Graham – Semantic dementia Update Comment Semantic dementia: a challenge to the multiple-trace theory? Kim S. Graham I n their recent reviews, Nadel and Moscovitch1–3 challenged the ‘standard’ model of memory consolidation, in which the hippocampal complex plays a temporary role in the acquisition and storage of recent episodic memories4–7. Instead, Nadel and Moscovitch1 proposed a multiple-trace theory in which ‘consolidation processes last on the order of decades, or indeed the entire lifetime’ (p. 221), on the basis that retrograde amnesia in patients with damage to the hippocampus and related structures is often long-lasting, extending back in time for as much as 25–40 years (although see Knowlton and Fanselow8 for an alternative view). The main aim of this commentary is to discuss whether neuropsychological data from patients with semantic dementia, who present with focal atrophy to the inferolateral temporal neocortex with sparing of the hippocampal complex9 are a challenge to Nadel and Moscovitch’s multiple-trace theory. The neuropsychology of semantic dementia Patients with semantic dementia, also called progressive fluent aphasia10, present with problems in producing content words in spontaneous speech and, as the disorder progresses, in word comprehension11–16. Detailed neuropsychological studies of patients with semantic dementia have documented four main characteristics: (1) a loss of verbal and non-verbal semantic knowledge about objects, facts, concepts, people and the meanings of words; (2) preservation of the phonological and syntactic aspects of language; (3) normal non-verbal problem-solving; and (4) good visual-spatial and perceptual abilities11–20. Neuroradiological investigations in patients with the syndrome reveal focal atrophy of the inferolateral temporal gyri of one or both temporal lobes, with sparing (at least early in the disease) of the hippocampi, parahippocampal gyri, and subiculum9,11,12,21–24. Experimental investigations of longterm memory in semantic dementia have found a neuropsychological profile which, in some senses, is complementary to that seen in amnesic patients. Two studies, which used the Autobiographical Memory Interview25,26, documented better retrieval of autobiographical memories from the recent time-period compared with childhood and early adulthood in patients with the disease21,27. This data initially suggested that patients with semantic dementia show a reverse temporal gradient on tests of autobiographical memory, a double dissociation when compared with data from amnesic patients21. Another, more detailed, study of autobiographical memory in a single case of semantic dementia (AM) revealed, however, that the preservation of recent autobiographical memories is typically more ‘step-like’ in nature, as opposed to a genuine temporal gradient. AM was asked to produce detailed and time-specific autobiographical memories in response to 15 cue words which were given in each of four time periods which spanned the entire of his life. AM produced significantly more detailed and time-specific memories for the most recent two years of his life, while the majority of his autobiographical memories from the preceding 63 years lacked detail, were not specific in time and were typically a superficial description of similar events that had occurred at different times21. Control subjects showed no significant difference in performance across the four time periods. A similar, striking, ‘step-like’ effect of time has also been demonstrated on tests of semantic knowledge: patients have been found to possess significantly better factual information about current celebrities17,27 and recent famous events28 than for people and events from the more distant past. As for autobiographical memory, patients with semantic dementia show better performance in the recent past for only a short period of time, typically spanning the one or two years prior to testing. Consistent with the better recall of recent autobiographical and semantic information on tests of remote memory in the syndrome are findings of preserved non-verbal recognition memory and good re-learning of vocabulary (Refs 22,29 and Graham et al., unpublished data). The multiple-trace model of memory consolidation In these studies, it has been proposed that the experimental data from semantic dementia is strong support for the standard model of memory consolidation, in which the hippocampal complex plays a time-limited role in the acquisition and storage of episodic and semantic memory4,5,7,17,21,28,30,31. In their 1997 article, however, Nadel and Moscovitch1 suggest a pertinent alternative explanation for the time-based effects seen on autobiographical memory tests in the