MSE and beliefs about forgetting and aging - 1 1 Title 2 Older adults’ beliefs about forgetting and aging predict memory self-efficacy above and 3 beyond actual memory performance and mental health 4 Running head 5 MSE and beliefs about forgetting and aging 6 Authors 7 Fanny VALLETab, Stefan AGRIGOROAEIc, Marine BEAUDOINb, Nathalie FOURNETd, 8 Adeline PAIGNONb, Jean-Luc ROULINd, Olivier DESRICHARDa 9 Institutional affiliation Groupe de Recherche en Psychologie de la Santé, Faculté de Psychologie et des Sciences de 10 a 11 l’éducation & Swiss National Centre of Competence in Research LIVES - Overcoming 12 Vulnerability: Life Course Perspectives, Geneva University, Geneva, Switzerland 13 b 14 Chambéry, France 15 c 16 Neuve, Belgium 17 d 18 Chambéry, France 19 Correspondence 20 Fanny Vallet 21 Groupe de Recherche en Psychologie de la Santé 22 Faculté de Psychologie et des Sciences de l’éducation 23 Geneva University 24 Geneva, Switzerland 25 +41(0) 22.379.97.63; [email protected]; [email protected] Laboratoire Inter-universitaire de Psychologie (LIP, EA 4145), Université de Savoie, Psychological Sciences Research Institute, Université catholique de Louvain, Louvain-la- Laboratoire de Psychologie et Neurocognition (LPNC, UMR 5105), Université de Savoie, MSE and beliefs about forgetting and aging - 2 1 2 Acknowledgements 3 We thank Rhône-Alpes Regional Council for their financial support. 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 MSE and beliefs about forgetting and aging - 3 1 2 Abstract 3 Memory Self-Efficacy (MSE) in older adults is linked to memory decline and mental health. 4 However, the relation between MSE and older adults’ beliefs about forgetting and aging is 5 rarely evaluated. In the present study, we hypothesized that beliefs about forgetting and aging 6 predict MSE for older adults, regardless of actual memory performance and mental health. 7 We analyzed data from a community sample of 298 participants aged between 57 and 87 8 years, using three indicators of MSE (including one of perceived temporal change in 9 memory), a measure of beliefs about forgetting and aging, and measures of memory 10 performance, depression, trait-anxiety, and subjective health. The results support our 11 hypothesis, thereby highlighting the need to consider adherence to beliefs about forgetting 12 and aging when investigating variations in MSE in older adults. 13 14 15 16 Keywords 17 Memory self-efficacy, aging, beliefs about forgetting and aging 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 MSE and beliefs about forgetting and aging - 4 1 2 3 Introduction The importance of considering both the cold (cognitive system efficiency) and hot 4 (motivation) determinants of behaviors when studying cognitive functioning has been 5 repeatedly highlighted by researchers (e.g., Esposito, Rochat, Juillerat Van der Linden, Lekeu, 6 Charnallet & Van der Linden, 2014; Hess, Emery & Neupert, 2012; Penningroth & Scott, 7 2007). This issue is particularly important when research shows quantitative differences in 8 cognitive performance between younger and older people, and when such results are viewed 9 as a consequence of differences in cognitive system efficiency, without considering 10 motivational explanations. In fact, motivational factors may influence how much effort older 11 people expend in performing cognitive tasks (Barber & Mather, 2013; Desrichard & Kopetz, 12 2005; Hess, 2005; Hess, Emery, & Neupert, 2012; Kit, Tuokko & Mateer, 2008; Lamont, 13 Swift & Abrams, 2015), so it is important to consider older people’s motivation to succeed on 14 a cognitive task when investigating memory decline and attempting to assess true memory 15 abilities. Furthermore, memory is a central factor in older adults’ self-concept and life- 16 satisfaction (Dark-Freudeman, West, & Viverito, 2006; Stephan et al., 2011). The present 17 paper examines how beliefs about memory and aging influence elders' motivational resources 18 relating to memory performance. 19 The motivational approach to memory decline has focused on several beliefs about 20 memory. For example, beliefs about memory functioning, strategies to control memory 21 functioning, and strategies to mitigate memory decline have been assessed in order to 22 examine how people at different ages approach memory demanding situations (Agrigoroaei & 23 Lachman, 2010; Hertzog, McGuire, Horhota, & Jopp, 2010; Horhota, Lineweaver, Ositelu, 24 Summers, & Hertzog, 2011). One widely studied motivational construct is Memory Self- 25 Efficacy (MSE), defined as “beliefs about one's own capability to use memory effectively in MSE and beliefs about forgetting and aging - 5 1 various situations” (Hertzog, Hultsch, & Dixon, 1989, p. 687). Poor MSE may lead people to 2 disengage from cognitive tasks because they expect to perform poorly. Consequently, because 3 MSE is known to decline with age (Beaudoin, Agrigoroaei, Desrichard, Fournet, & Roulin, 4 2008; Berry, West, & Dennehey, 1989; Hultsch, Hertzog, & Dixon, 1987; Lineweaver & 5 Hertzog, 1998; McDonald-Miszczak, Hertzog, & Hultsch, 1995) and to be linked to memory 6 performance (Beaudoin & Desrichard, 2011), it is plausible that MSE contributes to memory 7 decline with age (Beaudoin & Desrichard, submitted; Berry & West, 1993; Dixon, Rust, 8 Feltmate, & Kwong See, 2007; Valentijn et al., 2006; West, Bagwell, & Dark-Freudeman, 9 2008). 