Developmental Science 7:5 (2004), pp 581–598 PAPER Blackwell Publishing, Ltd. Learning during the newborn’s first meal: special resistance to retroactive interference Newborn resistance to retroactive interference Sarah J. Ferdinand Cheslock,1 Sarah K. Sanders and Norman E. Spear Center for Developmental Psychobiology, Department of Psychology, Binghamton University, USA 1. Sarah J. Ferdinand Cheslock is now at the Department of Psychology, Ithaca College, USA Abstract At their first postnatal meal, 3-hour-old rats learned an association between an odor and a sweet or bitter taste. Retention after a long interval or after associative interference was compared to that of 1-day-old rats. Despite equivalent and negligible effect of the long retention interval, contrary to infantile amnesia, newborns differed strikingly from 1-day-olds in susceptibility to associative interference. When lemon odor predicted saccharin in the first episode but quinine in the second, 1-day-olds had strong retroactive interference, but the newborn’s first memory was unaffected by the second. The results were identical when the first memory was a lemon-quinine association and the second a lemon-saccharin association. It is uncertain whether this special robustness of memories associated with the first postnatal meal is best understood in terms of cognitive primacy or neurochemical and physiological consequences of the birth process. Introduction Nearly 100 years ago, Freud coined the term infantile amnesia, which refers, most popularly, to the inability of adults to recall the events of their infancy (see Pillemer & White, 1989, for review). Most adults report that their earliest autobiographical memory is of an event that occurred somewhere between 2 and 4 years old (e.g. Dudycha & Dudycha, 1933; Usher & Neisser, 1993). The phenomenon of infantile amnesia is not unique to the human condition; it is pervasive across every altricial species tested to date (Spear & Riccio, 1994). Immature animals (and humans) forget faster than their mature counterparts, even when the level of original learning is equivalent (e.g. Arnold & Spear, 1997; Campbell & Spear, 1972; Hartshorn et al., 1998; Spear & Rudy, 1991). Much research on the accelerated rate of forgetting in infancy has been done in the rat, which is born very immature, both deaf and blind, but matures to independence from the mother in about 3 weeks and to sexual maturity in about 2 months (Spear, 1979). Compared to older infants and adults, infant rats have a particularly difficult time learning trace conditioning (Spear & Rudy, 1991). In a trace conditioning preparation, the removal of the conditioned stimulus (CS), e.g. tone, and the introduction of the unconditioned stimulus (US), e.g. shock, are separated by a temporal gap, usually several seconds. Theoretically, to form an association between the CS and the US, the rat has to maintain a memory representation of the CS during the trace interval. In this view, the difficulty infant rats have learning trace conditioning could be considered an example of accelerated forgetting in infants across very short retention intervals (Spear & Rudy, 1991). Cheslock, Varlinskaya, Petrov & Spear (2000), however, found that newborn odor-taste conditioning was unusually rapid and robust, even resistant to the influence of a trace interval. One hour after a single experience with lemon odor paired with a taste of milk, saccharin or sucrose, naïve newborn rats altered their responding to an empty surrogate nipple in the presence of the lemon odor. The control groups responded to the empty nipple in the same way unconditioned pups normally would, with only brief grasps (e.g. Smotherman, Goffman, Petrov & Varlinskaya, 1997). Pups previously given a paired presentation of lemon and milk (or saccharin or sucrose), however, attached persistently to the empty nipple, spending more than 50% of the test time on the nipple. Essentially, in the presence of the lemon odor CS, experimental subjects responded to the empty nipple as Address for correspondence: Norman E. Spear, Center for Developmental Psychobiology, Department of Psychology, Binghamton University, Binghamton, New York 13902-6000, USA; e-mail: [email protected] © Blackwell Publishing Ltd. 2004, 9600 Garsington Road, Oxford OX4 2DQ, UK and 350 Main Street, Malden, MA 02148, USA. 582 Sarah J. Ferdinand Cheslock, Sarah K. Sanders and Norman E. Spear if it provided the palatable fluid with which the odor was previously paired (e.g. milk; see Smotherman, Petrov & Varlinskaya, 1997). That newborns were capable of expressing single-trial olfactory conditioning is not particularly surprising. Altered responding to a nipple capitalizes on the newborn’s ontogenetic niche and is an age-appropriate behavioral measure (see Alberts & Gubernick, 1984; Campbell, 1967; Johanson & Terry, 1988; Oppenheim, 1981; Spear, 1984; Spear & Rudy, 1991; West & King, 1987). Furthermore, learning an odor-taste pairing is biologically significant to the neonatal rat – odor signals are critical to its survival, guiding the neonate to the mother’s nipple, where it will receive warmth, protection and milk (Blass, 1990; Blass & Teicher, 1980; Singh & Hofer, 1978; Teicher & Blass, 1976, 1977). Accordingly, novel odors (e.g. citrus, cedar) are commonly used as successful conditioned stimuli in infant learning experiments (e.g. Johanson & Hall, 1982; Johanson, Hall & Polefrone, 1984; Johanson & Teicher, 1980). Similarly, stimuli that mimic the mother, or her care, function quite effectively as unconditioned stimuli in both operant and Pavlovian conditioning paradigms in infant rats. For example, pairing an odor with stroking (which mimics mother’s licking) results in preference for that odor in 6-day-old rats (Sullivan & Hall, 1988), and 1-day-old rats learn to raise their heads for an infusion of milk into their mouths (Johanson & Hall, 1979). What was surprising about the single-trial olfactory conditioning found by Cheslock et al. (2000) was its resistance to challenges. For instance, 1 hour after a paired presentation of lemon and milk, pups attached significantly longer than controls to a surrogate nipple providing saline, a highly aversive fluid. Normally, the newborn strongly rejects a surrogate nipple providing saline, which is so aversive that suckling experience with saline attenuates responding to a water nipple an hour later (Nizhnikov, Petrov, Varlinskaya & Spear, 2002; Smotherman, Petrov & Varlinskaya, 1997). Cheslock et al. (2000) also found strong conditioning even when a trace interval as long as 60 seconds was imposed between CS termination and US onset. Similarly, Varlinskaya, Petrov, Simonik and Smotherman (1997) found that fetal rats tolerated a 30-second trace interval in a conditioning paradigm that tested an association between a surrogate nipple (conditioned stimulus) and milk (unconditioned stimulus), as measured by conditioned reduction in perioral cutaneous responsiveness after a 5minute retention interval. Furthermore, with pre-exposure to the nipple CS, fetuses in Varlinskaya et al.’s study tolerated a trace interval of 2 minutes; the authors cite change in motor behavior following experience with the nipple CS (e.g. increased mouthing) as a possible © Blackwell Publishing Ltd. 2004 behavioral trace bridging the time gap between the nipple CS and milk US. Cheslock et al. (2000) did not measure motor changes during the trace interval between the lemon odor CS and the milk US. For the newborns in Cheslock et al. (2000) and the fetuses in Varlinskaya et al. (1997), the conditioning treatment can be construed as the subjects’ first ‘meal’ of its postnatal life. The surprising and impressive ability of newborn (odormilk) and fetal (nipple-milk) conditioning to withstand trace intervals suggests that learning about the first meal may be special. The purpose of the present study was to test whether a newborn rat’s memory for an odor-taste association (their first postnatal ‘meal’) is different to a 1-day-old’s memory for the same event. The difference of interest here is the memory’s resistance to common but ordinarily powerful sources of forgetting – long intervals and associative interference. Experiment 1 of the present study compared simple retention for lemon-saccharin pairing(s) after both short (1-hour) and long (24-hour) retention intervals in naïve newborns and relatively sophisticated 1-day-old rats. Using parameters established by Experiment 1, subsequent experiments introduced a source of associative interference. Experiments 2 and 3 tested resistance of an original odor-taste memory to a subsequent, conflicting odor-taste experience. Experiment 2 of the present study used aversive conditioning (lemon odor paired with quinine taste; Nizhnikov, Petrov & Spear, 2002) in an attempt to interfere retroactively with appetitive conditioning (lemon odor paired with saccharin) and tested pups both 1 hour and 24 hours after conditioning. Experiment 3 used appetitive conditioning (lemon paired with saccharin) to interfere retroactively with aversive conditioning (lemon paired with quinine). General method Subjects Experimental subjects were cesarean-delivered, neonatal, Sprague Dawley rats (Taconic Company, Germantown, New York) bred in our colony at Binghamton University. Adult animals were housed in groups of two females and one male in opaque, maternity tubs (45 × 23 × 20 cm), partially filled with pine shavings. For time breeding, vaginal smears were collected daily, between 7:30 and 9:30 , for the microscopic detection of sperm. The day sperm was detected was considered embryonic day zero (E0). When both females in a given cage had positive smears (or after a maximum of 5 days), the male was removed. For non-time breeding (for use as foster dams) Newborn resistance to retroactive interference vaginal smears were not taken and the male was removed after 10 days. Females were