Structure and Agreement in Ojibwe Transitive Verbs∗

Structure and Agreement in Ojibwe Transitive Verbs∗
Kate Riccomini
University of Ottawa
[email protected]
March 28, 2015
1
Introduction
• The Algonquian languages are known for their complex transitive verbal morphology
• A number of people have proposed ways of accounting for it (e.g. Bruening 2005; Béjar
& Rezac 2009; Lochbihler 2012; Oxford 2013)
• My eventual goal is to derive the transitive agreement morphology
• To come up with the most accurate derivation of agreement, it is necessary to know
what is going on in the rest of the verb phrase
Goals of this paper:
• Describe the structural template of the different types of Ojibwe verbs
– I will show that Ojibwe verbs may contain multiple vPs
• Examine Cyclic Agree, one of the most successful previous accounts of Ojibwe verbal
agreement.
– I will show that it cannot, in fact, work with the verbal structures I propose
• Outline my own proposal for Ojibwe verbal agreement
2
Ojibwe Data
2.1
Verbal Morphology
The Ojibwe verb is made of at least two parts:
• initial - verbal root with potentially nominal, adjectival, or verbal meaning
Special thanks to Éric Mathieu, Kyumin Kim, and the members of the U. Ottawa Syntax-Semantics
Lab for helpful comments on previous versions of this paper.
∗
1
• final - verbalising head, marks transitivity, animacy of transitive object/intransitive
subject, carries some semantic meaning
(Valentine, 2001, p333)1
Some example Ojibwe verbs, with initials and finals glossed separately:2
(1) a. mskozi
misko -zi
red
-vai
“be red (an.)”
b. mskwaa
misko -aa
red
-vii
“be red (inan.)”
(Valentine, 2001, p342)
(2) a. mnopaw
mino -ipw
good -taste.vta
“like the taste of X (an.)”
b. mnopdan
mino -ipid.am
good -taste.vti
“like the taste of X (inan.)”
(Valentine, 2001, p461)
(3) a. nokii
anok -ii
work -move.vai
“work”
b. nokiitoon
anok -ii
-it.oo
work -move.vai -cause.vti
“cause X to work” 3
(Valentine, 2001, p435)
Algonquianists break verbs down into four types based on their morphology (Valentine, 2001,
p132):
•
•
•
•
transitive animate (vta) - transitive verb, animate object
transitive inanimate (vti) - transitive verb, inanimate object
animate intransitive (vai) - intransitive verb, animate subject
inanimate intransitive (vii) - intransitive verb, inanimate subject
The category of each verb is determined by the verb final. (Valentine, 2001, p132)
2.2
Transitive Agreement
• Topicality hierarchy and direct/inverse morphology
• Hierarchy ranks participants by personhood
• Full hierarchy: 2 > 1 > X > 3 > 3′ > 0 (Valentine, 2001, p268)
– where X is an unspecified actor, 3′ is third person obviative, and 0 is third person
inanimate
Verbs may optionally contain a third part, a medial, an incorporated element, including classifiers or
nominals (not discussed here).
2
Note, despite the glosses, these examples are not infinitives; it is not possible to form infinitives in
Ojibwe.(Valentine, 2001, p648)
3
Abbreviations used in the glosses: an = animate, inan = inanimate, sg = singular, pl = plural, 1 =
first person, 2 = second person, 3 = third person animate proximate, 3′ = third person animate obviative,
0 = third person inanimate, > = acts on/does to, as in “X does verb to Y”
1
2
• The highest participant on the hierarchy will be spelled out as a proclitic
• Transitive verbs have a suffix called a theme sign; the vta theme sign, is a portmanteau
morpheme indexing both arguments
– See Example (4)
(4) a. Direct:
Nwaabmaa.
ni- waab -am -aa
1- see -vta -1>3
“I see him.”
3
b. Inverse:
Nwaabmig.
ni- waab -am -ig
1- see -vta -3>1
“He sees me.”