10 From this perspective, it is crucial to know which factors influence MSE. Bandura 11 (1977) assumed that performance accomplishments and emotional arousal are sources of 12 efficacy expectations. Indeed, a meta-analysis of the MSE-actual memory performance 13 relationship has shown a low but significant weighted mean correlation between MSE and 14 memory performance, r = .15 (Beaudoin & Desrichard, 2011), suggesting that previous 15 experiences of memory failure may influence MSE, in turn affecting a person's motivation to 16 expend effort in performing memory tasks. However, the low effect size also suggests that 17 other factors are likely to impact MSE. In fact, MSE has been shown to be related to affective 18 factors such as depression (e.g., Johansson, Allen-Burge, & Zarit, 1997; Zelinsky & Gilewski, 19 2004) and anxiety (e.g., West, Bagwell, & King, 2006), suggesting that poor mental health 20 leads to low MSE. 21 Beliefs about forgetting and aging may also affect MSE (e.g., Lane & Zelinski, 2003; 22 McDonald-Miszczak et al., 1995). Cavanaugh, Feldman and Hertzog (1998) suggested that 23 self-knowledge about memory is not a fact; rather, it is a belief imbedded in more general 24 response patterns such as implicit theories about aging or personal constructs. Social and 25 culturally shared beliefs about forgetting and aging (Levy & Langer, 1994) have been MSE and beliefs about forgetting and aging - 6 1 identified, with participants reporting decreases in memory with age for other adults and for 2 themselves (Lineweaver, Berger, & Hertzog, 2009; Lineweaver & Hertzog, 1998; Ryan, 3 1992; Ryan & Kwong See, 1993). This belief leads people to attribute memory failures to 4 different causes according to the age of the person experiencing the memory failure. Hence, 5 memory failures are more likely to be attributed to mental difficulties and lack of ability in the 6 case of older adults, and to a lack of effort or lack of attention in the case of younger adults 7 (e.g., Erber, Prager, Williams, & Caiola, 1996; Erber, Szuchman, & Rothberg, 1990; Parr & 8 Siegert, 1993). In addition, older adults have a greater tendency than younger adults to cite 9 age as a cause of their memory failures (Devolder & Pressley, 1992; Jennings & Darwin, 10 11 2003; Vestergren & Nilsson, 2011). Indirect evidence suggests that beliefs about forgetting and aging influence the way 12 people evaluate their own memory efficiency. Jennings and Darwin (2003) reported a positive 13 link between beliefs in the impact of age on memory performance and reports of memory 14 failures. In addition, ageist response bias measured via the Knowledge of Memory Aging 15 Questionnaire is associated with higher self-reported frequency of forgetting (Cherry, 16 Brigman, Reese-Melancon, Burton-Chase, & Holland, 2013). Direct evidence provided by 17 Fort, Aboul, Holl, Kaddour, and Gana (2004) shows that a greater conception of memory 18 change with aging (i.e., stereotypes about aging) is associated with lower contentment with 19 one’s memory abilities and with increased reporting of memory problems. Thus, both 20 theoretical considerations and empirical results suggest that older adults’ beliefs about older 21 people experiencing more memory failures could influence their own MSE. However, the 22 literature provides only limited support for this hypothesis. In addition, we are unaware of any 23 studies that have specifically tested the relationship between MSE and beliefs about forgetting 24 and aging, while controlling for memory performance and mental health, although these 25 factors could influence both MSE and adherence to beliefs about forgetting and aging. The MSE and beliefs about forgetting and aging - 7 1 objective of the present research was to evaluate this hypothesis more directly by empirically 2 examining the relationship between beliefs about forgetting and aging and MSE, while 3 controlling for several factors. 4 5 6 Method Participants Participants were 298 independent-living adults (145 men and 153 women), aged 7 between 57 and 87 years (M = 69.416, SD = 7.750), who had been recruited for a three-stage 8 longitudinal study of memory and aging. This study was designed to achieve several 9 objectives, including the establishment of norms for working memory performance (Fournet 10 et al., 2012) and the assessment, using validated scales, of the determinants and consequences 11 of MSE (e.g., Beaudoin et al., 2008). In the first stage of the study, we used a random 12 stratified method to select participants from a pool list of people. The analyses described in 13 the present paper were conducted on data obtained during the second stage of the study 14 because this was the only stage in which all the measures presented (e.g., MIA-Capacity) 15 were assessed. We eliminated data for participants who scored lower than 26 on the Mini 16 Mental State Examination (MMSE, Folstein, Folstein, & McHugh, 1975; Kalafat, Hugonot- 17 Diener, & Poitrenaud, 2003), consequently, the participants’ mean score on the MMSE was 18 high (M = 28.523, SD = 1.198). We assessed participants’ autonomy via the Instrumental 19 Activities of Daily Living scale (IADL, Israël, 1996a; Lawton & Brody, 1969), evaluated on a 20 4-point scale from 1 (autonomous) to 4 (dependent), which showed that the participants were 21 autonomous (median value = 1, M = 1.082, SD = 0.187). 22 Measures 23 24 We administered the measures in random order, although the experiment was designed so participants never had to complete two memory-performance tasks one directly after the MSE and beliefs about forgetting and aging - 8 1 other. All the measures were paper and pencil, except for the squares and words used in the 2 short-term memory tasks, which were presented on a computer screen. 3 Beliefs about forgetting and aging 4 We measured beliefs about forgetting and aging (BAFA) via six vignettes 5 (Agrigoroaei, 2007) describing daily forgetting in which either a woman (i.e., Marie) or a man 6 (i.e., Paul) forgets something (e.g., “Five minutes ago, Paul put his keys somewhere but 7 doesn’t remember where” and “Yesterday Paul was introduced to someone, but he has 8 forgotten his name”). Each vignette was associated with two questions assessing two 9 complementary aspects of the beliefs (i.e., beliefs about forgetting and aging for others and 10 for self): “Do you think this type of situation occurs more often to young people or more often 11 to older people?” (adapted from research about gender stereotypes, White, Kruczek, Brown, 12 & White, 1989; White & White, 2006) and “If this situation occurred to you, would you say it 13 was because of your age?” (adapted from Prohaska, Keller, Leventhal, & Leventhal, 1987). 