separated 3 weeks after the first day of pairing with the male and were checked twice daily for births (between 7:00 and 9:00 and again between 4:00 and 7:00 ). Cesarean section Near expected term (E21) pups were delivered by cesarean section. Under ether anesthesia, the pregnant female was sacrificed by rapid cervical dislocation. A midline incision was then cut through the abdominal wall and the uterus was externalized. Pups were delivered in rapid succession through individual, small incisions in the uterus. After the delivery of the last pup, extra-embryonic membranes were removed and pups were stimulated to breathe by vigorous, but gentle, wiping with warm, moist toweling. Pups were then turned on their sides and gently compressed with balled up, moist toweling, which helped to clear the airway of fluids. This procedure, from cervical dislocation to the firm establishment of independent respiration in all pups, was typically completed within 3–5 minutes. Umbilical cords were then tied and cut, and pups were placed in a plastic container (12 × 12 × 6 cm), lined with warm, moist paper towels, maintained at 35 degrees (± 1 degree) Celsius by a heating pad. Once in the plastic container, subjects were occasionally stimulated to maintain independent respiration. Only pups that remained pink throughout the entire procedure were used as experimental subjects. The container was then placed in an incubator (35.0 degrees (± 0.5 degree) Celsius; 90% humidity; Nursery Hospital Incubator, Petiatrics, Wichita, Kansas) to simulate nest conditions. Fostering When necessary, experimental pups were fostered to a dam that delivered her litter vaginally between 12 and 36 hours earlier (e.g. Grota, 1968a, 1968b). No fewer than six and no more than 12 experimental pups were fostered per dam. During fostering, the dam was placed in a holding cage while all of her pups were removed from her home cage and the experimental pups were introduced. Before returning the foster dam to her home cage, the experimental pups were gently rolled in her soiled shavings. All animals were housed in a temperaturecontrolled (22 degrees Celsius) vivarium maintained on a 14 hour light/10 hour dark cycle (lights on at 7:00 ) with ad libitum access to food (Purina ‘Formulab diet’ [5008] breeding formula, Ralston-Purina, St. Louis, Missouri) and water. Rats were maintained and treated in accordance with guidelines established by the National Institutes of Health (1986). © Blackwell Publishing Ltd. 2004 583 Experimental design and data analysis In order to conservatively compare newborns and 1-dayolds, method of delivery was made equivalent (cesareansection) and all animals were tested wearing a restraining vest (see General procedures and equipment). It was thought important to make equivalent gestation age (cesarean-delivered pups are born slightly immature), but especially exposure to the catecholamine surge at birth, which is thought to be stronger after vaginal delivery than elective cesarean-section delivery (e.g. Vyas, Milner, Hopkin & Falconer, 1983). Norepinephrine, the primary catecholamine associated with the hormonal surge at birth, has also been linked to olfactory learning in infant rodents (Sullivan, McGaugh & Leon, 1991; Sullivan, Wilson & Leon, 1989). Newborn (P0) and one-day-old (P1) pups were given odor-taste, classical conditioning and were tested for responsiveness to an empty surrogate nipple in the presence of the odor. All subjects were tested for responsiveness to the empty surrogate nipple only once – either 1 hour or 24 hours after conditioning. This procedure yielded a 2 (age) × 2 (retention interval) testing factorial and generated four possible testing groups: trained and tested on P0 (P0-1 hr), trained on P0 and tested on P1 (P0-24 hr), trained on P1 and tested on P1 (P1-1 hr), and trained on P1 and tested on P2 (P1-24 hr). After cesarean section, pups to be conditioned on P1 spent 4 hours in the incubator before being fostered, to match the P0-24hr group. To avoid confounding litter with treatment, no more than one male and one female per litter were assigned to the same conditioning treatment group, and all conditioning treatment groups were represented within a litter (Holson & Pearce, 1992). Number of males and females in each group were equated, and order of testing was counterbalanced. Videotaped records of the surrogate nipple test were scored (by an experimenter unaware of the subjects’ conditioning treatment; inter-experimenter reliability > 90%) for (a) latency to the first oral grasp response (calculated to the nearest 1.0 seconds), (b) total time attached to the nipple (sum of the duration of all grasps, to the nearest 1.0 seconds), (c) mean grasp duration (total time attached/number of grasps). Comparisons were made with between-group analysis of variance (ANOVA) supplemented by Tukey HSD (honestly significant differences) tests (for discussion see Howell, 2002, pp. 391 and 398–399). General procedures and equipment One hour after cesarean section, pups were voided and weighed to the nearest 0.01 grams (model BP410, Sartorius, Edgewood, New York). Pups less than 5.25 grams 584 Sarah J. Ferdinand Cheslock, Sarah K. Sanders and Norman E. Spear were not used as experimental subjects. Immediately after both conditioning and testing, each subject was again weighed. Conditioning experiments involved exposing the subject to lemon odor (lemon, Lorann Oils, Inc., Lansing, Michigan) and to 5 µL/1 second intraoral infusions of fluids (0.1% saccharin sodium or quinine sulfate, Fisher Scientific, Fair Lawn, New Jersey) in a variety of schedules, which are detailed within the method section of each experiment. Fluids were delivered through a tongue cannula, which was inserted midline of the lower jaw, with the flanged tip resting on the dorsal surface of the subject’s tongue – in a medial position, centered between anterior and posterior placement (Hall & Rosenblatt, 1977; Kehoe & Blass, 1985). The free end of the cannula was connected to PE-50 polyethylene tubing, which was connected to a Gilmont syringe in a rotary micro-syringe pump, controlled by an on/off switch (Kashinsky, Rozboril, Robinson & Smotherman, 1990). Lemon odor was injected into a cotton swab (0.1 cc), which was ‘waved’ about 2 centimeters above the subject’s head, during conditioning. For the test, the cotton tip was cut from the wand and mounted on the handle of the surrogate nipple with an alligator clip, about 1.5 cm from the pup’s nose. Several air cleaners and fans were used for odor clearance, making sure that no conditioning or testing surface was vibrated or in the path of air from a fan. During both conditioning and testing, the subject was swaddled snuggly in a vest made of soft, light-weight, quick-drying material, adjusted for the subject’s size with a clip (Heron & Spear, in preparation; Petrov, Varlinskaya & Spear, 2001). Once swaddled, the subject was placed in the conditioning or testing apparatus and clipped into a stationary holder in a lateral recumbent position, rotated partially supine, simulating the natural position of neonatal pups when suckling from the dam (Eilam & Smotherman, 1998). This vest calms the hyperactivity of the 1-day-old, allowing for presentation of the surrogate nipple. Conditioning took place on a sheet of Plexiglas maintained at 35.0 degrees (± 1 degree) Celsius by heating pads and illuminated by an overhead light. Testing occurred in a transparent glove box (63 × 50 × 25 cm). The ambient temperature of the glove box was maintained at 28 degrees (± 1 degree) Celsius by two heating pads placed inside the box. Two openings in the front wall of the glove box allowed access to the subject for presentation of the surrogate nipple. The subject was positioned on a small, round mirror, 7.5 centimeters in diameter, maintained at 35.0 degrees (± 0.5 degree) Celsius via a temperature controller (Model 40-90B; Frederick Haer Co., Brunswick, Maine). The subject was illuminated by a fiberoptic, cool light (Model LS-150; Lights by O’Ryan, Vancouver, Washington). The surrogate nipple was © Blackwell Publishing Ltd. 2004 carved from a block of soft vinyl, 25 millimeters in length and tapered to a 1-millimeter diameter at the rounded tip. The base of the nipple was attached to a dental probe in order to facilitate presentation by the experimenter. Perioral stimulation applied with the surrogate nipple was employed to promote attachment (Petrov, Varlinskaya & Smotherman; 1997; Smotherman, Goffman et al., 1997). General conditioning methods Handling of 1-day-olds was based on extensive pilot work by Heron & Spear (in preparation), who found that minimizing maternal separation was key in controlling variance. Minimal disturbance of the dam was also very important. To minimize handling of the dam, a white, plastic divider (which was placed over the open section of the cage top) was used to gently nudge the dam off the litter. The divider was then positioned between the dam and the litter, while the experimenter located the pup. Once the pup was located, the divider was removed, the cage top was replaced, and the divider was again placed over the part of the cage top that was not covered by food. Care was taken to avoid disturbing a foster dam (retrieving and returning pups for training and/or testing) no more than 6–8 times in total on a given day. Briefly, pups were retrieved from the dam and immediately tongue cannulated, then placed in the incubator for a 15-minute recovery before the beginning of conditioning. After conditioning, pups were returned to the incubator for 2 minutes before the tongue cannula was removed and they were returned to the dam for the retention interval. Before testing, pups were retrieved from the dam and immediately placed in the testing chamber. Newborn procedures matched 1-day-old procedures, except that P0-1hr pups spent the retention interval in the incubator with