disorder: impaired strategic retrieval caused by additional frontal pathology. According to Nadel and Moscovitch’s1–3 multiple-trace model, repeated reinstatement of an event (dependent upon the hippocampal complex and neocortex) results in the formation of multiple memory traces, which comprise almost all the components of the event, within the hippocampal complex. While this process allows the consolidation of ‘factual’ information into our semantic system, episodic memories remain dependent upon continuing interactions between the hippocampal complex and neocortex. In Nadel and Moscovitch1, the authors proposed that older memories are represented by more traces compared to recent memories and that the number of memory traces is positively correlated with ease of retrieval (i.e. older episodic memories should be easier to retrieve from the hippocampus than recent memories). Given this point, neurologically normal subjects (if tested appropriately) should show better retrieval of older episodic memories compared to those from their more recent past. With respect to the pattern of retrograde amnesia typically seen in amnesic patients, whereby older personal episodes are less affected than more recent memories, Moscovitch and Nadel3 suggest that lesion size is a critical factor: as older memories will, in general, have more memory traces, the representation of older memories will comprise greater amounts of the hippocampus than more recent memories. This proposal predicts that partial hippocampal lesions will be more likely to impair recent autobiographical memories compared to older memories and, furthermore, size of hippocampal lesion will be positively correlated with the extent of retrograde amnesia. Evidence from semantic dementia Turning now to semantic dementia, does the multiple-trace model explain the pattern of remote memory impairment seen on tests of autobiographical memory in the disease? Given that there is accruing evidence that the hippocampus is relatively spared early in the 1364-6613/99/$ – see front matter © 1999 Elsevier Science. All rights reserved. PII: S1364-6613(99)01284-X Trends in Cognitive Sciences – Vol. 3, No. 3, March 1999 Kim Graham is at the MRC Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit, 15 Chaucer Road, Cambridge, UK CB2 2EF. tel: +44 1223 355294 ext. 790 fax: +44 1223 359062 e-mail: Kim.Graham @mrc-cbu.cam.ac.uk 85 Update Graham – Semantic dementia Comment disease9, the multiple-trace model predicts that patients with semantic dementia should show a similar temporal pattern of performance to control subjects on tests of autobiographical memory: better retrieval of older episodic memories, which are represented by more hippocampal traces, compared to personal events from their more recent past. Of course, given the progressive deterioration of the temporal neocortex over the course of the disease, it is likely that autobiographical memory, which is reliant upon continuing interactions between the hippocampus and neocortex, will be impaired in patients with the disease. The multiple-trace theory does not predict, however, that this deficit will be affected by the age of the memories, over and above that expected for control subjects. Yet patients with semantic dementia do show a striking effect of time, whereby very recent autobiographical and semantic memories are better preserved than those from the more distant past. To explain this result, Nadel and Moscovitch1 proposed that malfunctioning, or inoperative, frontal processes could affect retrieval of remote memories more than recent memories writing that, ‘Indeed, because one can envisage situations in which recovery of remote memories requires more strategic retrieval than recovery of recent memories (i.e. what one did yesterday versus last summer versus the summer of one’s 14th year), it may be possible to obtain a temporal gradient opposite to that observed following medial temporal lobe lesions. This prediction has recently been supported by a study of subjects with semantic dementia, typically assumed to involve frontal–temporal damage’ (p. 224). While this is a valid proposal, it is not clear that the striking reverse temporal ‘step-function’ seen in our patients (for example, AM who showed preservation of autobiographical memories for the last two years of his life) would be a necessary consequence of this type of damage. First, Nadel and Moscovitch predict that patients with semantic dementia should show a reverse temporal gradient in autobiographical memory. While this is true for tests such as the Autobiographical Memory Interview25, it is not evident on tasks which measure the integrity of autobiographical memory for the whole of a patient’s