14 This measure allowed us to measure adherence to the belief that normal daily forgetting 15 occurs more frequently in older adults than in younger adults and associations between one’s 16 own age and forgetting. We used the responses to determine differences between participants’ 17 adherence to BAFA. Responses to these two questions were given on a 6-point scale, from 1 18 (much more often to young people) to 6 (much more often to old people) for the first question, 19 and from 1 (certainly not because of my age) to 6 (very likely because of my age) for the 20 second question. We calculated BAFA scores by averaging the 12 responses obtained for the 21 six vignettes (α = .826). Higher scores indicate a greater tendency to associate forgetting with 22 aging. Final scores could range from 1 to 6. 23 Memory self-efficacy 24 We used two MSE measures that are commonly used in studies of aging: the Memory 25 Self Efficacy Questionnaire (MSEQ, Berry et al., 1989) and the Capacity subscale of the MSE and beliefs about forgetting and aging - 9 1 Metamemory In Adulthood questionnaire (MIA-Capacity, Dixon, Hultsch, & Hertzog, 1988). 2 Although both these questionnaires measure MSE, they differ in the way they assess it, as the 3 MSEQ is based on performance-prediction and the MIA-Capacity subscale is based on 4 memory-rating measures (Beaudoin & Desrichard, 2011). 5 The MSEQ includes six memory exercises of the type “If someone showed you 6 pictures of 16 familiar objects (e.g., lamp, umbrella, etc.), would you be able to look at them 7 once and recall the names of all 16 objects, 12 of the 16 objects, 8 of the 16 objects, 4 of the 8 16 objects, 2 of the 16 objects?” In the French version of the MSEQ (Beaudoin et al., 2008), 9 participants predict their performance on each task, even though they are not required to 10 perform the task, by giving yes/no responses for each possible answer. The maximum number 11 of "yes" responses for each exercise is five. This is the highest level of memory performance 12 prediction. Final scores were calculated by averaging the levels of memory predictions across 13 all six exercises (α = .793), with a high score indicating a high level of MSE. 14 The MIA-Capacity scale (Boucheron, 1995) asks participants to respond to 17 items 15 (e.g., “I am good at remembering names”) on a 5-point scale from 1 (strongly agree) to 5 16 (strongly disagree). Scores were calculated by reversing 15 of the items (two items were 17 worded negatively) and then averaging the responses to the 17 items (α = .779). A high score 18 indicates a high level of MSE. 19 Retrospective functioning 20 Because the MSEQ and MIA-Capacity scales measure MSE at a single point in time, 21 we also used a modified version of the Retrospective Functioning subscale of the Memory 22 Functioning Questionnaire (MFQ-RF, Gilewski & Zelinski, 1988; French version: Israël, 23 1996b) in order to obtain a measure of perceived temporal change in memory. The MFQ-RF 24 asks participants to compare their current memories with their memories at different times in 25 the past (i.e., one month ago, one year ago, 5 years ago, 10 years ago, 20 years ago and when MSE and beliefs about forgetting and aging - 10 1 participants were 18 years old) for six items. Responses are given on a 5-point scale from 1 2 (current memory is much worse than past memory) to 5 (current memory is much better than 3 past memory). Scores are calculated by averaging the responses to the six items (α = .844), 4 with a high score indicating a low perceived decline in memory. 5 Memory performance 6 We chose to measure episodic memory and short-term memory performances, as these 7 memory domains are known to be sensitive to age-related change (Maylor, 2005), and 8 especially in the case of episodic memory to be linked to MSE (Beaudoin & Desrichard, 9 2011). We used both spatial and verbal materials to test each of these types of memory. One 10 reason for using different materials to assess different types of memory is that participants 11 may evaluate different types of memory differently; hence, the relation between memory and 12 MSE may differ depending on the memory task used. The moderate correlations among the 13 different types of memory performance (maximum correlation = .422) underline the utility of 14 measuring several different aspects of memory. 15 Shape recall. We used a visual episodic memory measure from the Batterie 16 d’Efficience Mnésique l44 (BEM 144, a French battery of memory tests, Signoret, 1991). 17 Participants were asked to look at a complex shape for one minute and then to reproduce it in 18 as much detail as possible. Three independent judges rated the results, giving scores on a 19 scale from 0 to 12 (intraclass coefficient = .989, p < .001). A high score indicated a high level 20 of performance. 21 Story recall. The verbal episodic memory measure we used was also taken from the 22 BEM 144. After listening to the experimenter read a 12-sentence story, participants had to 23 repeat the story in as much detail as possible. The experimenter attributed points for each pre- 24 defined element that was recalled. Instructions, story reading, and point attributions were 25 standardized. Experimenters were trained to administrate this task in order to ensure measures MSE and beliefs about forgetting and aging - 11 1 of performance were reliable. Final scores could range from 0 to 12 and a high score indicated 2 a high level of performance. 3 Word-list recall. We used a second measure of verbal episodic memory taken from the 4 MEM-III (Weshsler, 2001). Participants were asked to memorize a set of 24 familiar words, 5 with no restrictions on the time they had to study the words. They then had to recall as many 6 of the words as possible. Scores were the number of words recalled correctly (from 0 to 24) 7 and a high score indicated a high level of performance. 