littermates. Pups tested 24 hours after newborn conditioning were fostered together after the last subject’s cannula was removed. The first newborn cannulation took place 2.5 hours after delivery, keeping conditioning and testing roughly between 3 and 6 hours after delivery, the optimum time window for nipple presentation (e.g. Smotherman, Goffman et al., 1997). Experiment 1: simple retention Experiment 1 compared newborn and 1-day-old memory for lemon odor–saccharin taste pairing(s) after both short (1-hour) and long (24-hour) intervals. As a general rule, the younger the animal (or human), the faster the forgetting (see Campbell & Coulter, 1976; Campbell Newborn resistance to retroactive interference & Spear, 1972; Hartshorn et al., 1998; Spear, Miller & Jagielo, 1990). However, based on the unusually robust memories that seemed to result from first-meal experiences for newborn and fetal rats (tolerance of trace intervals – Cheslock et al., 2000; Varlinskaya et al., 1997), memory for an odor-taste association was expected to be more robust to the challenge of a lengthy retention interval in the newborn (its first postnatal ‘meal’) than in the 1-day-old, which would be contrary to the general rule. Odor-taste conditioning in the newborn, as measured by change in responsiveness to an empty surrogate nipple scented with the odor, seems to require only a single trial; the behavioral consequences of one pairing did not differ from that of five massed pairings (Cheslock et al., 2000). However, as mentioned above, pilot data in the newborn suggested substantial forgetting 24 hours after a single pairing. (These pilot data were collected before the advent of the restraining vest. It is possible that the difference in 1-hour and 24-hour conditioned responding is something as uninteresting as ease of presentation of the surrogate nipple: the 1-day-old pup is much more active than the newborn, making it substantially more difficult for the experimenter to keep the tip of the surrogate nipple positioned in front of the pup’s mouth.) Similarly, pilot data collected in the 1-day-old (Heron & Spear, in preparation) suggested that one pairing may not be sufficient for expression of 1-day-old learning even after only 1 hour. (These pilot data were collected, however, before refinement of 1-day-old handling procedures.) One-day-olds showed strong conditioning after five pairings – and after refinement of handling procedures. Nevertheless, it was predicted that both newborns and 1-day-olds would show memory after the short and long retention intervals with some measure of forgetting after 24 hours. Increasing number of pairings and/or distribution of trials has been shown to help reduce the fast rate of forgetting that characterizes infancy (e.g. Coulter, 1979; Spear, 1979; Sussman & Ferguson, 1980). Therefore, to maximize the degree of conditioning and to find parameters that yield roughly equivalent conditioned responding across age and time, Experiment 1 included a onepairing group as well as a six-pairings group, which received three distributed trials of two pairings each. Based on previous experiments and some pilot data, it was expected that at the 1-hour test, newborn memory for one or six pairings would not differ; 1-day-old short-term retention (1 hour), however, was expected to be better when conditioned with six pairings. After 24 hours, it was expected that retention after six pairings might be superior in both the newborn and the 1-day-old. © Blackwell Publishing Ltd. 2004 585 Method A total of 144 cesarean-delivered neonatal rats, derived from 30 pregnancies, served as subjects in Experiment 1 (n = 12, per cell – age × conditioning treatment group × retention interval). During conditioning, subjects were presented the lemon odor (CS) either paired (one or six times) or explicitly unpaired (six times) with discrete, intraoral saccharin infusions (US). Conditioning in the C6 group occurred in three trials of two pairings each. All subjects received one saccharin infusion for every 30 seconds of odor exposure, and all subjects spent a total of 21 minutes on the conditioning surface. Pups in the UP6 group received the same amount of CS and US exposures presented to the C6 group. The UPS group served as the control group for both C1 and C6 in an effort to reduce total conditioning time. P0-P0 conditioning and testing had to be conducted between about 3 and 6 hours after delivery, creating a time constraint. Pups in the C1 and C6 groups were expected to express memory of the lemon-saccharin pairing(s) by increasing their attachment behavior toward the nipple compared to pups in the UP6 group. The conditioning parameters for each group were as follows: Pups in the C1 group were positioned on the conditioning surface (see General methods) and left undisturbed for 20.5 minutes. Lemon odor was presented between minutes 20.5 and 21, and a single 5 µL intraoral infusion of 0.1% saccharin was given at minute 21. Pups in the C6 group were undisturbed for 8 minutes. Between minutes 8 and 9, lemon odor was presented, and at 8.5 minutes and 9 minutes, discrete infusions of saccharin were given. Between minutes 14 and 15, and between minutes 20 and 21, the procedure was repeated, resulting in a total of six pairings. Pups in the UP6 group received intraoral infusions of saccharin at 1, 1.5, 2, 13.5, 14 and 14.5 minutes; they received lemon odor presentations between minutes 7 and 8.5 and between minutes 19.5 and 21. Either 1 hour or 24 hours after the completion of conditioning, pups were tested for responsiveness to an empty surrogate nipple in the presence of the lemon odor CS. See Figure 1 for a schematic of the conditioning protocols for Experiment 1. Because latency to the first grasp and mean grasp duration violated the assumption of homogeneity of variance, those measures were subjected to a log 10 transformation before statistical analysis. A total of five scores met the outlier criterion of greater than 2.5 standard deviations above or below the mean: one from log 10 (latency), two from log 10 (mean grasp duration) and two from total time attached (raw data). Because leaving outlying scores in the analysis did not influence the outcome of the ANOVAs for total time attached or for 586 Sarah J. Ferdinand Cheslock, Sarah K. Sanders and Norman E. Spear three-way analysis of variance (2 age × 2 gender × 3 treatment groups, ANOVA) showed no body weight differences. Analysis of variance for the UP6 groups alone showed no differences across age or retention intervals, for any measure. The lack of differences across age and retention interval in the UP6 groups suggests that unconditioned responsiveness to the empty surrogate nipple in the presence of lemon odor did not differ according to age. These UP results mirror the findings from Petrov et al. (2001) of no difference in unconditioned responding to an empty nipple between cesarean-delivered newborns and vaginally delivered 1- and 2-day-old pups. Figure 1 Schematic representation of the conditioning protocols for Experiment 1: simple forgetting Results mean grasp duration, statistics are reported with the outliers remaining in the dataset for those measures. Removing outliers did influence some results of ANOVAs for latency to the first grasp; where removal of outliers influenced results, both sets of statistical data are reported; otherwise, outliers remained in the analysis. For 120 of the 144 subjects, the experimenter testing the pup was unaware of the pup’s conditioning treatment. Individual one-way analyses of variance (ANOVA) showed no effect of whether the experimenter conducting the surrogate nipple test was aware of the conditioning treatment of the subject, for any measure, whether outliers were removed or not. Body weight on postnatal day 1 (P1) was compared between pups in the P0-24 hr group and pups in the P1-1 hr group to assess whether conditioning experience on postnatal day 0 (P0) altered fostering success as measured by P1 body weight. A Individual three-way analyses of variance (ANOVA, 3 conditioning treatment groups × 2 ages × 2 retention intervals) were conducted for log 10 (latency to the first grasp), total time attached and log 10 (mean grasp duration). There were no significant interactions for any dependent variable. As described in the paragraphs below, the results of Experiment 1 showed strong and equivalent conditioning in newborns and 1-day-olds, with no evidence of forgetting. Figure 2 presents total time attached, and Figure 3 presents log 10 (mean grasp duration). Because the experimenter presents the nipple to the pup, effects of latency to the first grasp (as a function of conditioning treatment) are not usually expected. In the present study, however, it was important to evaluate whether latency to grasp may have been related to age. If latency effects were sizeable, they could influence total time attached to the nipple. Analysis of log 10 (latency to the first grasp), however, revealed no effects of latency Figure 2 Newborn data are on the left and 1-day-old data are on the right. There was no effect of age on total time attached to the surrogate nipple. There was also no evidence of forgetting; 1-hour responsiveness did not differ from 24-hour responding. There was a sizeable effect of conditioning treatment group, with subjects in groups C1 and C6 not differing from each other but attaching much longer than the unpaired controls (U6). © Blackwell Publishing Ltd. 2004 Newborn resistance to retroactive interference 587 Figure 3 Newborn data are on the left and 1-day-old data are on the right. There were no effects of age on log 10 (mean grasp duration). Both ages displayed significant conditioning at both retention intervals. Mean grasp duration was greater in the paired groups (C1 and C6) than in the unpaired group (UP6). C1 and C6 did not differ from each other. Furthermore, mean grasp duration was greater 