(Valentine, 2001, p287)
Verbal Templates
3.1
3.1.1
•
•
•
•
•
Theoretical Tools
The External Argument as Spec VoiceP
Kratzer (1996), building on Marantz (1984) and Hale & Keyser (1993)
The external argument (EA) is truly external from the verb, i.e. outside of vP
Voice, a head that introduces the EA and assigns accusative case 4
VoiceP appears in the derivation just above vP
Hirose (2001), for Plains Cree
– Multiple vP structures, where each vP introduces an argument in its specifier
– For Hirose, the external argument is the specifier of the highest vP (for verbs that
have an external argument); this high vP functions like Kratzer’s (1996) VoiceP
• Oxford (2013), for Proto-Algonquian, the reconstructed ancestor of the Algonquian
language family
– Voice hosts an agreement probe, as well as introducing the external argument in
spec-Voice.
– Oxford derives most of the Proto-Algonquian theme signs as Voice heads; in my
proposal, Voice is null, and the theme signs are portmanteau agreement morphemes
3.1.2
The Internal Argument as Spec vP
• In Distributed Morphology, lexical items are assumed to be made up of an acategorical
root, and a categorising morpheme (v, n, or a) (Halle & Marantz, 1993; Embick &
Noyer, 2007); a structure where first Merge is V and v captures this5
• v is a verbalising head as per Hale & Keyser (1993) and Marantz (1997)
4
5
Case assignment is not applicable to Ojibwe, which does not appear to have Case.
√
Where V and are both labels for the same head.
3
• Ditransitives introduce a second argument by introducing a second vP, something
which can be easily captured by assuming internal arguments are introduced as specvPs; Hirose (2001) had a similar idea about how arguments were introduced in Plains
Cree, although he explained it as a modification of the Theta Criterion
• A number of authors have equated the verb final with v, (e.g. Hirose 2001; Brittain
2003; Mathieu 2006; Ritter & Rosen 2010; Slavin 2012; Oxford 2013; Mathieu 2014)
• Both Hirose (2001) (for Plains Cree, as mentioned) and Oxford (2013) (for ProtoAlgonquian) propose structures in which the internal argument is introduced in specvP
3.2
Transitive Verbs
Transitive verbs:
• may have either vta or vti finals
• will therefore have either an animate or inanimate object
• one of the arguments, either the subject or the object, must be animate; only animate
subjects are extant in my available data (Valentine, 2001, p426)
• all transitive verb finals are followed by a theme sign
– vta theme signs constitute part of agreement morphology, and vary based on the
personhood of the subject and object
– vti theme signs are ornamental morphology added at PF (Embick & Noyer, 2007);
they are invariant and add nothing semantic or syntactic to the verb (Valentine,
2001, p306)
Example transitive derivations6 :
(5) a. Gwaabndaan.
gi- waab -andam -n
2- see -vti
-0.sg
“You see it.”
(Valentine, 2001, p311)
b.
VoiceP
DP
2
Voice′
∅
vP
DP
0.sg
v′
v
-andam
vti
6
√
waab
“see”
In the following trees, I am abstracting away from the agreement facts. I will return to them in Section
4
4
(6) a. Gwaabmaa.
gi- waab -am -aa
2- see -vta -2>3
“You see him/her.”
(Valentine, 2001, p287)
b.
VoiceP
DP
2
Voice′
∅
vP
DP
3.sg
3.3
v′
√
v
-am waab
vta “see”
Ditransitives
Ditransitive verbs:
• have vta morphology
• have a multiple vP structure
• show agreement with the subject and the Goal (not the Theme)
(Valentine, 2001, p136)
Example ditransitive derivation:
(7) a. ndazhtamaag
nid- izhi -it
-amaw
-ig
1- thus -cause.vti -for.X.vta -3>1
“she is making it for me.”
(adapted from Valentine 2001, p700)
5
b.
VoiceP
DP
3
Voice′
∅
vP
DP
1
v′
v
-amaw
“for X”
4
vP
DP
∅
0
v′
v
-it
“cause”
√
izhi
“thus”
Agreement Morphology
4.1
Agreement in the Syntax: Cyclic Agree
• Original proposed by Béjar & Rezac (2009)
• Agree checks the features of the complement; if unchecked features remain, it proceeds
to check features of the specifier
• For Ojibwe, v is a probe with a fully specified set of person features arranged in a
hierarchy of entailment relations
• Like Béjar & Rezac (2009), Lochbihler (2012) proposes there is a v head that agrees
cyclically, first with the internal argument, then with the external argument
• When a feature is checked, features entailed by it are made active, but not deleted
themselves
• In the direct, the external argument deletes inactive features, and in the inverse, it
deletes already active features
– See Example (8) and (9)
• When vocabulary insertion occurs, the proclitic is inserted for the argument with the
most highly specified features, and the theme sign for the other; the choice of theme sign
is further determined by whether the less specified argument has checked an inactive
or already active feature
(8) a. Direct:
Nwaabmaa.
ni- waab -am -aa
1- see -vta -1>3
“I see him.”