8 9 Verbal span. We assessed short-term verbal memory via a measure taken from Fournet et al. (2012). Participants were presented with words in a sequence and asked to 10 recall these words in the order in which they appeared. We used two equivalent lists of single- 11 syllable, concrete words that were phonologically different and controlled for frequency. The 12 task was divided into levels of increasing difficulty, with two trials at each level. Scores were 13 short-term spans and a high score indicated a high level of performance. 14 Location span. The short-term visuo-spatial memory measure we used was also taken 15 from Fournet et al. (2012). After being presented with a pattern of squares that were filled in 16 one by one, participants had to recall the locations of the filled-in squares in the order in 17 which they appeared. The task was divided into levels of increasing difficulty, with two trials 18 at each level. Scores were short-term spans and a high score indicated a high level of 19 performance. 20 Mental health 21 Anxiety. We used the trait version of the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI, 22 Bruchon-Schweitzer & Paulhan, 1993; Spielberger, Gorsuch, Lushene, Vagg, & Jacobs, 23 1983). Participants were asked to respond to 20 items using a 4-point scale from 1 (almost 24 never) to 4 (almost always). Scores were calculated by averaging the responses for the 20 MSE and beliefs about forgetting and aging - 12 1 items after reversing items where this was appropriate (α = .880). A high score indicated a 2 high level of anxiety. 3 Depression. We used the depression subscale of the Symptom Check-List Revised 4 (SCL-90 R, Derogatis, 1977; Pariente & Guelfi, 1990). Participants were asked to respond to 5 11 items using a 5-point scale from 1 (no, not at all) to 5 (yes, extremely). Scores were 6 calculated by averaging responses to the 11 items (α = .848). A high score indicated a high 7 level of depression. 8 9 10 11 12 Subjective health. Subjective health was measured via a single question “Do you think your health is currently…” Participants were asked to respond using a 5-point scale from 1 (very good) to 5 (very bad). A high score indicated a low level of subjective health. Results Bivariate correlations, means, and standard deviations of BAFA and MSE scores are 13 presented in Table 11. Correlations revealed a significant negative relationship between 14 beliefs about forgetting and aging and MSE and perceived temporal changes in memory 15 scores: the stronger the belief that forgetting and age are associated, the lower a participant’s 16 self-evaluated memory efficacy (MSEQ and MIA-Capacity) and the higher a participant’s 17 self-evaluated memory decline over time (MFQ-RF). MSE and beliefs about forgetting and aging - 13 1 Table 1 2 Correlations between BAFA and MSE scores Measure N Means SD 1 2 3 4 1- MSEQ 298 3.02 0.55 - 2- MIA-Capacity 298 3.31 0.50 .302** - 3- MFQ-RF 297 2.35 0.49 .104 .273** - 4- Beliefs about forgetting and aging 5- Age 298 3.81 0.72 -.255** -.331** -.308** - 298 69.42 7.75 -.198** -.179** -.241** .345** 6- Level of education 294 4.95 1.74 .098 -.048 .037 -.095 7- Sexa 298 - - -.003 -.078 .084 .032 8- Shape recall 296 8.93 1.90 .193** .062 .151** -.201** 9- Story recall 297 8.35 1.93 .206** .056 .047 -.199** 10- Word list recall 296 10.83 4.96 .247** .146* .168** -.191** 11- Verbal span 298 4.85 1.07 .060 .161** .198** -.184** 12- Location span 297 5.02 1.00 .140* .093 .095 -.137* 13- Anxiety 294 1.89 0.40 -.158** -.206** -.178** .176** 14- Depression 298 1.51 0.45 -.143* -.240** -.254** .187** 15- Subjective health 298 2.25 0.67 -.093 -.141* -.147* .114* MSEQ = Memory Self-Efficacy Questionnaire, MIA-Capacity = Capacity subscale of the Memory In Adulthood questionnaire, MFQ-RF = Memory Functioning Questionnaire - Retrospective Functioning a Sex: -1= Female, 1= Male **p<.01; *p<.05 3 4 5 BAFA scores also correlated with most of the other measures, with higher levels of 6 BAFA being associated with lower memory performance and subjective health and higher 7 levels of depression and anxiety. 8 9 We tested an integrative model by using AMOS 23 Software to conduct a Structural Equation Modeling2 analysis in which all the predictors (i.e., sociodemographic, memory 10 performance, and mental health variables) determine BAFA and all the predictors plus BAFA 11 determine the MSE latent variable (see Figure 1). All the predictors were measured variables, MSE and beliefs about forgetting and aging - 14 1 using the means of the scales as indicators. We also added all the covariances between 2 predictors, and the residuals for BAFA, MSE, and the three measured variables associated 3 with MSE. This model corresponds to a mediation analysis with BAFA as a mediator. We 4 estimated direct, indirect, and total effects by Bootstrapping (2000 samples, 90% bias- 5 corrected confidence interval, with the Monte Carlo method). 6 With the exception of the χ² value, which was significant χ²(24)= 39.889, p = .022, all 7 the fit indices for the model were excellent (Hu & Bentler, 1999; Kline, 2011): χ²/df = 1.662, 8 RMSEA = .048 [.019; .074], SRMR = .0306, CFI = .979. The standardized factor loadings of 9 MSE on mean MSEQ, MIA-Capacity and MFQ scores were .405, .508 and .575, respectively. 10 The results for the estimates of the regression coefficients (Table 2) show an effect of BAFA 11 on the MSE latent variable. This effect was significant and in the expected direction, with 12 higher BAFA scores being associated with lower MSE score, regardless of the influence of 13 the sociodemographic group, memory performance, and mental health variables. Even when 14 controlling for the other variables, the variance in MSE was explained by word list recall and 15 the variance in BAFA was explained by variables such as age, story recall performance, and 16 anxiety. Moreover, BAFA was found to be a significant mediator of the link between age and 17 MSE. 18 MSE and beliefs about forgetting and aging - 15 1 2 Figure 1. The structural model tested. 