1 hour after conditioning than after 24 hours. in the present study. When the outlier was removed, the main effect of retention interval reached significance, with latency slightly longer after 1 hour than after 24 hours, F(1, 131) = 5.28, p < .03; mean/SEM: 1 hour = 2.07/.04; 24 hours = 1.94/.04. Although there was no effect of age at time of conditioning or an age × retention interval interaction, a quick look at the raw means suggests that the main effect of retention interval was driven by the P1-24 hr group (raw means/SEM: P0-1 hr = 138.64s/14.54; P0-24 hr = 130.50s/16.43; P1-1 hr = 152.61s/18.98; P1-24 hr = 97.31s/9.95). In other words, 2-day-olds tended to be slightly quicker in grasping than newborns or 1-day-olds. Differences between groups, however, were not significant and were not large enough to influence the results of total time attached. When testing newborn, 1- and 2-day-old pups for responsiveness to an empty surrogate nipple 1 hour after experience with an empty surrogate nipple, water, ethanol or saccharin, Petrov et al. (2001) found a similar main effect of age; newborns showed longer latencies than did one- and two-day-olds. Analysis of total time on the surrogate nipple showed strong and equivalent conditioned responding in both the newborns and the 1-day-olds and no evidence of forgetting across time. For total time attached there was a main effect of conditioning treatment group: F(2, 132) = 126.61, p < .001; subjects in the paired groups (C1 and C6) did not differ from each other and attached significantly longer than subjects in the unpaired treatment group (UP6). Figure 2 depicts the main effect of conditioning treatment and the lack of effects of age and retention interval for total time attached to the surrogate nipple. Mean grasp duration (total time attached/number of grasps) essentially revealed that newborns and 1-day© Blackwell Publishing Ltd. 2004 olds responded equivalently at both time intervals, with both ages displaying significant conditioning (relative to unpaired controls). Analysis of log 10 (mean grasp duration) revealed a main effect of retention interval, F(1, 132) = 7.74, p < .007, as well as main effect of conditioning treatment, F(2, 132) = 116.28, p < .001. Mean grasp duration was greater in the paired groups (C1 and C6) than in the unpaired group (UP6); C1 and C6 did not differ from each other. Furthermore, mean grasp duration was greater 1 hour after conditioning than after 24 hours, implying forgetting (Figure 3). There was no retention interval × conditioning treatment group interaction, however. As such, there was no evidence of forgetting. Furthermore, retention did not differ as a function of age; there was no main effect of age nor any interactions with age. Taken together, the results of Experiment 1 revealed equivalent conditioning and retention across time in newborns and 1-day-olds. Even with spaced conditioning and the considerable challenge of a long-retention interval, there was no evidence of an effect of number of CS–US pairings on retention. In view of the similar responsiveness to the surrogate nipple at test by newborns and 1-day-olds, the present procedures could then be used to compare resistance to conflicting memory treatments at these ages without confounding by strength of the initially acquired memory. Experiment 2: appetitive-aversive conflicting memories and retention interval To further test the tenacity of the first meal experience, Experiment 2 presented conflicting information in two 588 Sarah J. Ferdinand Cheslock, Sarah K. Sanders and Norman E. Spear different conditioning phases. In the first, an odor (CS) was paired with ingestion of a preferred flavor (saccharin), and in the second, the same odor was paired with an aversive flavor (quinine). Both 3-hour-old (newborns) and 1-day-old rats were given these conditioning phases and both were tested for retention after 1 hour or 24 hours. The same CS was employed in both conditioning phases, but the US to which the CS was paired was quite different, leaving the subject with two conflicting memories about the CS. If a memory acquired about the first meal is especially robust, newborns should be resistant to the influence of the second conditioning experience, even at the relatively short retention interval (1 hour), when retroactive interference normally dominates (e.g. Bouton & Peck, 1992; Spear, 1967, 1971). In other words, after 1 hour, newborns were expected to respond based on phase 1 with very little influence from phase 2, while responding in 1-day-olds was expected to be greatly influenced by phase 2. After a long interval (24 hours), when proactive interference normally dominates (e.g. Bouton & Peck, 1992; Spear, 1967, 1971), responding in both the newborns and the 1-day-olds was expected to be directed by phase 1 conditioning, with little influence from phase 2. Just as increasing the amount of training or practice usually increases simple retention, increasing the number of conflicting memory trials traditionally increases the degree of interference (see Slamecka & Ceraso, 1960; Spear, 1978). Typically, maximum retroactive interference is found when the number of conflicting memory trials (phase 2) somewhat exceeds original learning (phase 1). Dramatically increasing the number of conflicting memory trials, however, does not add to the effect of increased retroactive interference; the curve flattens (Slamecka & Ceraso, 1960). Therefore, to maximize the potential for retroactive interference at the short retention interval (1 hour), there was one pairing in phase 1, but four (in two trials of two pairings each) in phase 2. Method Cesarean-delivered, newborn and 1-day-old rats were assigned to one of four conditioning groups: (1) paired saccharin/paired quinine (PS/PQ; both phase 1 and phase 2 paired), (2) paired saccharin/unpaired quinine (PS/UPQ; phase 1 paired but phase 2 unpaired), (3) unpaired saccharin / paired quinine (UPS/PQ; phase 1 unpaired but phase 2 paired), or (4) unpaired saccharin/ unpaired quinine (UPS/UPQ; both phases unpaired). Subjects were also assigned to one of two testing groups: 1 hour (P0-1 hr and P1-1 hr) or 24 hours (P0-24 hr and P1-24 hr). Phase 1 was the ‘source of proactive interference,’ and phase 2 was the ‘source of retroactive interference.’ © Blackwell Publishing Ltd. 2004 Either 1 hour or 24 hours after conditioning treatment, subjects were tested for responsiveness to an empty surrogate nipple in the presence of lemon odor. One hundred and twenty-eight pups from 27 pregnancies served as experimental subjects in Experiment 2 (n = 8 per cell – age × conditioning group × retention interval). There was both a source of proactive and of retroactive interference in group PS/PQ; responding at test, relative to appropriate controls, indicated whether subjects in group PS/PQ were displaying retroactive interference, proactive interference or neither. PS/UPQ received no source of retroactive interference and served as the ‘retroactive interference’ control group. Because only phase 1 (appetitive) was paired in PS/UPQ, those subjects were expected to show strong conditioned responding to the surrogate nipple at test. If PS/PQ < PS/UPQ, retroactive interference would be indicated. UPS/PQ served as the ‘proactive interference’ control group. UPS/PQ subjects received no source of proactive interference as only the aversive phase was paired. Therefore, UPS/PQ subjects were expected to reject the surrogate nipple. If PS/PQ > UPS/PQ, proactive interference would be indicated. UPS/UPQ completed the factorial; because none of the stimuli were paired in this group, UPS/UPQ subjects were expected to respond to the nipple in the manner in which a pup given no conditioning experience would – a series of brief grasps followed by indifference (Petrov et al., 2001). Although UPS/UPQ was not necessary to test for retroactive and proactive interference, this group served to test for any unexpected changes in responsiveness to the empty surrogate nipple that might result from unpaired experiences with saccharin, quinine and lemon, possibly resulting from backward, trace or context conditioning. For all groups, conditioning treatment lasted 20 minutes. The conditioning protocol for the P group (the experimental group) was as follows: phase 1 = lemon odor exposure from minute 7.5 to minute 8 with a 5 µL, intraoral infusion of 0.1% saccharin at minute 8; phase 2 = lemon odor exposure from minute 13 to minute 14 and from minute 19 to minute 20 with intraoral infusions of 0.1% quinine at minute 13.5, 14, 19.5 and 20. PS/UPQ, UP/PQ and UPS/UPQ were variations of the PS/PQ group in which one or both of the phases were unpaired. PS/UPQ: phase 1 = lemon odor from minute 6 to minute 6.5 with intraoral saccharin infusion at minute 6.5; phase 2 = intraoral quinine infusions at minute 11.5, 12, 12.5 and 13 and lemon odor exposure from minute 18 to 20. UPS/PQ: phase 1 = intraoral saccharin infusion at 2.5 minutes and lemon odor exposure from 7.5 to 8 minutes; phase 2 = same as PS/PQ group. UPS/UPQ: phase 1 = intraoral saccharin infusion at minute 1 and lemon odor exposure from minute 6 to 6.5; Newborn resistance to retroactive interference Figure 4 Schematic representation of the conditioning protocol for Experiment 2: appetitive-aversive conflicting memories. phase 2 = same as PS/UPQ. Figure 4 provides a schematic of experimental design for Experiment 2. For all groups, the infusion pump direction was reversed 2 minutes after the phase 1 infusion to remove any saccharin remaining in the intraoral cannula. The tubing was then switched from the saccharin syringe to the quinine syringe in preparation for phase 2. Because latency to the first grasp and mean grasp duration violated the assumption of homogeneity of variance, those measures were subjected to a log 10 transformation before statistical analysis. Only one score (from log 10 [mean grasp