is more plausible that TPJ-R activation is due to such basic attention-shifting processes rather than to ToM, but 2 the author does not identify any relevant attentional aspect that distinguishes the false-belief from the photo task. Nevertheless, if the TPJ activation does reflect such basic attention-shifting processes, the findings by Kobayashi et al. would suggest something even more remarkable, namely, that even the brain location of very basic attentional processes is subject to linguistic/cultural variation or bilingualism. Methodological reservations The reported activation differences are unusually small and there is remarkably little coherence in regions reported for the different groups (bilinguals–monolinguals adults–children) studied and contrasts analysed. Hence the lack of activation of the TPJ in children and in bilingual adults might be due to a lack of power rather than a systematic difference between the groups. For instance, a direct contrast between monolingual English speakers and bilingual persons (Table 3 in Ref. [1]) was barely significant for cartoons but not for verbal stories. Moreover, a third study by Kobayashi et al. [11] with English-speaking monolingual children and adults showed only a main effect for condition (ToM versus non-ToM) in the TPJ bilaterally but no clear age difference apart from a difficult to interpret triple interaction among age, story type (cartoon, verbal) and condition. The reason for the weak results and lack of coherence might lie in the design TICS-664; No of Pages 4 Update of treating the monolingual and bilingual group differently in the number of conditions (bilingual persons were given an additional story block for the second language) and in the choice of control task (e.g. opponents fighting), which might itself have elicited a non-negligible amount of ToM thoughts about emotions – and more than a photo task would have elicited. Moreover, from the text it seems that the ToM stories in the cartoons were quite different (e.g. thinking that the other ate one’s food – a moral issue) from those in the language conditions (e.g. thinking that the other missed a news item). Although there are potential problems with the studies by Kobayashi et al. [1,7,11], their findings are nevertheless suggestive. Data indicate potential parallels between cultural– linguistic effects on brain function and on development of ToM Cultural differences In recent years many reports appeared to show cognitive style differences between Eastern and Western cultures. Chinese adults show stronger perspective-taking abilities than do Americans in communication games [12]. However, Chinese children seem to have no advantage over Americans in ToM tasks even though they perform considerably better on executive tasks [13]. Language development There are persistent reports that performance on falsebelief tests relates to verbal intelligence [14] and, perhaps, specifically to a child’s ability to process tensed that-complements (e.g. ‘‘he said that the chocolate is in the cupboard’’) [15]. This link with language is most impressively underlined by orally taught deaf children who learn sign language belatedly. They not only have a language delay of some years but also are equally delayed on passing the false-belief test [16]. Moreover, first-generation adult signers of Nicaraguan sign language, who developed the language from scratch to a moderate level of grammatical sophistication, have serious problems with the false-belief test – problems not seen in second-generation signers, for whom that language has reached a more advanced level of complexity (J. E. Pyers, PhD Thesis, University of California, 2004). An interesting counterpoint to these developmental dependencies is the fact that ToM performance can persist in the face of even severe impairment of grammar due to neurological insult [17]. Bilingualism Goetz [18] and A. Kovacs (unpublished) report superior performance by bilingual children on false-belief tasks. Considering that understanding false beliefs has been found to relate especially strongly to children’s ability to admit alternative names for things [19], the greater familiarity of bilingual children with alternative common names might be responsible for their ToM advantage. Bialystok [20] explains performance differences on the ToM-related appearance–reality task as a result of better attentional inhibition processes. This is an intriguing idea considering the involvement of TPJ-R in false-belief processing and in attentional refocusing as mentioned above. Trends in Cognitive Sciences Vol.xxx No.x Concluding remarks The findings of Kobayashi et al. do not solve the problem of assigning a specific functional role to the TPJ. If anything, they deepen the mystery of what the TPJ might be responsible for. However, their data raise the possibility that the brain physiology of this mysterious function might be liable to cultural and linguistic variation with intriguing links to similar developmental