Professor Petger Schaberg Academic Article The Effects of a Positive Writing Task on Test Anxiety Immediately Before an Exam Abstract Test anxiety is a common experience on college campuses. Many treatments for test anxiety have been developed, but few have focused on creating a brief intervention that could be implemented immediately before an exam. Studies have focused on test anxiety as it pertains to tasks performed in a lab, rather than in realistic testing environments, and on long-term treatments instead of brief interventions. A brief, effective intervention for test anxiety, able to be administered immediately before an exam, could be easily included at the beginning of any test. This type of intervention has the potential to help large numbers of test anxious students in a short amount of time. The present study tested the effect of writing about a successful experience on symptoms of test anxiety immediately preceding a regularly scheduled exam. 30 undergraduate students arriving for a chemistry exam participated. Subjects were randomly assigned to a control or treatment group. The control group was directed to write about their morning routine, while the treatment group was directed to write about successfully overcoming a challenge. Anxiety in males versus females was also examined. No significant difference was found between the treatment and control groups nor was a significant difference found between males and females. Possible explanations for these findings and future research directions are discussed. Introduction Test Anxiety Test anxiety has come to be a common experience in modern society as academic achievement is increasingly used to evaluate individuals in the professional world. The adverse effects of test anxiety often result in an underestimation of one’s abilities and reduced access to educational and occupational opportunities (Spielberger and Vagg 1995). Most people experience the physiological and psychological components of test anxiety at some point or another. Common physiological symptoms include sweating, rapid pulse, “butterfilies” in the stomach, and other uncomfortable sensations. Psychological symptoms include feeling distracted, worried, incompetent, and self-critical. Test anxiety has been found to be negatively correlated with performance in experimental and normal student populations; this correlation is strongest for the cognitive components of test anxiety (Pekrun 2001). It is thought that selfcentered, task-irrelevant worry cognitions are detrimental to performance because they occupy essential cognitive resources (Spielberger and Vagg 1995). Because cognitive aspects of test anxiety are so influential to performance, the present study uses a writing task designed to alter negative cognitions as a means of combating the negative impact of test anxiety. Negative selfview has been found to be a particularly strong predictor of test anxiety as well (Wong 2008). This finding supports the hypothesis that writing about a positive experience and overcoming a challenge will reduce test anxiety by increasing confidence in addition to reducing the cognitive load. Writing Interventions Several writing exercises have been shown to alleviate test anxiety and improve performance on exams. A previous study done at the University of Colorado found that a writing exercise involving values affirmation helped female students perform better in physics classes, narrowing the gender gap in science achievement (Miyake et al. 2010). Women who wrote about their most important values at the beginning of the course scored an average of one full letter grade above women in the control group. While Miyake’s study did not focus on the relationship between the values affirmation task and anxiety, it did reveal that a simple writing task can dramatically improve academic performance. Lumley and Provenzano (2003) found that anxious students earned higher grades when they wrote about their worries and fears at the beginning of the semester. This was thought to be due to the fact that disclosing worries freed cognitive processes and improved mood. Similarly, Ramirez and Beilock (2011) found that taking ten minutes before an exam to write about worries improved exam scores in test anxious students. The Ramirez and Beilock study was especially significant because the writing exercise was administered immediately before a regularly scheduled exam. This finding indicates that other types of writing interventions for test anxiety may also be successfully administered immediately before exams. Nelson and Knight (2010) found that participants who completed a positive-thought writing task before a quiz exhibited more positive affect, less test anxiety, better performance on the quiz, and were more likely to appraise the quiz confidently than participants in a control group. Their findings were in agreement with previous theories that positive affect, optimism, and self efficacy may be effective coping mechanisms in stressful situations, and the writing exercise they devised seemed to employ these mechanisms. Because Nelson and Knight found such encouraging results from a simple writing exercise, this task was chosen to be tested in the current study. One limitation to Nelson and Knight’s findings was that the stressor was a 15question pop quiz. This stressor is very different from the lengthy and heavier-weighted exams that college students encounter more frequently. The present study chose to administer this writing task to students in the