6
(Valentine, 2001, p287)
b. Cycle 1:
v
Cycle 2:
′
VP
v
uπ
u3
DP
‘him’
vP
DP
‘I’
V
waabm
v′
VP
v
uπ
u1
u3
u2
u1
DP
‘him’
V
waabm
u2
(adapted from Lochbihler 2012, p47)
(9) a. Inverse:
Nwaabmig.
ni- waab -am -ig
1- see -vta -3>1
“He sees me.”
(Valentine, 2001, p287)
b. Cycle 1:
Cycle 2:
v′
v
uπ
u3
vP
VP
DP
‘I’
DP
‘him’
V
waabm
v′
v
uπ
u1
u3
u2
u1
VP
DP
V
‘I’ waabm
u2
(adapted from Lochbihler 2012, p49)
• This version of Cyclic Agree has the major advantage that the inverse is not a more
marked case than the direct
• Also, it does not violate the Inclusiveness Condition by adding an extra v probe; consequently, no stipulation is needed to suppress spell out of an extra head, no extra head
having been created
4.1.1
Ditransitives and Other Multi-vP Structures
Both Béjar & Rezac’s (2009) and Lochbihler’s (2012) proposals have difficulty accounting
for ditransitive verbs, and other multiple vP structures.
7
For ditransitives:
• Cyclic Agree, as stated, specifies the probe searches all the way down through the
complement before searching upwards into the specifier
• This means that, for ditransitives, v should be able to Agree with both the Goal and
the Theme
• However, the verb Agrees with the Agent and the Goal, but not the Theme
• One way to repair this problem is to stipulate, as Hamilton (2014) does, that Cyclic
Agree only looks down one level into the complement, a locality condition we don’t see
in operation in other languages
For all multiple vP structures:
•
•
•
•
4.2
v contains the Agreement probe
Multiple vPs means multiple Agreement probes
Yet, only one instance of Agree appears to occur
Even if it is assumed that only transitive v contains a fully specified Agree probe,
ditransitives contain two transitive v heads
Towards a Better Explanation
• My own proposal for Ojibwe agreement morphology is partially based on Oxford (2013)
• Like Oxford (2013), I use multiple Agree (Hiraiwa, 2001) and multiple equi-distant
specifiers (Chomsky, 2001)
• Oxford (2013) was working with Proto-Algonquian, so parts of his analysis do not
apply to Ojibwe
In my proposal:
• The internal argument moves up to spec-VoiceP, making it equi-distant with the external argument for the purpose of agreement
• The theme sign is a portmanteau morpheme appearing above VoiceP that Agrees with
both the external and internal arguments
• Bliss, Ritter & Wiltschko (2014) argue that part of agreement, in the form of the nonlocal theme signs in Ojibwe and Blackfoot, is spelled out as Point-of-View aspect, on
a head above vP; more research is needed if this is the best analysis to describe the
Ojibwe data
• There is also the pronominal proclitic, which must move to attach to the verb above
T, in spec-TP
• As per languages like English, there is an EPP feature that causes one of the arguments
to move up to spec-TP
• However, because the internal and external arguments are equally far apart when this
EPP feature is triggered, the syntax needs a different way to choose which argument
moves
8
• Therefore, instead of the most local argument (the subject) moving up to spec-TP, the
highest argument on the hierarchy (regardless of whether it is subject or object) is the
argument that moves to spec-TP, and is spelled out as the pronominal proclitic
– See Example (10) and (11)
– Solid lines indicate movement
– Dashed lines indicate Agree
(10) Direct:
TP
DP
Subject
T′
T
FP
F
Agr
VoiceP
DP
Object
Voice′
DP
Subject
Voice′
∅
vP
DP
Object
9
v′
√
v
Final Initial
(11) Inverse:
TP
DP
Object
T′
T
FP
F
Agr
VoiceP
DP
Object
Voice′
DP
Subject
Voice′
∅
vP
DP
Object
v′
v
Final
5
√
Initial
Conclusion
In sum, this paper has:
• Explored the structure of Ojibwe transitive verbal morphology
• Discussed Cyclic Agree as a method of accounting for Ojibwe verbal agreement
• Used this understanding of Ojibwe verbal structure to motivate an approach to agreement using Multiple Agree and equi-distance of specifiers as theoretical tools
References
Béjar, S. & Rezac, M. (2009). Cyclic agree. Linguistic Inquiry, 40 (1), pp. 35–73.