3 Note. BAFA= Beliefs About Forgetting and Aging, MSE = Memory Self-Efficacy. MSE and beliefs about forgetting and aging - 16 1 Table 2. Direct, total, and indirect effects of the predictors on BAFA and MSE. Direct/total effects on Indirect effects on Total effects on MSE Direct effects on MSE BAFA MSE Standardized Standardized Standardized Standardized P-value P-value P-value P-value coefficient coefficient coefficient coefficient BAFA - - -0.473 0.001 - - - - Age 0.293 0.001 -0.255 0.003 -0.117 0.175 -0.139 <0.001 Level of education 0.016 0.825 -0.149 0.083 -0.141 0.071 -0.008 0.805 Sexa -0.002 0.998 0.103 0.324 0.102 0.272 0.001 1 Shape recall -0.006 0.963 0.046 0.643 0.043 0.617 0.003 0.961 Story recall -0.138 0.028 0.094 0.270 0.029 0.719 0.065 0.021 Word list recall -0.061 0.411 0.280 0.004 0.251 0.005 0.029 0.364 Verbal span -0.088 0.143 0.171 0.041 0.129 0.105 0.042 0.120 Location span 0.101 0.137 -0.164 0.072 -0.116 0.173 -0.048 0.108 Anxiety 0.145 0.053 -0.232 0.038 -0.163 0.118 -0.069 0.042 Depression 0.008 0.915 -0.157 0.137 -0.153 0.127 -0.004 0.909 Subjective health -0.021 0.712 0.012 0.899 0.002 0.962 0.010 0.697 2 Note. BAFA= Beliefs About Forgetting and Aging, MSE = Memory Self-Efficacy. 3 a Sex: -1= Female, 1= Male MSE and beliefs about forgetting and aging - 17 1 Discussion 2 The present study examined whether beliefs about forgetting and aging account for a 3 significant part of the variance in memory self-evaluations made by older adults. Our results 4 indicated moderately significant relationships between BAFA and MSE and participants’ 5 perceptions of their memory decline. In general, the more people adhere to the belief that 6 daily forgetting is associated with age, the poorer they consider their actual memory capacity, 7 and the more they consider their memory to have declined over the years. 8 9 The results showed a link between MSE and older adults’ beliefs about the association between age and poor memory. The more they adhere to these stereotypical beliefs, the more 10 they consider their memory capacity to be poor. This result supports the idea conveyed by 11 previous studies (e.g., Cherry et al., 2013; Fort et al., 2004; Jennings & Darwin, 2003), which 12 have reported a link between stereotypes and memory evaluations. Thus, the present study 13 confirms the notion that MSE is by no means a simple reflection of individual performances 14 and affective states, and is influenced by Western societies’ ideas about aging. This point 15 sheds light on the relation between memory performance, motivation to complete memory 16 tasks, and social beliefs about aging, as cognitive performances depend on motivational 17 components (i.e., MSE, Beaudoin & Desrichard, 2011) that are sensitive to stereotypes. This 18 result should encourage further research into the social determinants of memory performance 19 and memory decline. Because there are cultural differences in beliefs about aging and 20 cognition (e.g., Levy & Langer, 1994; Löckenhoff et al., 2009), beliefs about cognition, 21 including those conveyed by an individual’s culture, have to be taken into account in order to 22 understand individuals’ cognitive performances. 23 However, the precise nature of the MSE-beliefs relationship may be difficult to 24 disentangle because it may be affected by several variables. For example, people may have 25 noticed that their memory is really poor, and such perceptions could provide a third factor that MSE and beliefs about forgetting and aging - 18 1 correlates with both MSE and BAFA, thereby creating a spurious correlation between MSE 2 and BAFA. Similarly, the literature suggests mental health problems, such as anxiety or 3 depression, and poor subjective health may be related to lower memory self-assessments and 4 negative views of aging (Lane & Zelinski, 2003; McDonald-Miszczak et al., 1995). 5 Nevertheless, our study helps clarify the relationships between MSE and beliefs by showing 6 there is a link between BAFA and MSE, and that this link is independent of other variables. In 7 fact, we were able to show that the significant relationship between MSE and BAFA persists 8 after statistically controlling for cognitive and mental health potential confounds such as 9 actual memory performance, anxiety, depression, and subjective health. Hence, BAFA 10 appears to predict self-assessment of memory capacities, even when memory performance 11 and mental health are taken into account. Consequently, our results support the hypothesis 12 that variability in MSE is related to adherence to BAFA. Furthermore, our results also show 13 that BAFA is a mediator of the relation between age and MSE, thereby suggesting that these 14 beliefs may contribute to MSE decline. Implicit theories about memory and aging are widely 15 held to be determinants of MSE decline (Cavanaugh et al., 1998; Lane & Zelinski, 2003; 16 McDonald-Miszczak et al., 1995), and the present study provides empirical evidence 17 supporting this supposition. Nevertheless, more information is needed about the processes by 18 which beliefs influence MSE decline, for example, how stereotypes about memory and aging 19 affect older individuals’ MSE, and the ways in which general BAFA affect the attribution of 20 everyday forgetting to age. In fact, it is likely that such attributions reinforce beliefs 21 associating forgetting with age. Further research is needed to explore the dynamic interactions 22 between beliefs and self-evaluation. 