minutes before a 160 point exam worth 15% of the total class grade. This setting more accurately represents the test anxiety most typical to undergraduates at the University of Colorado. Methods Procedure A convenience sample of 30 students (11 females and 19 males) from an introductory chemistry class at the University of Colorado, Boulder was used. Participants were all undergraduates with ages ranging from 18 to 24 years old. Participation was voluntary and anonymous. All research methods were approved by the Institutional Review Board for class projects involving human research subjects. Participants were randomly assigned to one of two writing activities. The treatment group was assigned to write about a past experience in which they successfully overcame a challenge and experienced feelings of pride and joy. The control group was assigned to write about their morning routine. After the writing assignment participants completed a brief survey measuring physiological and cognitive symptoms of anxiety. The writing activities were completed immediately before a regularly scheduled exam in order to gain an accurate measure of test anxiety in a realistic environment. Measurement The Symptoms of Test Anxiety Scale (STAS) created by Berk and Nanda (2006) was used as a measure of anxiety. This scale incorporates aspects of five of the most frequently used measures of test anxiety in a very concise checklist format. The various measures used to formulate the STAS were the Test Anxiety Scale, Reactions to Tests Scale, Revised Test Anxiety Scale, and Cognitive Test Anxiety Scale (Berk and Nanda 2006). These scales all measure various physiological and cognitive dimensions of test anxiety, but are lengthy and complex. The comprehensive STAS was used because its brevity and ease allows it to be administered immediately before an exam. Subjects were directed to put a check next to any symptoms they were currently experiencing. Each check was scored as 1 and each blank as 0. A few control symptoms were also included (e.g. relaxed, confident) and scored as -1 if checked. Subjects were also asked to report if they were sick the day of the exam. Because symptoms of anxiety can be similar to symptoms of illness, results from subjects who reported being sick were not included in the study. Statistical Analysis A two-way ANOVA was used to examine the relationships between the writing task and gender with anxiety scores. The statistical program R was used to run the ANOVA. Post- hoc tests were used to reveal more about specific relationships. A challenge involved in the statistical analysis of this data was that the sample was unbalanced. Balance refers to the number of subjects in each subgroup of the study. The female control group had five subjects, the female treatment group had three subjects, the male control group had six subjects, and the male treatment group had eight subjects. This lack of balance may slightly affect the outcome of the ANOVA, but correcting this problem requires much more advanced software than was available for this study. Results There was no significant difference found between the treatment group and the control group (p=.5306). The mean number of symptoms of test anxiety reported by the treatment group was 12.92 and the mean for the control group was 11.65. Additionally, there was no significant difference found for the main effect of gender on test anxiety (p=.7953). The mean for males was 12.0 and the mean for females was 12.57. There was also no significant interaction between gender and writing task group on symptoms of test anxiety (p=.5578). Details of the results are displayed in the following tables and graphs. Mean Number of Test Anxiety Symptoms Reported Control Treatment Females 12.8 12.33 12.57 Males 10.5 13.5 12.0 11.65 12.92 Table 1 Anova Table (Type II tests) Sum Sq Df F value Pr (>F) Gender 2.83 1 0.0693 0.7953 Group 16.70 1 0.4090 0.5306 Gender:Group Residuals 14.57 734.97 1 18 0.3568 0.5578 Table 2 Plot of Means 10 12 14 c tx 8 mean of TestAnxiety1$score 16 TestAnxiety1$group f m Figure 1 The x-axis depicts gender. The treatment group is represented by the red triangles and the control group is represented by black circles. TestAnxiety1$gender Discussion Possible Explanations for Results Because previous studies (Nelson and Knight 2010; Miyake et al. 2010; Lumley and Provenzano 2003) have found similar writing exercises to be effective in reducing test related anxiety and improving academic performance, it was surprising that the positive writing task in this study had no effect. It appears that this specific writing task is not an effective intervention immediately before an exam. One explanation for this may be that the task required too much cognitive effort. Subjects had to remember a positive experience in which they overcame an obstacle, describe their feelings, and explain what this experience meant for them. It is possible that this task provoked already stressed cognitive resources. This could have resulted in added stress or prevented subjects from thoroughly completing the task. It may be that this type of treatment is most effective when administered in a state of lower anxiety, a few hours or days prior to the exam. Another explanation for this lack of treatment effect in this study is the possibility that the control task had an unexpected effect on anxiety. One hypothesis is that in a state of high anxiety, such as immediately before an exam, an easy task may reduce symptoms of anxiety and increase confidence