life21. Second, given ‘that the number of memory traces’ favours better recall of older memories and ‘impaired strategic retrieval’ assists recall of recent memories in the multiple-trace theory, is it necessarily true that these two ‘opposing’ factors will interact to produce the ‘step-function’ seen in patients with semantic dementia? Evidence from the frontal variant of fronto–temporal dementia Ignoring the theoretical issues concerning malfunctioning retrieval processes for the moment, one piece of data which would support Nadel and Moscovitch’s1–3 position would be documentation of a reverse temporal gradient in patients with the frontal variant of fronto– temporal dementia. Hodges and Gurd32 described a 67-year-old man with a sixmonth history of personality change: having previously been a placid, sociable and responsible man, he became increasingly disorganised, unmotivated and started to display poor judgement. A CT scan at initial presentation revealed mild selective frontal atrophy with enlargement of the anterior portion of the lateral ventricles bilaterally. Contrary to Nadel and Moscovitch’s theory, Hodges and Gurd’s patient showed no evidence of a reverse temporal gradient on any tests of remote memory (naming of famous faces, public events and autobiographical memory). The patient’s autobiographical memories were, ‘rambling responses that were lacking in specific detail and were often not time-specific… All the datable responses (six out of 10) came from his early life (1930s and 1940s)’ (p. 825). It is clear from this description of the patient’s performance on the autobiographical memory test that, (1) he was impaired compared to control subjects; and (2) he showed the opposite temporal pattern to that seen in semantic dementia, with most of his memories coming from his early life (1930s and 1940s). While one has to be cautious in extrapolating from a single case, in particular because Hodges and Gurd’s patient showed evidence of a mild anterograde amnesia which may have been due to hippocampal damage, the result emphasises that patients with the frontal variant of fronto–temporal dementia may not show the same reverse temporal stepfunction that has been consistently documented in semantic dementia. Conclusion While Nadel and Moscovitch1–3 are to be congratulated for challenging the validity of the standard model of memory consolidation and suggesting a compelling alternative, it is not clear whether the experimental data from patients with semantic dementia are consistent with the multiple-trace theory. In particular, it is difficult to see how the authors’ ‘strategic retrieval hypothesis’ can account for the striking step-function (the preservation of very recent memories) seen on tests of autobiographical and semantic memory in patients with the disease. Nor is there, at least presently, any empirical evidence that patients with the frontal variant of frontal–temporal dementia will show a reverse temporal gradient on tests of autobiographical memory32. Nadel and Moscovitch1 write that, ‘we will encourage investigators to reexamine the notion of long-term consolidation and to consider alternatives that are more biologically and psychologically plausible’ (p. 225). Given that 86 Trends in Cognitive Sciences – Vol. 3, No. 3, March 1999 the multiple trace theory has to invoke two separate explanations to justify the classic Ribot gradient and the reverse temporal gradient seen in amnesia and semantic dementia respectively, while the standard model of memory consolidation can account for both patterns without additional neuropsychological deficits, the latter theory seems to provide (at least for the present) a more parsimonious explication for neuropsychological deficits of long-term memory. Acknowledgements I would like to thank Mike Page, Adrian Owen, Jon Simons, Mieke Verfaellie and John Hodges for comments on this article. References 1 Nadel, L. and Moscovitch, M. (1997) Memory consolidation, retrograde amnesia and the hippocampal complex Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 7, 217–227 2 Nadel, L. and Moscovitch, M. (1998) Hippocampal contributions to cortical plasticity Neuropharmacology 37, 431–439 3 Moscovitch, M. and Nadel, L. (1998) Consolidation and the hippocampal complex revisited: in defense of the multiple-trace model – Discussion point Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 8, 297–300 4 Squire, L.R. (1992) Memory and the hippocampus: a synthesis from findings with rats, monkeys, and humans Psychol. Rev. 99, 195–231 5 McClelland, J.L., McNaughton, B.L. and O’Reilly, R.C. (1995) Why there are complementary learning systems in the hippocampus and neocortex: insights from the successes and failures of connectionist