23 Our study design does not allow us to draw conclusions about the direction of 24 causality of the relation between MSE (and perceived temporal changes in memory) and 25 BAFA. Although this limitation does not reduce the utility of the concept of BAFA, further MSE and beliefs about forgetting and aging - 19 1 exploration of the processes (e.g., age-group identification) driving these effects is needed in 2 order to clarify the relationship between beliefs and MSE and to determine how people define 3 others or themselves as “old”. Therefore, it would be interesting to study beliefs about several 4 age categories in order to explore their specific beliefs about the perceived effect of age on 5 forgetting and the impact of these beliefs on MSE. More interestingly, people’s MSE should 6 differ according to whether they categorize themselves as young or old. In fact, it has been 7 found that subjective age influences MSE (Stephan et al., 2011), so it would be interesting to 8 study the role of age-group identification in older adults. Given the usefulness of the BAFA 9 measure and the interesting results obtained using it, further research needs to be carried out 10 11 to determine its properties and its correlates. A second limitation of our study results from the small effect sizes. Beta values did not 12 exceed -.473, despite the reliability of the BAFA and the MSE measures. However, it is 13 probable that our methodology led us to underestimate effect sizes, as we used general scales 14 to assess both BAFA and MSE. Individuals may perceive some specific memory domains 15 (e.g., memory of recent events, word finding problems) to be more age-sensitive than others 16 (e.g., Lineweaver & Hertzog, 1998); therefore, the relation between MSE and beliefs would 17 probably be stronger if the memory domains in which they are measured are matched. 18 Our results highlight the importance of adherence to BAFA, such as implicit theories 19 about memory and aging or stereotypes, and show that variations in MSE may be due to older 20 adults’ adherence to these beliefs (Devine, 1989). This suggests that research focused on the 21 relationship between variables such as memory performance or mental health and individual 22 differences in MSE should also assess adherence to BAFA, especially when attempting to 23 understand MSE and memory performance with respect to aging. Moreover, MSE and, more 24 broadly, memory self-evaluations and memory complaints are particularly important in 25 neuropsychological consultations, which generally involve some form of memory assessment. MSE and beliefs about forgetting and aging - 20 1 Taking into account patients' beliefs would allow self-report memory assessments to be seen 2 as reflections of real memory performance or mental health, but also as being anchored in 3 BAFA. Because memory complaints and general self-assessments of memory are used to 4 investigate mild cognitive impairment, it is important to know that these indicators reflect a 5 number of factors, including real cognitive decline, affects, and BAFA. 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 MSE and beliefs about forgetting and aging - 21 1 References 2 Agrigoroaei, S., & Lachman, M. E. (2010). Personal control and aging: How beliefs and 3 expectations matter. Dans J. C. Cavanaugh, C. K. Cavanaugh, J. Berry, & R. West 4 (Éd.), Aging in America, Vol 1: Psychological aspects, Praeger perspectives (p. 177- 5 201). Santa Barbara, CA US: Praeger/ABC-CLIO. 6 Agrigoroaei, S. (2007). Memory Self-efficacy Decrease in Elderly People: Theoretical Review 7 and Study of its Relations With Cognitive, Affective, and Socio-cognitive Factors. 8 Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Université de Savoie, France. 9 10 11 Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of behavioral change. Psychological Review, 84, 191-215. Barber, S. J., & Mather, M. (2013). Stereotype threat can both enhance and impair older 12 adults’ memory. 13 10.1177/0956797613497023 Psychological Science, 24, 2522-2529. doi: 14 Beaudoin, M., Agrigoroaei, S., Desrichard, O., Fournet, N., & Roulin, J-L. (2008). Validation 15 of the French version of the Memory Self-Efficacy Questionnaire. European Review 16 of Applied Psychology/Revue Européenne de Psychologie Appliquée, 58(3), 165-176. 17 doi:10.1016/j.erap.2007.09.001 18 19 20 Beaudoin, M., & Desrichard, O. (submitted). Memory self-efficacy and memory performance in older adults: The mediating role of task persistence. Beaudoin, M., & Desrichard, O. (2011). Are memory self-efficacy and memory performance 21 related? A meta-analysis. 22 10.1037/a0022106 Psychological Bulletin 137(2), 211-241. doi: 23 Berry, J. M., & West, R. L. (1993). Cognitive self-efficacy in relation to personal mastery and 24 goal setting across the life span. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 25 16(2), 351-379. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.25.5.701 MSE and beliefs about forgetting and aging - 22 1 Berry, J. M., West, R. L., & Dennehey, D. M. (1989). Reliability and validity of the Memory 2 Self-Efficacy Questionnaire. Developmental Psychology, 25(5), 701-713. doi: 3 10.1037/0012-1649.25.5.701 4 Boucheron, C. (1995). Version française du M.I.A. (Metamemory in Adulthood). European 5 Review of Applied Psychology/Revue Européenne de Psychologie Appliquée, 45(3), 6 163-170. 7 Bruchon-Schweitzer, M., & Paulhan, I. (1993). Adaptation française de l’Inventaire 8 d’Anxiété Trait-Etat. Forme Y (STAI Y). Paris: Les Éditions du Centre de Psychologie 9 Appliquée. 10 Cavanaugh, J. C., Feldman, J. M., & Hertzog, C. (1998). Memory beliefs as social cognition: 11 A reconceptualization of what memory questionnaires assess. Review of General 12 Psychology, 2(1), 48-65. doi: 10.1037/1089-2680.2.1.48 13 Cherry, K. E., Brigman, S., Reese-Melancon, C., Burton-Chase, A., & Holland, K. (2013). 14 Memory aging knowledge and memory self-appraisal in younger and older adults. 15 Educational Gerontology, 39, 168-178. doi: 10.1080/03601277.2012.699838 16 Dark-Freudeman, A., West, R. L., & Viverito, K. M. (2006). Future Selves and Aging: Older 17 Adults' Memory Fears. 18 10.1080/03601270500388125 Educational Gerontology, 32(2), 85-109. doi: 19 Derogatis, L.R. (1977). SCL-90 : Administration, Scoring and Procedures Manual I for the 20 R(evised) Version. John Hopkins University School of Medicine, Clinical 21 Psychometrics Research Unit, Baltimore. 