Bliss, H., Ritter, E., & Wiltschko, M. (2014). A comparative analysis of theme marking in
blackfoot and nishnaabemwin. In Valentine, J. R. & Macaulay, M. (Eds.), Papers of the
Forty-Second Algonquian Conference, (pp. 10–33). SUNY Press.
Brittain, J. (2003). A distributed morphology account of the syntax of the algonquian verb.
Ms., Memorial University.
Bruening, B. (2005). The Algonquian inverse is syntactic: binding in Passamaquoddy. University of Delaware.
10
Chomsky, N. (2001). Derivtion by phase. In M. Kenstowicz (Ed.), Ken Hale: A life in
language (pp. 1–52). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Embick, D. & Noyer, R. (2007). Distributed morphology and the syntax/morphology interface. In G. Ramchand & C. Reiss (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of linguistic interfaces (pp.
289–324). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Hale, K. & Keyser, S. J. (1993). On argument structure and the lexical expression of syntactic
relations. In K. Hale & S. J. Keyser (Eds.), The view from building 20: Essays in linguistics
in honor of Sylvian Bromberger (pp. 53–109). Cambridge, MA: MIT press.
Halle, M. & Marantz, A. (1993). Distributed morphology and the pieces of inflection. In
K. Hale & S. J. Keyser (Eds.), The view from building 20: Essays in linguistics in honor
of Sylvian Bromberger (pp. 111–176). Cambridge, MA: MIT press.
Hamilton, M. (2014). Multiple instances of agreement in Mi’gmaq verbs. http://ling.auf.
net/lingbuzz/002303.
Hiraiwa, K. (2001). Multiple agree and the defective intervention constraint in japanese.
MIT working papers in linguistics, 40 (40), 67–80.
Hirose, T. (2001). Origins of predicates: Evidence from plains cree. Copyright - Copyright
UMI - Dissertations Publishing 2001; Last updated - 2014-01-09; First page - n/a.
Kratzer, A. (1996). Severing the external argument from its verb. In Phrase structure and
the lexicon (pp. 109–137). Springer.
Lochbihler, B. (2012). Aspects of argument licensing. PhD thesis, Doctoral Dissertation,
McGill University.
Marantz, A. (1984). On the nature of grammatical relations. Number 10 in Linguistic Inquiry
Monographs. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.
Marantz, A. (1997). No escape from syntax: Don’t try morphological analysis in the privacy
of your own lexicon. University of Pennsylvania working papers in linguistics, 4 (2), 14.
Mathieu, É. (2006). The syntax of abstract and concrete finals in Ojibwe. In Elfner, E. &
Walkow, M. (Eds.), NELS: Proceedings of the North East Linguistic Society, volume 37,
(pp. 101–114). GLSA, University of Massachusetts/Amherst.
Mathieu, É. (2014). Nominalizations in Ojibwe. In I. Paul (Ed.), Cross-linguistic Investigations of Nominalization Patterns, volume 210 of Linguistics Today (pp. 3–24). John
Benjamins Publishing Company.
Oxford, W. R. (2013). Microparameters of agreement: A diachronic perspective on Algonquian verb inflection. PhD thesis, University of Toronto.
Ritter, E. & Rosen, S. T. (2010). Animacy in Blackfoot: Implications for event structure
and clause structure. In E. D. Malka Rappaport-Hovav & I. Sichel (Eds.), Syntax, lexical
semantics and event structure (pp. 124–152). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
11
Slavin, T. (2012). The syntax and semantics of stem composition in Ojicree. PhD thesis,
University of Toronto.
Valentine, R. (2001). Nishnaabemwin Reference Grammar. Toronto, ON: University of
Toronto Press.
12