variation. This is a provocative suggestion that awaits confirmation. Acknowledgements We thank the anonymous reviewers for their helpful comments and Benjamin Weiss for his help with the meta-analysis chart and the European and Austrian Science Funds (ESF/FWF project I93-G15 ‘‘Metacognition of Perspective Differences’’) for financial support. References 1 Kobayashi, C. et al. (2007) Cultural and linguistic effects on neural bases of ‘Theory of Mind’ in American and Japanese children. Brain Res. 1164, 95–107 2 Saxe, R. and Kanwisher, N. (2003) People thinking about thinking people: the role of the temporo-parietal junction in ‘theory of mind’. Neuroimage 19, 1835–1842 3 Happé, F. et al. (1996) ‘Theory of mind’ in the brain. Evidence from a PET scan study of Asperger syndrome. Neuroreport 8, 197–201 4 Frith, U. and Frith, C.D. (2003) Development and neurophysiology of mentalizing. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 358, 459– 473 5 Bird, C.M. et al. (2004) The impact of extensive medial frontal lobe damage on ‘Theory of Mind’ and cognition. Brain 127, 914– 928 6 Apperly, I.A. et al. (2004) Frontal and temporo-parietal lobe contributions to theory of mind: Neuropsychological evidence from a false belief task with reduced language and executive demands. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 16, 1773–1784 7 Kobayashi, C. et al. (2006) Cultural and linguistic influence on neural bases of ‘Theory of Mind’: an fMRI study with Japanese bilinguals. Brain Lang. 98, 210–220 8 Leslie, A.M. (2005) Developmental parallels in understanding minds and bodies. Trends Cogn. Sci. 9, 459–462 9 Mitchell, J.P. (2008) Activity in right temporo-parietal junction is not selective for theory of mind. Cereb. Cortex 18, 262–271 10 Decety, J. and Lamm, C. (2007) The role of the right temporoparietal junction in social interaction: How low-level computational processes contribute to meta-cognition. Neuroscientist 13, 580–593 11 Kobayashi, C. et al. (2007) Children’s and adults’ neural bases of verbal and nonverbal ‘theory of mind’. Neuropsychologia 45, 1522–1532 12 Wu, S. and Keysar, B. (2007) The effect of culture on perspective taking. Psychol. Sci. 18, 600–606 13 Sabbagh, M.A. et al. (2006) The development of executive functioning and theory of mind. A comparison of Chinese and U. S. preschoolers. Psychol. Sci. 17, 74–81 14 Milligan, K. et al. (2007) Language and theory of mind meta-analysis of the relation between language ability and false-belief understanding. Child Dev. 78, 622–646 15 de Villiers, J.G. and Pyers, J.E. (2002) Complements to cognition: a longitudinal study of the relationship between complex syntax and false-belief understanding. Cogn. Dev. 17, 1037–1060 16 Peterson, C.C. and Siegal, M. (1995) Deafness, conversation and theory of mind. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 36, 459–474 17 Varley, R. et al. (2001) Severe impairment in grammar does not preclude theory of mind. Neurocase 7, 489–493 18 Goetz, P.J. (2003) The effects of bilingualism on theory of mind development. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 6, 1–15 19 Perner, J. et al. (2002) Theory of mind finds its Piagetian perspective: why alternative naming comes with understanding belief. Cogn. Dev. 17, 1451–1472 20 Bialystok, E. and Senman, L. (2004) Executive processes in appearance-reality tasks: The role of inhibition of attention and symbolic representation. Child Dev. 75, 562–579 3 TICS-664; No of Pages 4 Update 21 Fletcher, P.C. et al. (1995) Other minds in the brain: a functional imaging study of ‘theory of mind’ in story comprehension. Cognition 57, 109–128 22 Gallagher, H. et al. (2000) Reading the mind in cartoons and stories: an fMRI study of ‘theory of mind’ in verbal and nonverbal tasks. Neuropsychologia 38, 11–21 23 Vogeley, K. et al. (2001) Mind reading: neural mechanisms of theory of mind and self-perspective. NeuroImage 14, 170–181 24 Saxe, R. and Wexler, A. (2005) Making sense of another mind: the role of the right temporo-parietal junction. Neuropsychologia 43, 1391–1399 25 Saxe, R. and Powell, L.J. (2006) It’s the thought that counts: specifc brain regions for one component of theory of mind. Psychol. Sci. 17, 692–699 4 Trends in Cognitive Sciences Vol.xxx No.x 26 Saxe, R. et al. (2006) Reading minds versus following rules: dissociating theory of mind and executive control in the brain. Soc. Neurosci. 1, 284– 298 27 Young, L. et al. (2007) The neural basis of the interaction between theory of mind and moral judgment. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 104, 8235–8240 28 Perner, J. et al. (2006) Thinking of mental and other representations: the roles of left and right temporo-parietal junction. Soc. Neurosci. 1, 245–258 29 Sommer, M. et al. (2007) Neural correlates of true and false belief reasoning. NeuroImage 35, 1378–1384 1364-6613/$ – see front matter ß 2008 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2008.02.001 Available online xxxxxx
© Copyright 2025 Paperzz