and feelings of accomplishment. It requires very little cognitive effort to describe one’s typical morning routine. This task can also be completed in list form, rather than complete sentences. Such an easy task may actually reduce the cognitive load and have a calming effect. Possible Causes of Type I Error One possible source of error in this study was the lack of control over the environment. Because the limited time frame required several people to complete the study at the same time, subjects would occasionally talk to one another during the study, make jokes, or fail to thoroughly complete the task. Many subjects were also in a hurry to find a seat or do some last minute studying. Clear directions were provided on the worksheets, yet six participants failed to complete the writing task, only checking symptoms of anxiety. The results from these six participants were not included. After throwing out these scores, and those of participants reporting illness, the total number of subjects in the study was brought to 22, causing the sample to be rather unbalanced. Of the participant who followed directions, only half of the treatment group thoroughly completed the writing task, possibly masking any benefit thorough completion may have had. Limitations Time and money were the main limitations of this study. The short time frame allotted for this experiment, required the fastest type of approval from the Institutional Review Board. The class project approval was granted very quickly, but required that all participant information be kept anonymous. This meant that data on test performance could not be measured. In similar studies, researchers were able to offer students extra credit on the exam and obtain 100% participation rates. Because this was an undergraduate class project, offering extra credit was not possible. With zero funding for this experiment, there was little else to offer participants. There was also little incentive for volunteers to help run the study. As a result, many test-anxious students declined to participate and research volunteers with no background in conducting psychological studies were used. Future Research Directions Perhaps the most interesting finding in this study was the indication that the control task may reduce anxiety. Future investigations could examine this possibility more closely. It is unclear whether the specific task of writing about one’s morning routine is calming, or if the simple act of performing any easy cognitive task reduces anxiety. If the latter is true, placing a few easy questions at the beginning of an exam could prove to be an effortless way to reduce anxiety and improve performance. Future studies may also look to compare the effectiveness of several brief interventions or combinations of interventions that could be performed immediately before and exam. An example of this would be a comparison of values affirmation, worry disclosure, and positive- thought writing tasks. While this study may have failed to identify an effective and brief intervention for test anxiety, it has provided interesting insight into the direction of further research, and added to the increasing knowledge about the complex nature of test anxiety. Appendix A. Symptoms of Test Anxiety Scale (STAS; Berk and Nanda 2006) References Berk, R.A. & Nanda, J. (2006). A randomized trial of humor effects on test anxiety and test performance. Humor: International Journal of Humor Research, 19(4), 425-454. Hembree, R. (1988). Correlates, causes, effects, and treatment of test anxiety [Abstract]. Review of Educational Research, 58(1), 47-77. Lumley, M.A. & Provenzano, K.M. (2003). Stress management through written emotional disclosure improves academic performance among college students with physical symptoms. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95(3), 641-649. Miyake, A., Kost-Smith, L.E., Finklestein, N.D., Pollock, S.J., Cohen, G.L., & Ito, T.A. (2010). Reducing the gender achievement gap in college science: A classroom study of values affirmation. Science, 330(6008), 1234-1237. Nelson, D.W. & Knight, A.E. (2010). The power of positive recollections: Reducing test anxiety and enhancing college student efficacy and performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 40(3), 732-745. Pekrun, R.(2001). Test anxiety and academic achievement. In International Encyclopedia of the Social and Behavioral Sciences (Vol. 23, pp. 15610- 15614). Oxford, UK: Elsevier Science, Ltd. Putwian, D.W., Woods, K.A., & Symes, W. (2010). Personal and situational predictors of test anxiety of students in post-compulsory education. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 80, 137-160. Putwain, D.W., Connors, L., & Symes, W. (2010). Do cognitive distortions mediate the test anxiety—examination performance relationship? Educational Psychology, 30(1), 11-26. Ramirez, G. & Beilock, S.L. (2011). Writing about testing worries boosts exam performance in the classroom. Science, 331(6014), 211-213. Sapp, M. (1999). Test anxiety: Applied research, assessment, and treatment interventions (2nd ed.). Lanham, MD: University Press of America. Spielberger, C.D. & Vagg, P.R. (1995). Test anxiety: Theory, assessment, and treatment. Bristol, PA: Taylor & Francis. Wong, S.S. (2008). The relations of cognitive triad, dysfunctional attitudes, automatic thoughts, and irrational beliefs with test anxiety. Current Psychology, 27, 177-191. Zeidner, M. (1998). Test anxiety: The state of the art. New York, NY: Plenum Press.
© Copyright 2025 Paperzz