duration]) met outlier criterion – greater than 2.5 standard deviations above or below the mean. Removal of that score did not alter the results of the ANOVA, so it was left in the dataset. Half of the subjects were tested without knowledge of the pup’s experimental treatment and the other half were tested with knowledge of the pup’s experimental treatment. This procedure allowed a thorough analysis of the effect of knowledge of the pup’s treatment group at testing. Independent, one-way ANOVAs revealed no effect of knowledge of conditioning treatment for any dependent measure. Results The results of Experiment 2 showed that at the short retention interval, newborns responded according to what they had learned in phase 1 as if phase 2 had never happened. One-day-olds, however, responded according to what they learned in phase 2 as if phase 1 never happened. After a long retention interval, both newborns and 1-day-olds were responding according to phase 1 © Blackwell Publishing Ltd. 2004 589 training (see Figures 5 and 6). These results supported our predictions: expression of newborn memory for the first meal was very resistant to the influence of subsequent experiences. One-day-old memory tested at the short retention interval after the same experience, however, was not resistant, but instead very responsive to the subsequent conflicting memory treatment. These results were verified by the following statistical analysis. Individual four-way ANOVA were conducted for log 10 (latency to the first grasp), total time attached and log 10 (mean grasp duration). There were 2 conditions for phase 1 (paired or unpaired) × 2 conditions for phase 2 (paired or unpaired) × 2 ages (P0 or P1) × 2 retention intervals (1 hour or 24 hours). There were no effects of latency to the first grasp. Analysis of total time attached, however, showed main effects of phase 1, phase 2, age and retention interval: F(1, 112) = 1150.89, p < .001; F(1, 112) = 17.83, p < .001; F(1, 112) = 6.48, p < .02; and F(1, 112) = 16.78, p < .001, respectively. Similarly, there were main effects of phase 1, phase 2 and age for mean grasp duration: F(1, 112) = 1068.99, p < .001; F(1, 112) = 25.14, p < .001; and F(1, 112) = 35.87, p < .001. For both total time attached and log 10 (mean grasp duration) there were also four-way-interactions of phase 1, phase 2, age and retention interval: F(1, 112) = 27.98, p < .001; F(1, 112) = 36.10, p < .001, respectively (see Figures 5 and 6). Total time attached and log 10 (mean grasp duration) showed the same pattern of results for the main effects of phase 1, phase 2 and age. Overall, when phase 1 (appetitive) was paired, subjects attached for much longer, and if phase 2 (aversive) was paired, subjects attached for slightly less time and vice versa. Newborns attached slightly longer than 1-day-olds. Furthermore, the main effect of retention interval for total time attached showed that attachment was slightly greater after 24 hours than after 1 hour. The effects of interest, however, were revealed by the four-way interactions of phase 1, phase 2, age and retention interval (see Figures 5 and 6). Newborns showed strong proactive interference and no evidence of retroactive interference at either retention interval. Both PS/PQ and PS/UPQ showed lengthy attachments at both the 1-hour and 24-hour retention intervals. They did not differ from each other or across time. Phase 2 had no measurable impact on responding. Furthermore, responsiveness in PS/PQ was significantly greater than responsiveness in UPS/PQ after both 1 hour and 24 hours, indicating substantial proactive interference after both the short and long-retention intervals. Phase 1 strongly determined responding as if Phase 2 had never occurred. The 1-day-old subjects, however, showed a much more conventional pattern of interference. Group PS/PQ showed 590 Sarah J. Ferdinand Cheslock, Sarah K. Sanders and Norman E. Spear Figure 5 The top panels show analyses of retroactive interference – PS/PQ compared to PS/UPQ (the retroactive interference control group). The bottom panels show analyses of proactive interference – PS/PQ compared to UPS/PQ (the proactive interference control group). Data for the short retention interval are on the left, long retention interval on the right. As measured by total time attached (seconds), newborns showed strong proactive interference at both the short and long retention intervals. One-day-olds showed strong retroactive interference at the short retention interval, but strong proactive interference at the long retention interval. substantial retroactive interference at the 1-hour test, but proactive interference at the 24-hour test. After 1 hour, but not 24 hours, responding in PS/UPQ was significantly greater than responding in PS/PQ, indicating retroactive interference at the short retention interval. For PS/PQ, experience with the aversive phase (phase 2) significantly reduced responsiveness to the surrogate nipple. In fact, phase 2 attenuated responding at the short retention interval so much that PS/PQ did not differ from UPS/PQ. After 24 hours, responding in PS/PQ was significantly greater than UPS/PQ and did not differ from PS/ UPQ, indicating proactive interference after the long retention interval. Taken together, these results show that learning about the first meal is © Blackwell Publishing Ltd. 2004 powerfully resistant to modification, lending support to the hypothesis that learning about the first meal is special. Of note is that UPS/PQ and UPS/UPQ never differed from each other or across time for any dependent measure; responding was consistently very low in both of these groups. To demonstrate the lack of differences between these two control groups, Figure 7 presents UPS /PQ and UPS/UPQ for log 10 (mean grasp duration) in both ages and at both retention intervals. Paired presentation of lemon and quinine in phase 2 was not capable of reducing responding to an empty surrogate nipple beyond that of a group which received unpaired presentation of those stimuli. The lack of differences found between UPS/PQ Newborn resistance to retroactive interference 591 Figure 6 The top panels show analyses of retroactive interference – PS/PQ compared to PS/UPQ (the retroactive interference control group). The bottom panels show analyses of proactive interference – PS/PQ compared to UPS/PQ (the proactive interference control group). Data for the short retention interval are on the left, long retention interval on the right. As measured by log 10 (mean grasp duration), newborns showed strong proactive interference at both the short and long retention intervals. One-day-olds showed strong retroactive interference at the short retention interval, but strong proactive interference at the long retention interval. and UPS/UPQ in the present study most likely resulted from a floor effect. In other words, because unconditioned responding to an empty surrogate nipple is very low, testing for responsiveness to the empty surrogate nipple was probably not a sensitive enough measure to show a conditioned reduction in responding. Testing on a more desirable nipple, such as a nipple providing milk, would likely have revealed a difference between UPS/PQ and UPS/UPQ, with the former showing reduced responsiveness compared to the later. Quinine is a very aversive stimulus for the neonate, capable of inducing strong aversive conditioning (Nizhnikov, Petrov, Varlinskaya & Spear, 2002). © Blackwell Publishing Ltd. 2004 Experiment 3: aversive-appetitive conflicting memories The present study expected that memory for the first meal experience would be robust against challenges to its expression. Experiment 1, however, found no measurable evidence that simple forgetting – the effect of a long retention interval – differed for a newborn’s memory of lemon-saccharin association (their first meal) and a 1day-old’s memory for the same association, contrary to the usual finding that younger infants forget more rapidly. Experiment 2, on the other hand, revealed dramatic differences in the effects of associative interference on 592 Sarah J. Ferdinand Cheslock, Sarah K. Sanders and Norman E. Spear Figure 7 Log 10 (mean grasp duration) for UPS / UPQ and UPS/PQ groups, showing that there was no significant difference between those two groups at either age (newborns on left, 1-day-olds on the right) or at either retention interval (1 hour or 24 hours; on the x-axis). newborns and 1-day-olds. It is possible that measures of simple forgetting are not sensitive enough to reveal differences between newborn and 1-day-old learning about odors predicting a taste. It is also possible that the special properties of newborn learning lie, at least in part, in its resistance to later modification and not in its durability to extra-experimental effects associated with time. In Experiment 2, pups were first presented an appetitive association to lemon odor (saccharin), then an aversive one (quinine), and then tested responding to the empty surrogate nipple in the presence of lemon after 1 hour or 24 hours. In most experiments testing conflicting memories, the most recent association (quinine) directs responding over the short term, but the original association (saccharin) regains control over responding after a lengthy interval (e.g. 24 hours; Bouton & Peck, 1992; Spear, 1967, 1971). The 1-day-olds responded with exactly that pattern of behavior. In the newborns, however, the first association (their first meal) strongly directed responding at both intervals, as if the subsequent conflicting memory had never been acquired. The purpose of Experiment 3 was to test whether newborn learning of an odor-taste association would still be similarly robust against the influence of a subsequent conflicting experience if the first experience was aversive (lemon-quinine) and the second pleasant (lemon-saccharin). One could argue that a particularly robust first