22 Desrichard, O., & Köpetz, C. (2005). A threat in the elder: the impact of task- 23 instructions, self-efficacy and performance expectations on memory performance 24 in 25 10.1002/ejsp.249 the elderly. European Journal of Social Psychology, 35(4), 537-552. doi: MSE and beliefs about forgetting and aging - 23 1 Devine, P. G. (1989). Stereotypes and prejudice: Their automatic and controlled components. 2 Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56(1), 5-18. doi: 10.1037/0022- 3 3514.56.1.5 4 Devolder, P. A., & Pressley, M. (1992). Causal attributions and strategy use in relation to 5 memory performance differences in younger and older adults. Applied Cognitive 6 Psychology, 6(7), 629-642. doi: 10.1002/acp.2350060706 7 8 Dixon, R. A., Hultsch, D. F., & Hertzog, C. (1988). The Metamemory in Adulthood (MIA) questionnaire. Psychopharmacology Bulletin, 24, 671–688. 9 Dixon, R. A., Rust, T. B., Feltmate, S. E., & Kwong See S. (2007). Memory and aging: 10 Selected research directions and application issues. Canadian Psychology/Psychologie 11 Canadienne, 48(2), 67-76. 10.1037/cp2007008 12 Erber, J. T., Prager, I. G., Williams, M., & Caiola, M. A. (1996). Age and forgetfulness: 13 Confidence in ability and attribution for memory failures. Psychology and Aging, 14 11(2), 310-315. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.11.2.310 15 Erber, J. T., Szuchman, L. T., & Rothberg, S. T. (1990). Everyday memory failure: Age 16 differences in appraisal and attribution. Psychology and Aging, 5(2), 236-241. doi: 17 10.1037/0882-7974.5.2.236 18 Esposito, F., Rochat, L., Juillerat Van der Linden, A.-C., Lekeu, F., Charnallet, A., & Van der 19 Linden, M. (2014). Apathy in aging: Are lack of interest and lack of initiative 20 dissociable? 21 doi:10.1016/j.archger.2013.09.002 Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics, 58, 43-50. 22 Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., & McHugh, P. R. (1975). Mini-mental state: A practical 23 method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of 24 Psychiatric Research, 12(3), 189-198. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6 MSE and beliefs about forgetting and aging - 24 1 Fort, I., Adoul, L., Holl, D., Kaddour, J. l., & Gana, K. (2004). Psychometric Properties of the 2 French Version of the Multifactorial Memory Questionnaire for Adults and the 3 Elderly. Canadian Journal on Aging, 23(4), 347-357. doi: 10.1353/cja.2005.0020 4 Fournet, N., Roulin, J.-L., Vallet, F., Beaudoin M., Agrigoroaei S., Paignon, A. Dantzer, C. & 5 Desrichard O. (2012). Evaluating short-term and working memory in older adults: 6 French 7 10.1080/13607863.2012.674487 8 9 normative data. Aging and Mental Health, 16(7), 922-30. doi: Gilewski, M. J., & Zelinski, E. M. (1988). Memory Functioning Questionnaire (MFQ). Psychopharmacology Bulletin, 24(4), 665-670. 10 Hertzog, C., Hultsch, D. F., & Dixon, R. A. (1989). Evidence for the convergent validity of 11 two self-report metamemory questionnaires. Developmental Psychology, 25(5), 687- 12 700. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.25.5.687 13 Hertzog, C., McGuire, C. L., Horhota, M., & Jopp, D. (2010). Does believing in « use it or 14 lose it » relate to self-rated memory control, strategy use, and recall? The International 15 Journal of Aging & Human Development, 70(1), 61‑87. 16 17 Hess, T. M. (2005). Memory and aging in context. Psychological Bulletin, 131, 383-406. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.131.3.383 18 Hess, T. M., Emery, L., & Neupert, S. D. (2012). Longitudinal relationships between 19 resources, motivation, and functioning. The Journals of Gerontology. Series B, 20 Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 67, 299-308. doi:10.1093/geronb/gbr100 21 Horhota, M., Lineweaver, T., Ositelu, M., Summers, K., & Hertzog, C. (2012). Young and 22 older adults’ beliefs about effective ways to mitigate age-related memory decline. 23 Psychology and Aging, 27(2), 293‑304. MSE and beliefs about forgetting and aging - 25 1 Hu, L.-T., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure 2 analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling, 3 6(1), 1-55. doi: 10.1080/10705519909540118 4 Hultsch, D. F., Hertzog, C., & Dixon, R. A. (1987). Age differences in metamemory: 5 Resolving the inconsistencies. Canadian Journal of Psychology/Revue Canadienne de 6 Psychologie, 41(2), 193-208. doi: 10.1037/h0084153 7 Israël, L. (1996a). Les activités instrumentales de la vie quotidienne, IADL. In J. D. Guelfi 8 (Ed.), L'évaluation clinique standardisée en psychiatrie (pp. 483-484): Editions 9 médicales Pierre Fabre, tome II. 10 Israël, L. (1996b). Questionnaire du fonctionnement de la mémoire. Auto-évaluation des 11 plaintes mnésique (Memory Functionning Questionnaire). In J. D. Guelfi (Ed.), 12 L'évaluation clinique standardisée en psychiatrie (pp. 493-501): Editions médicales 13 Pierre Fabre, tome II. 14 Jennings, J. M., & Darwin, A. L. (2003). Efficacy beliefs, everyday behavior, and memory 15 performance among older elderly adults. Educational Gerontology, 29(1), 71-91. doi : 16 10.1080/713844231 17 Johansson, B., Allen-Burge, R., & Zarit, S. H. (1997). Self-reports on memory functioning in 18 a longitudinal study of the oldest old: Relation to current, prospective, and 19 retrospective performance. The Journals of Gerontology: Series B: Psychological 20 Sciences and Social Sciences, 52b(3), 139-146. 21 Kalafat, M., Hugonot-Diener, L., & Poitrenaud, J. (2003). Standardisation et Etalonnage 22 français du 'Mini Mental State' (MMS) version GRECO. Revue de Neuropsychologie, 23 13(2), 209-236. 24 Kit, K. A., Tuokko, H. A., & Mateer, C. A. (2008). A review of the stereotype threat literature 25 and its application in a neurological population. Neuropsychology Review, 18(2), 132- MSE and beliefs about forgetting and aging - 26 1 2 3 148. doi: 10.1007/s11065-008-9059-9 Kline, R. B. (2011). Principles and Practice of Structural Equation Modeling (3rd ed.). New York, NY: Guilford Press. 