models of learning and memory Psychol. Rev. 102, 419–457 6 Murre, J.M.J. (1997) Implicit and explicit memory in amnesia: some explanations and predictions by the TraceLink Model Memory 5, 213–232 7 Alvarez, P. and Squire, L. (1994) Memory consolidation and the medial temporal lobe: a simple network model Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 91, 7041–7045 8 Knowlton, B.J. and Fanselow, M.S. (1998) The hippocampus, consolidation and on-line memory Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 8, 293–296 9 Mummery, C.J. et al. (1999) Disrupted temporal lobe connections in semantic dementia Brain 122, 61–73 10 Mesulam, M.M. (1982) Slowly progressive aphasia without generalised dementia Ann. Neurol. 11, 592–598 11 Hodges, J.R. and Patterson, K. (1996) Nonfluent progressive aphasia and semantic dementia: a comparative neuropsychological study J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 2, 511–524 12 Breedin, S.D., Saffran, E.M. and Coslett, H.B. (1994) Reversal of the concreteness effect in a patient with semantic dementia Cognit. Neuropsychol. 11, 617–660 13 Hodges, J.R., Graham, N. and Patterson, K. (1995) Charting the progression in semantic dementia: implications for the organisation of semantic memory Memory 3, 463–495 14 Hodges, J.R. et al. (1992) Semantic dementia: progressive fluent aphasia with temporal Graham – Semantic dementia Update Comment lobe atrophy Brain 115, 1783–1806 15 Hodges, J.R., Patterson, K. and Tyler, L.K. (1994) Loss of semantic memory: implications for the modularity of mind TraceLink model, in Neural Modelling in 32 Hodges, J.R. and Gurd, J. (1994) Remote Brain Disorders (Reggia, J. et al., eds), Elsevier memory and lexical retrieval in a case of (in press) frontal Pick’s disease Arch. Neurol. 51, 821–827 Cognit. Neuropsychol. 11, 505–542 16 Snowden, J.S., Goulding, P.J. and Neary, D. (1989) Semantic circumscribed dementia: cerebral a form atrophy of Behav. Neurol. 2, 167–182 17 Hodges, J.R. and Graham, K.S. (1998) A reversal of the temporal gradient for famous person knowledge in semantic dementia: Multiple-trace theory and semantic dementia: Response to K.S. Graham (1999) implications for the neural organisation of long-term memory Neuropsychologia 36, 803–825 18 Snowden, J., Griffiths, H. and Neary, D. (1994) Semantic dementia: autobiographical contribution to preservation of meaning. Cognit. Neuropsychol. 11, 265–288 19 Snowden, J., Griffiths, H. and Neary, D. (1995) Autobiographical experience and word meaning Memory 3, 225–246 20 Snowden, J.S., Neary, D. and Mann, D.M.A. (1996) Fronto–temporal Lobar Degeneration: Fronto–temporal Aphasia, Dementia, Progressive Semantic Dementia, K.S. Hodges, Churchill Livingstone 21 Graham, and J.R. (1997) Differentiating the roles of the hippocampal complex and the neocortex in long-term memory storage: evidence from the study of semantic dementia and Alzheimer’s disease Neuropsychology 11, 77–89 22 Graham, K.S., Becker, J.T. and Hodges, J.R. (1997) On knowledge the and relationship memory for between pictures: evidence from the study of patients with semantic dementia and Alzheimer’s disease study J. Int. Neuropsychol. 3, 534–544 23 Harasty, J.A. et al. (1996) Quantification of cortical atrophy in a case of progressive fluent aphasia Brain 119, 181–190 24 Hodges, J.R., Garrard, P. and Patterson, K. (1998) Semantic dementia, in Pick’s Disease and Pick Complex (Kertesz, A. and Munoz, D.G., eds), pp. 83–104, John Wiley & Co. 25 Kopelman, M.D., Wilson, B.A. and Baddeley, A.D. (1990) The Autobiographical Memory Interview, Thames Valley Test Company 26 Greene, J.D.W., Baddeley, A.D. and Hodges, J.R. (1995) Autobiographical memory and executive function in early dementia of Alzheimer type Neuropsychologia 33, 1647–1670 27 Snowden, J.S., Griffiths, H.L. and Neary, D. (1996) Semantic–episodic memory interactions in semantic retrograde dementia: memory implications function for Cognit. Neuropsychol. 13, 1101–1137 28 Graham, K.S., Pratt, K.H. and Hodges, J.R. (1998) A reverse temporal gradient for public events in a single case of semantic dementia Neurocase 4, 461–470 29 Graham, K.S. et al. Relearning and subsequent forgetting of semantic category exemplars in a case of semantic dementia. Neuropsychol. (in press) 30 Murre, J.M.J. (1996) A model of amnesia and consolidation of memory Hippocampus 6, 675–684 31 Graham, K.S., Murre, J.M.J. and Hodges, J.R. Episodic memory in semantic dementia: a computational approach based on the W e are indebted to Graham for raising the issue of semantic dementia and its implications for multiple-trace theory [Graham, K.S. (1999) Semantic dementia: a challenge to the multipletrace theory of memory consolidation? Trends Cognit. Sci. 