odortaste experience that was appetitive is advantageous to the neonate’s survival, because acquiring milk from the dam is the newborn rats’ only means of nourishment. A robust memory of a negative odor-taste experience at the newborn’s first meal would, however, seem to be a disadvantage if it interfered drastically with the pup’s subsequent need to obtain nutrition. © Blackwell Publishing Ltd. 2004 Method The conditioning and testing protocols for Experiment 3 were similar to those used in Experiment 2. As in Experiment 2, there were two conflicting phases of conditioning in Experiment 3. Opposite of Experiment 2, however, phase 1 was aversive (lemon odor-quinine taste) and phase 2 was appetitive (lemon odor-saccharin taste). Cesarean-delivered newborn and 1-day-old rats were assigned to one of three conditioning treatments: paired quinine/paired saccharin (PQ/PS), unpaired quinine/ paired saccharin (UPQ/PS), or paired quinine/unpaired saccharin (PQ/UPS). UPQ/PS subjects did not receive a source of proactive interference as only phase 2 (the appetitive phase) was paired. Therefore UPQ/PS subjects were expected to show sustained attachment to the nipple at test and served as the proactive interference control group. If PQ/PS < UPQ/PS, proactive interference would be indicated. PQ/UPS served as the retroactive interference control group. PQ/UPS did not receive a source of retroactive interference as only phase 1 (the aversive phase) was paired. Accordingly, PQ/UPS subjects were not expected to show sustained attachment at test. If PQ/PS > PQ/UPS, retroactive interference would be indicated. The group given both phases unpaired (UPS /UPQ) in Experiment 2 was not included in Experiment 3 (the equivalent would have been UPQ/UPS). In Experiment 2, UPS/UPQ did not differ from UPS/PQ, indicating that the empty surrogate nipple test is not sensitive enough to show reduced responding in a group given paired experience with lemon odor and quinine taste, compared with a group given experience with quinine unpaired with lemon. Thirty-six cesarean-delivered rat pups, derived from seven pregnancies, served as Newborn resistance to retroactive interference experimental subjects in Experiment 3 (n = 6 per cell; age × conditioning treatment). All subjects remained in the conditioning apparatus for a total of 20 minutes. For pups in the PQ/PS group, phase 1 consisted of exposure to lemon odor from minute 7.5 to 8, with a 5 µL infusion of 0.1% quinine administered at minute 8. Phase 2 consisted of lemon odor exposure between minute 13 and 14 and again between minute 19 and 20, with intraoral infusions of 0.1% saccharin occurring at minute 13.5, 14, 19.5 and 20. For UPQ /PS, phase 1 consisted of an intraoral quinine infusion at 2.5 minutes and lemon odor exposure from 7.5 minutes to 8 minutes; phase 2 was identical to that in the PQ/PS group. For PQ/UPS, phase 1 consisted of lemon odor exposure from 6 minutes to 6.5 minutes, with an infusion of quinine at 6.5 minutes; phase 2 consisted of saccharin infusions at 11.5, 12, 12.5, and 13 minutes and lemon odor exposure between 18 and 20 minutes. There were no effects of age at the 24-hour test in Experiment 2; both newborns and 1-day-olds showed strong proactive interference. Therefore, pups in Experiment 3 were tested for responsiveness to an empty surrogate nipple in the presence of the lemon odor CS only after 1 hour, to assess whether the differential responsiveness at the short retention interval found in Experiment 2 (proactive interference in newborns but retroactive interference in 1-day-olds) would hold when the aversive phase was first. Figure 8 provides a schematic of the conditioning protocols for Experiment 3. Latency to the first grasp and mean grasp duration violated the assumption of homogeneity of variance. Therefore, those measures were subjected to a log 10 593 Figure 8 Schematic representation of conditioning protocols for Experiment 3: aversive-appetitive conflicting memories. transformation before statistical analysis. No scores met criterion for outliers. Results As in Experiment 2, analysis of total time attached and log 10 (mean grasp duration) both revealed substantial retroactive interference in 1-day-olds and proactive interference in newborns (Figures 9 and 10, respectively). There were no effects of latency to the first grasp. The results generally mirrored those of Experiment 2 in revealing that the newborn’s first meal was represented by an especially robust memory that was more resistant Figure 9 The analysis of retroactive interference is shown on the left (PQ/PS compared to the retroactive interference control group, PQ/UPS). The analysis of proactive interference is shown on the right (PQ/PS compared to the proactive interference control group, UPQ/PS). As measured by total time attached (seconds) newborns showed strong proactive interference, while 1-day-olds showed strong retroactive interference. © Blackwell Publishing Ltd. 2004 594 Sarah J. Ferdinand Cheslock, Sarah K. Sanders and Norman E. Spear Figure 10 The analysis of retroactive interference is shown on the left (PQ/PS compared to the retroactive interference control group, PQ / UPS). The analysis of proactive interference is shown on the right (PQ/PS compared to the proactive interference control group, UPQ / PS). As measured by log 10 (mean grasp duration) newborns showed strong proactive interference, while 1-day-olds showed strong retroactive interference. to retroactive interference than a similar memory acquired by 1-day-old pups. For both total time attached and log 10 (mean grasp duration) there were main effects of conditioning treatment and age as well as an interaction of the two (a) total time attached: F(2, 30) = 227.02, p < .001; F(1, 30) = 129.40, p < .001; and F(2, 30) = 85.81, p < .001, respectively, (b) mean grasp duration: F(2, 30) = 318.39, p < .001; F(1, 30) = 78.13, p < .001; and F(2, 30) = 97.48, p < .001, respectively (Figures 9 and 10). The pattern of effects was the same for total time attached and for mean grasp duration. Overall, 1-day-olds spent more time attached and had longer mean grasp duration than did newborns. Subjects in the UPQ/PS group spent more time attached and had longer mean grasp duration than subjects in the PQ/PS group. In turn, PQ/PS subjects spent more time attached and had longer mean grasp duration than did PQ/UPS subjects. PS/UPQ > PQ/PS > PQ/UPS. The two-way interactions showed strong proactive interference in newborns and strong retroactive interference in the 1-day-olds. For the newborns, UPQ/PS spent significantly longer time attached and longer mean grasp duration than did subjects in both PQ/PS and PQ /UPS, which did not differ from each other. The measures used, in other words, detected absolutely no effect of the retroactively conflicting memory treatment. This contrasts strikingly with effects on the 1-day-olds. At this age, subjects in PQ/PS and subjects in the UPQ/ PS did not differ from each other and showed significantly longer time attached/longer mean grasp duration than did PQ/UPS subjects. For the 1-day-old, in short, experience with a retroactively conflicting memory treatment influenced subsequent responding so strongly © Blackwell Publishing Ltd. 2004 that original learning experience was completely undetectable. General discussion Experiment 1 of the present study compared newborn learning and memory for an odor-taste association after a short (1-hour) and a long (24-hour) retention interval. The prediction was that if there is something special about memory for the first postnatal meal, newborn memory for lemon–saccharin association(s) might be more durable than 1-day-old memory for the same experience. Overall, the results of Experiment 1 showed strong simple retention in both newborns and 1-dayolds, even after 24 hours, which is impressive given the rapid forgetting that normally characterizes infancy (e.g. Campbell & Coulter, 1976; Campbell & Spear, 1972; Hartshorn et al., 1998; Spear, Miller & Jagielo, 1990). There was no effect of age on retention – not even the expected interaction between age and retention interval – and, in fact, no evidence of forgetting. The magnitude of conditioned responding after both intervals was substantial. For example, after 1 hour, paired subjects (collapsed across age) showed an average of more than 7 minutes (in a 10-minute test) attached to the nipple in the presence of lemon odor, with unpaired controls showing less than 2.5 minutes attached. Experiment 2 revealed the special nature of the newborn pup’s memory for its first postnatal meal by introducing conflicting memories. The first learning experience in Experiment 2, phase 1, was appetitive (saccharin), and the second, phase 2, was aversive (quinine). It was predicted that newborn learning would be highly resistant Newborn resistance to retroactive interference to the influence of a subsequently acquired memory. The 1-day-olds were expected to respond in the more conventional manner, responding at test based on the last thing learned after a short retention interval and responding at test based on the first thing learned after a long interval (e.g. Bouton & Peck, 1992; Spear, 1971). The results of Experiment 2 supported these predictions: after a short retention interval, 1-day-old rats were dramatically influenced by phase 2 experience, responding as if phase 1 had never happened, whereas newborns responded as if phase 2 had never happened. The first learning experience, for the newborn, seems to overpower the influence of a subsequent conflicting experience. After 24 hours, both the newborns and the 1-day-olds were responding in accord with the phase 1 experience. Because the 1-day-olds responded according to phase 2 after 1 hour and phase 1 after 24 hours, it is clear that 1-day-olds learned about both phases. Newborns, however, responded according to the first phase after both 1 hour and 