4 Lane, C. J., & Zelinski, E. M. (2003). Longitudinal hierarchical linear models of the Memory 5 Functioning Questionnaire. Psychology and Aging, 18(1), 38-53. doi: 10.1037/0882- 6 7974.18.1.38 7 Lamont, R. A., Swift , H. J., & Abrams, D. (2015). A Review and Meta-Analysis of Age- 8 Based Stereotype Threat: Negative Stereotypes, Not Facts, Do the Damage. 9 Psychology and aging. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0038586 10 11 Lawton, M. P., & Brody, E. M. (1969). Assessment of older people: Self-maintaining and instrumental activities of daily living. The Gerontologist, 9(3), 179-186. 12 Levy, B., & Langer, E. (1994). Aging free from negative stereotypes: Successful memory in 13 China among the American deaf. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66(6), 14 989-997. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.66.6.989 15 Lineweaver, T. T., Berger, A. K. & Hertzog, C. (2009). Expectations about memory change 16 across the life span are impacted by aging stereotypes. Psychology and Aging, 24(1), 17 169-176. doi: 10.1037/a0013577 18 Lineweaver, T. T., & Hertzog, C. (1998). Adults' efficacy and control beliefs regarding 19 memory 20 Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 5(4), 264-296. doi: 10.1076/anec.5.4.264.77 21 and aging: Separating general from personal beliefs. Aging, Löckenhoff, C. E., De Fruyt, F., Terracciano, A., McCrae, R. R., De Bolle, M., Costa, P. T., 22 Jr., ... Yik, M. (2009). Perceptions of aging across 26 cultures and their culture-level 23 associates. Psychology and Aging, 24(4), 941-954. doi: 10.1037/a0016901 MSE and beliefs about forgetting and aging - 27 1 Maylor, E. A. (2005). Age-related changes in memory. In M. L. Johnson, V. L. Bengtson, P. 2 G. Coleman, & T. B. L. Kirkwood (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of age and 3 ageing (pp. 200-208). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. 4 McDonald-Miszczak, L., Hertzog, C., & Hultsch, D. F. (1995). Stability and accuracy of 5 metamemory in adulthood and aging: A longitudinal analysis. Psychology and Aging, 6 10(4), 553-564. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.10.4.553 7 Pariente, P. & Guelfi, J. D. (1990). Version française de la SCL-90-R : inventaire d'auto- 8 évaluation 9 inventaires multidimensionnels. Psychiatry and Psychobiology, 5, 49-63. 10 psychopathologique chez les adultes. Première partie : Parr, W. V., & Siegert, R. (1993). Adults’ conceptions of everyday memory failures in others: 11 Factors that mediate the effects of target age. Psychology and Aging, 8(4), 599-605. 12 doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.8.4.599 13 Penningroth, S. L., & Scott, W. D. (2007). A motivational-cognitive model of prospective 14 memory: The influence of goal relevance. In L. V. Brown (Ed.), Psychology of 15 Motivation. New-York: Nova Science Publishers. 16 Prohaska, T. R., Keller, M. L., Leventhal, E. A., & Leventhal, H. (1987). Impact of symptoms 17 and aging attribution on emotions and coping. Health Psychology, 6(6), 495-514. doi : 18 10.1037/0278-6133.6.6.495 19 20 21 22 23 24 Ryan, E. B. (1992). Beliefs about memory changes across the adult life span. Journals of Gerontology, 47(1), 41-46. Ryan, E. B., & Kwong See, S.T. (1993). Age-based beliefs about memory changes for self and others across adulthood. Journals of Gerontology, 48(4), 199-201. Signoret, J-L. (1991). Batterie d’efficience Mnésique BEM 144. Paris : Editions Scientifiques Elsevier MSE and beliefs about forgetting and aging - 28 1 2 Spielberger, C. D., Gorsuch, R. L., Lushene, R., Vagg, P. R., & Jacobs, G. A. (1983). Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press. 3 Stephan, Y., Caudroit, J., & Chalabaev, A. (2011). Subjective health and memory self- 4 efficacy as mediators in the relation between subjective age and life satisfaction 5 among older adults. Aging & Mental Health, 15(4), 428-436. 6 Valentijn, S. A. M., Hill, R. D., Van Hooren, S. A. H., Bosma, H., Van Boxtel, M. P. J., 7 Jolles, J., & Ponds, R. W. H. M. (2006). Memory self-efficacy predicts memory 8 performance: Results from a 6-year follow-up study. Psychology and Aging, 21(1), 9 165-172. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.21.2.165 10 11 12 13 Vestergren, P., & Nilsson, L.-G. (2011). Perceived causes of everyday memory problems in a population-based sample aged 39-99. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 25(4), 641‑646. Wechsler, D. (2001). Echelle clinique de mémoire de Wechsler, 3ème édition. Paris: Editions du Centre De Psychologie Appliquée 14 West, R. L., Bagwell, D. K., & Dark-Freudeman, A. (2008). Self-efficacy and memory aging: 15 The impact of a memory intervention based on self-efficacy. Aging, Neuropsychology, 16 and Cognition, 15(3), 302-329. doi: 10.1080/13825580701440510 17 West, R. L., Bagwell, D. K., & King, A. (2006). The impact of group size on memory and 18 memory beliefs: A methodological report. Experimental Aging Research, 32(3), 297- 19 316. doi: 10.1080/03610730600699092 20 21 22 23 24 25 White, M. J., Kruczek, T. A., Brown, M. T., & White, G. B. (1989). Occupational sex stereotypes among college students. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 34(3), 289‑298. White, M. J., & White, G. B. (2006). Implicit and explicit occupational gender stereotypes. Sex Roles, 55(3-4), 259‑266. Zelinski, E. M., & Gilewski, M. J. (2004). A 10-item Rasch modeled memory self-efficacy scale. Aging & Mental Health, 8(4), 293-306. doi: 10.1080/13607860410001709665 MSE and beliefs about forgetting and aging - 29 1 Conflict of Interest Statement 2 The authors declare that there are no conflicts of interest. 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 MSE and beliefs about forgetting and aging - 30 1 Footnotes 1 Because the effect of age and BAFA on MSE may be curvilinear rather than linear, we tested the quadratic effects of age and BAFA on each MSE measure. None of these effects was significant. 2 This model was tested on a sample of 284 participants because results for participants with missing data or extreme MFQ-RF values were excluded.
© Copyright 2025 Paperzz