3, 85–87]1. Investigating people with semantic dementia to study hippocampal function and episodic memory is an innovative departure from the typical strategy of studying people with amnesia who have lesions to the medial temporal lobe and diencephalon. By capitalizing on the observation that the medial temporal lobes are spared in the early to middle stages of semantic dementia, one can evaluate their contribution to episodic (and semantic) memory when the lateral inferotemporal cortex, which supports semantic memory, is damaged. In their investigations, Graham and Hodges2 confirmed and extended Snowden et al’s3 initial observations that episodic and semantic memories based on recent experiences are relatively spared in semantic dementia, whereas remote memories are deteriorated or lost. Graham and Hodges2,4 concluded that these results provided evidence for traditional consolidation theory, which states that the medial temporal lobes or hippocampal complex play only a time-limited role in memory. By this view, once memories are consolidated, they are retrieved directly from the neocortex and other structures in which the memory or engram is represented. Because semantic dementia results from neocortical degeneration, only these remote, consolidated memories would be lost. More recent memories would be spared because they had not yet been consolidated and could still be recovered via the medial temporal lobe structures that are intact. We believe this interpretation is neither the only, nor the best, available for data from these intriguing cases. We recently offered an alternative to traditional consolidation theory, the multiple-trace theory5–7, which Graham1 believes fares less well than the traditional view in accounting for the evidence from semantic dementia. In order to respond to her criticisms, we first would like to restate our theory briefly because we believe that Graham may have misinterpreted some aspects of it. For example, Graham quotes us out of context when she says we proposed that ‘consolidation processes last on the order of decades, or indeed the entire lifetime’ (p. 221). That statement referred not to our theory but to what the traditional theory would have to conclude based on reports of retrograde amnesia that lasts decades or even a lifetime. Also, she refers only to the hippocampus whereas we explicitly stated that our theory also includes the adjacent structures in the medial temporal lobes, all of which comprises what we called the hippocampal complex. The multiple-trace theory According to our theory, during consolidation the neural elements in neocortex and elsewhere mediating a particular conscious experience are bound into a cohesive unit by the hippocampus and adjacent medial temporal lobe structures (the hippocampal complex). The resulting memory trace or engram consists of a cohesive medial temporalneocortical ensemble. The medial temporal component, which may provide the spatial context of the experience, acts as a pointer or index to the neocortical elements needed to provide the detailed content of the experience. The medial temporal lobe units are an integral part of the episodic memory trace and are needed to recover it as long as it exists. Over time, as an episodic memory is recovered, it is recoded so that multiple, related memory traces are formed and dispersed over wider areas of the hippocampal complex. Because we believe that semantic memories can be acquired or retained without the medial temporal lobes (see Ref. 8), we confined our model only to episodic, autobiographical memories. By virtue of the multiple-trace aspect of the model, more extensive lesions to the hippocampal complex are required to expunge more remote memories that are multiply-represented and widely distributed. Because each of the subregions of the hippocampal complex has a distinct function, and because autobiographical, episodic memories are multifaceted and rich in detail, it is likely that each of these subregions is involved in storing or recovering some aspect of a remembered experience. Incomplete lesions to the complex will lead to a temporally-graded retrograde amnesia (RA). Damage to all the subregions, or to those 1364-6613/99/$ – see front matter © 1999 Elsevier Science. All rights reserved. PII: S1364-6613(99)01290-5 Trends in Cognitive Sciences – Vol. 3, No. 3, March 1999 M. Moscovitch is at the Department of Psychology, University of Toronto, Toronto, Canada. tel: +1 416 785 2500 ext. 3132 fax: +1 416 785 2862 e-mail: momos@ credit.erin.utoronto.ca L. Nadel is at the Department of Psychology, University of Arizona, AZ, USA. tel: +1 502 621 7449 fax: +1 520 621 9306 e-mail: nadel@u. arizona.edu 87
© Copyright 2025 Paperzz