24 hours, never showing any influence of the second learning experience. Therefore, it is possible that the newborns never learned about phase 2. One can interpret the results as a failure of retrieval at test based on interference from the first phase of conditioning (proactive interference) or as a failure of original learning – they never learned phase 2. It is clear that newborns are capable of forming very strong memories about lemon odor paired with quinine taste, as was presented in phase 2 of Experiment 2 (Nizhnikov, Petrov & Spear, 2002; Experiment 3 of present study). Newborns are also quite capable of learning a second phase of conditioning shortly after a first: newborns showed both strong sensory preconditioning and strong second-order conditioning at a 1 hour test (Cheslock, Varlinskaya, High & Spear, 2003). Sensory preconditioning and second order conditioning involve two phases of conditioning; in the above mentioned study, one phase involved the temporal pairing of two odors (lemon and banana) and the other phase involved the temporal pairing of one of the odors with an intraoral infusion of milk. We are inclined, therefore, to interpret the present results as a failure of expression of phase 2 memory as opposed to a failure of learning. Regardless, experience with the first phase was strong enough to override (learning of, or expression of) the second phase. The purpose of Experiment 3 was to confirm whether the age-related dichotomy found after short retention intervals in Experiment 2 (proactive interference in newborns but retroactive interference in 1-day-olds) would also apply if the first experience was negative and the second was positive, the reverse of Experiment 2. In Experiment 3, the CS in phase 1 was paired with an aversive substance (quinine), and in phase 2 the same CS © Blackwell Publishing Ltd. 2004 595 was paired with an appetitive substance (saccharin). One-day-olds again responded after the short interval in the fashion typical for older rats, with strong retroactive interference, but newborns showed strong proactive interference. One-day-olds, in other words, behaved according to what they learned in phase 2, as one would predict from the propensity of older rats to respond based on the last thing learned after a short retention interval (e.g. Bouton & Peck, 1992). Bucking convention again, the newborns responded according to what they learned in phase 1, completely ignoring phase 2. In other words, if the first learning experience was negative, the newborn rejected the nipple, despite subsequent positive experiences. Because in the paired groups of Experiments 2 and 3 of the present study lemon odor was paired with both a positive (saccharin) and a negative (quinine) consequence, one might expect behavior toward an empty surrogate nipple in the presence of lemon odor to be somewhat conflicted, in the classic approach–avoidance sense (e.g. Dollard et al., 1939; Dollard & Miller, 1950; Gentry & Dunlap, 1942; Hearst, 1963, 1967). Pups in that group, one might predict, would approach the nipple over and over, grasping and disengaging, conflicted between a motivation to accept the nipple based on the positive pairing and to reject the nipple based on the negative pairing. There was no evidence of conflict from the measures applied in Experiments 2 and 3, however. The newborns in the experimental group (paired), which received conflicting information, were so unaffected by the second (conflicting) phase of conditioning that they never differed from the control group that did not receive the second phase. Conversely, the 1-day-old experimental subjects were so responsive to the second phase that they did not differ from the group that never received the first phase of conditioning. Pups in all groups were very clear about their responsiveness toward the nipple; pups that accepted the nipple did so without much vacillation, and pups that rejected the nipple showed very unstable and ‘agitated’ testing behavior. Pups in the group given only the appetitive conditioning phase (aversive phase unpaired) always showed very few grasps and a long total time attached. The group that only received the aversive conditioning phase (appetitive phase unpaired) showed several grasps and a short time attached. The paired pups in Experiments 2 and 3, then, seem to be less ‘conflicted’ than expected and more absolute in their responsiveness, either to phase 1 or to phase 2. Interference effects are often greater in younger animals or humans (e.g. Kail, 2002; Smith & Spear, 1981; Spear, Gordon & Chiszar, 1972). One might predict, then, that expression of the original CS–US association would be more likely to be altered by interference techniques in the newborn than in the 1-day-old. However, as the 596 Sarah J. Ferdinand Cheslock, Sarah K. Sanders and Norman E. Spear newborn’s first (or one of the very first) explicit learning experiences as well as its first meal, newborn memory for the original odor-taste association was highly resistant to modulation by a subsequent, conflicting learning experience. The first learning experience, whether appetitive or aversive, directed the newborns’ responsiveness toward the nipple. The 1-day-old, already experienced with odor-food pairings occurring in the nest, responded to the conflicting memory treatment in the conventional fashion of older animals – displaying retroactive interference at the short retention interval and proactive interference at the long retention interval. Why learning about the first meal may be special is uncertain. It is possible, and perhaps likely, that newborn rats (in this case, born by cesarean section and housed with littermates in an incubator) have already begun to accumulate postnatal associations (e.g. between the smell of the dam’s fur and the taste of amniotic fluid just after delivery). Nevertheless, cesarean-delivered newborn rats have an extremely limited repertoire of postnatal experiences, and as such, are in a state of virtual primacy for postnatal learning. Primacy, the first event in a given context, has traditionally been associated with promotion of memory strength (e.g. Kennet, McGuire, Willis & Schaie, 2000; Wright, 1994). Primacy, then, may have contributed to the remarkable robustness of newborn and fetal conditioning against the challenge of trace intervals. The results of the present study, showing that memory for an odor-taste association differs so dramatically between newborns (naïve to suckling experience) and relatively sophisticated 1-day-olds, encourages a direct test of the primacy hypothesis, perhaps comparing naïve newborns to experienced newborns. We very much hope to conduct such an investigation in the future. Given the importance of odor signals in guiding the newborn rat to the mother’s nipple (Teicher & Blass, 1977), odor-taste pairings may enjoy a certain level of ‘biological preparedness’ (Garcia & Koelling, 1966; Seligman, 1971) in the neonate. The newborn may be specially prepared to learn odor-taste associations, similar to the way an adult is prepared to learn an association between taste and illness. It is also possible that newborn learning may be special because of the physiological consequences of birth, including a surge of norepinephrine (e.g. Kudo, 1989; Lagercrantz & Slotkin, 1986), a hormone that has been associated with olfactory learning in infant rats (e.g. Sullivan, McGaugh & Leon, 1991; Sullivan, Wilson & Leon, 1989). Acknowledgements The research presented in this article was supported by grants from the National Institute of Mental Health © Blackwell Publishing Ltd. 2004 (R01MH35219) and the National Institute of Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism (RO1AA13098). We express our appreciation to Teri Tanenhaus for assistance with the manuscript and to Wayne Kashinsky and Lloyd Rozboril for equipment design and manufacture. References Alberts, J.R., & Gubernick, D.J. (1984). Early learning as ontogenetic adaptation for ingestion by rats. Learning and Motivation, 15, 334–359. Arnold, H.M., & Spear, N.E. (1997). Infantile amnesia: using animal models to understand forgetting. In P.J.B. Slater, J.S. Rosenblatt, C.T. Snowden & M. Milinski (Eds.), Advances in the study of behavior, Vol. 26 (pp. 251–284). New York: Academic Press. Blass, E.M. (1990). Suckling: determinants, changes, mechanisms, and lasting impressions. Developmental Psychobiology, 26, 520–533. Blass, E.M., & Teicher, M.H. (1980). Suckling. Science, 210, 15–22. Bouton, M.E., & Peck, C.A. (1992). Spontaneous recovery in cross-motivational transfer (counterconditioning). Animal Learning and Behavior, 20, 313–321. Campbell, B.A. (1967). Developmental studies of learning motivation in infraprimate mammals. In. H.W. Stevenson, E.H. Hess & H.L. Rheingold (Eds.), Early behavior: Comparative and developmental approaches (pp. 43–72). New York: Wiley. Campbell, B.A., & Coulter, X. (1976). Ontogeny of Learning and Memory. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Campbell, B.A., & Spear, N.E. (1972). Ontogeny of memory. Psychological Review, 79, 215–236. Cheslock, S.J., Valirnskaya, E.I., High, J.M., & Spear, N.E. (2003). Higher order conditioning in the newborn rat: effects of temporal disparity imply infantile encoding of simultaneous events. Infancy, 4, 157–176. Cheslock, S.J., Varlinskaya, E.I., Petrov, E.S., & Spear, N.E. (2000). Rapid and robust olfactory conditioning with milk before suckling experience: promotion of nipple attachment in the newborn rat. Behavioral Neuroscience, 114, 484–495. Coulter, X. (1979). The determinants of infantile amnesia. In N.E. Spear & B.A. Campbell (Eds.), The ontogeny of learning and memory. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Dollard, J., & Miller, N.E. (1950). Personality and psychotherapy: An analysis in terms of learning, thinking, and culture. New York: McGraw-Hill. Dollard, J., Doob, L.W., Miller, N.E., Mowrer, O.H., Sears, R.R., Ford, C.S., Hovland, C.I., & Sollenberger, R.T. (1939). Frustration and aggression. Oxford: Yale University Press. Dudycha, G.J., & Dudycha, M.A. (1933). Some factors and characteristics of childhood memories. Child Development, 4, 265–278. Gentry, E., & Dunlap, K. (1942). An attempt to produce neurotic behavior in rats. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 33, 107–112. Newborn resistance to retroactive interference Eilam, D., & Smotherman, W.P. (1998). How the neonatal rat gets to the nipple: common motor modules and their involvement in the expression of early motor behavior. Developmental Psychobiology, 32, 57 – 66. Garcia, J., & Koelling, R.A. (1966). Relation of cue to consequence in avoidance learning. Psychonomic Science, 4, 123 –124. Grota, L.J. (1968a). Effects of parity on acceptance of cesareandelivered offspring by foster mothers. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 66, 545 – 546. Grota, L.J. (1968b). Factors influencing the acceptance of cesarean delivered offspring by foster mothers. Physiology and Behavior, 3, 265 – 269. Hall, W.G., & Rosenblatt, J.S. (1977). Suckling behavior and intake control in the developing rat pup. Journal of Comparative Physiological Psychology, 91, 1232 – 1247. Hartshorn, K., Rovee-Collier, C., Gerhardstein, P., Bhatt, R.S., Wondoloski, T.L., Klein, P., Gilch, J., Wurtzel, N., & Campos-de-Carvalho, M. (1998). The ontogeny of long-term memory over the first year-and-a-half of life. Developmental Psychobiology, 32, 69 – 89. Hearst, E. (1963). Escape from a stimulus associated with both reward and punishment. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 56, 1027 – 1031. Hearst, E. (1967). Oscillatory behavior during approachavoidance conflict. Journal of Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 10, 75 – 84. Heron, J., & Spear, N.E. (in preparation). Higher order conditioning in the 1-day-old rat: sensory preconditioning and acquired equivalence. Holson, R.R., & Pearce, B. (1992). Principals and pitfalls in the analysis of prenatal treatment effects in mulitparous species. Neurotoxicity and Teratology, 14, 221 – 228. Howell, D.C. (2002). Statistical methods for psychology (5th edition). Pacific Grove, CA: Duxbury. Johanson, I.B., & Hall, W.G. (1979). Appetitive learning in one-day-old rat pups. Science, 205, 419 – 421. Johanson, I.B., & Hall, W.G. (1982). Appetitive conditioning in neonatal rats: conditioned orientation to an odor. Developmental Psychobiology, 15, 379 – 397. Johanson, I.B., & Teicher, M.H. (1980). Classical conditioning of an odor preference in 3-day-old rats. Behavioral and Neural Biology, 29, 132 – 136. Johanson, I.B., & Terry, M.H. (1988). Learning in infancy: a mechanism for behavioral change during development. In E.M. Blass (Ed.), Handbook of behavioral neurobiology: Developmental and behavioral ecology, Vol. 9 (pp. 245–282). New York: Plenum Press. Johanson, I.B., Hall, W.G., & Polefrone, J.M. (1984). Appetitive conditioning in neonatal rats: conditioned ingestive responding to stimuli paired with oral infusions of milk. Developmental Psychobiology, 17, 357 – 381. Kail, R. (2002). Developmental change in proactive interference. Child development, 73, 1703 –1714. Kashinksy, W.M., Rozboril, L.W., Robinson, S.R., & Smotherman, W.P. (1990). An inexpensive rotary infusion pump for delivering microliter volumes of fluids to animal subjects. Physiology and Behavior, 47, 1279 – 1281. Kehoe, P., & Blass, E.M. (1985). Gustatory determinants © Blackwell Publishing Ltd. 2004 597 of suckling in albino rats 5–20 days of age. Developmental Psychobiology, 18, 67–82. Kennet, J., McGuire, L., Willis, S.vL., & Schaie, K.vW. (2000). Memorability functions in verbal memory: a longitudinal approach. Experimental Aging Research, 26, 121–137. Kudo, T. (1989). The role of fetal catecholamines before and during birth. Nippon Sanka Fujinka Gakkai Zasshi, 41, 1027–1032. Lagercrantz, H., & Slotkin, T.A. (1986). The ‘stress’ of being born. Scientific American, 254, 100–107. National Institutes of Health. (1986). Guide for the care and use of laboratory animals (DHEW Publication No. 86–23). Washington, D.C.: U.S. Government Printing Office. Nizhnikov, M.E., Petrov, E.S., & Spear, N.E. (2002). Olfactory aversive conditioning in the newborn (3-hour-old) rat impairs later suckling for water and milk. Journal of Experimental Psycholology: Animal Behavior Processes, 28, 277–283. Nizhnikov, M.E., Petrov, E.S., Varlinskaya, E.I., & Spear, N.E. (2002). Newborn rats’ first suckling experience: taste differentiation and suckling plasticity. Physiology and Behavior, 76, 181–98. Oppenheim, R.W. (1981). Ontogenetic adaptations and retrogressive processes in the development of the nervous system and behaviour: a neuroembryological perspective. In. K.J. Connolly, & H.F.R. Prechtl (Eds.), Maturation and development: Biological and psychological perspectives (pp. 73–109). Spastics International Medical Publications. Petrov, E.S., Varlinskaya, E.I., & Smotherman, W.P. (1997). The newborn rat ingests fluids through a surrogate nipple: a new technique for the early study of suckling behavior. Physiology and Behavior, 62, 1155–1158. Petrov, E.S., Varlinskaya, E.I., & Spear, N.E. (2001). Selfadministration of ethanol and saccharin in newborn rats: effects on suckling plasticity. Behavioral Neuroscience, 115, 1318–1331. Pillemer, D.B., & White, S.H. (1989). Childhood events recalled by children and adults. In H.W. Reese (Ed.), Advances in child development and behavior, Vol. 21 (pp. 297–340). Orlando: Academic Press. Seligman, M.E. (1971). Phobias and preparedness. Behavior Therapy, 2, 307–320. Singh, P.J., & Hofer, M.A. (1978). Oxytocin reinstates maternal olfactory clues for nipple orientation and attachment in rat pups. Physiology and Behavior, 20, 385–389. Slamecka, N.J., & Ceraso. J. (1960). Retroactive and proactive inhibition of verbal learning. Psychological Bulletin, 57, 449–475. Smith, G.J., & Spear, N.E. (1981). Role of proactive interference in infantile forgetting. Animal Learning and Behavior, 9, 371–380. Smotherman, W.P., Goffman, D., Petrov, E.S., & Varlinskaya, E.I. (1997). Oral grasping of a surrogate nipple by the newborn rat. Developmental Psychobiology, 31, 3–17. Smotherman, W.P., Petrov, E.S., & Varlinskaya, E.I. (1997). Experimental study of the first suckling episode: rat pups ingest fluids through a surrogate nipple. Behavioral Neuroscience, 111, 1383–1394. Spear, N.E. (1967). Retention of reinforcer magnitude. Psychological Review, 74, 216–234. Spear, N.E. (1971). Forgetting as retrieval failure. In 598 Sarah J. Ferdinand Cheslock, Sarah K. Sanders and Norman E. Spear W.K. Honig and P.H.R. James (Eds.), Animal memory. New York: Academic Press. Spear, N.E. (1978). The Processing of memories: Forgetting and retention. Hilldale, NJ: Erlbaum. Spear, N.E. (1979). Memory storage factors in infantile amnesia. In G. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation, Vol. 13 (pp. 91 – 154). New York: Academic Press. Spear, N.E. (1984). Ecologically determined dispositions control the ontogeny of learning and memory. In. R. Kail & N.E. Spear (Eds.), Comparative perspectives on the development of memory (pp. 325 – 358). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Spear, N.E., & Riccio, D.C. (1994). Memory Phenomena and Principles. Boston: Allyn and Bacon. Spear, N.E., & Rudy, J.W. (1991). Tests of the ontogeny of learning and memory: issues, methods, and results. In H.N. Shair, G.A. Barr & M.A. Hofer (Eds.), Developmental psychobiology: New methods and changing concepts. New York: Oxford University Press. Spear, N.E., Gordon, W.C., & Chiszar, D.A. (1972). Interactions between memories in the rat: effect of degree of prior conflicting learning on forgetting after short intervals. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 78, 471– 477. Spear, N.E., Miller, J.S., & Jagielo, J.A. (1990). Animal memory and learning. Annual Review of Psychology, 41, 169 – 211. Sullivan, R.M., & Hall, W.G. (1988). Reinforcers in infancy: classical conditioning using stroking or intra-oral infusions of milk as the UCS. Developmental Psychobiology, 21, 215–223. Sullivan, R.M., McGaugh, J.L., & Leon, M. (1991). Norepinephrineinduced plasticity and one-trial olfactory learning in neonatal rats. Developmental Brain Research, 60, 219 – 228. © Blackwell Publishing Ltd. 2004 Sullivan, R.M., Wilson, D.A., & Leon, M. (1989). Norepinephrine and learning-induced plasticity in infant rat olfactory system. Journal of Neuroscience, 9, 3998–4006. Sussman, P.S., & Ferguson, H.B. (1980). Retained elements of early avoidance training and relearning of forgotten operants. Developmental Psychobiology, 13, 545–562. Teicher, M.H., & Blass, E.M. (1976). Suckling in newborn rats: eliminated by nipple lavage, reinstated by pup saliva. Science, 193, 422–424. Teicher, M.H., & Blass, E.M. (1977). First suckling response of the newborn albino rat. Roles of olfaction and amniotic fluid. Science, 198, 635–636. Usher, J.A., & Neisser, U. (1993). Childhood amnesia and the beginnings of memory for four early life events. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 122, 155–165. Varlinskaya, E.I., Petrov, E.S., Simonik, D.K., & Smotherman, W.P. (1997). Classical conditioning in the fetal rats with a long delay between presentations of the CS and US. Developmental Psychobiology, 30, 49–59. Vyas, H., Milner, A.D., Hopkin, I.E., & Falconer, A.D. (1983). Role of labour in the establishment of functional residual capacity at birth. Archives of Diseases of Childhood, 58, 512–517. West, M.J., & King, A.P. (1987). Settling nature and nurture into an ontogenetic niche. Developmental Psychobiology, 20, 549–562. Wright, A.A. (1994). Primacy effects in animal memory and human nonverbal memory. Animal Learning and Behavior, 22, 219–223. Received: 6 November 2003 Accepted: 29 January 2004
© Copyright 2025 Paperzz