SOCIAL POLICY REPORT Society for Research in Child Development Volume XI, Number 1 1997 Schooling, the Hidden Curriculum, and Children’s Conceptions of Poverty Judith A. Chafel T his Social Policy Report synthesizes several streams of research bearing on the problem of poverty in the United States today. Four themes are developed: (1) American society is characterized by rising income inequality; (2) our schools provide an arena for reproducing, resisting, and reconstructing an inequitable economic and social order; (3) a hidden (and not so hidden) curriculum transmits dominant ideological messages about society; and (4) children increasingly with age ascribe to societal beliefs that perceive the poor as responsible for their condition. These conceptions relate the cause of poverty to individual deficiencies (that is, they “blame the victim”), downplay other more important factors, and as a result hamper the design of antipoverty policy. A number of implications drawn from the discussion pertain to the conduct and funding of research on (1) the messages conveyed to children by schools; and (2) children’s beliefs about poverty derived from other sources. The various streams of research reviewed here are merely suggestive inasmuch as they represent rather disparate, loosely related bodies of work. The purpose of the discussion is to point out the need for further research to establish their connections. Socialization occurs via many path- ways. Although none of the studies reviewed examines the source of children’s ideas about economic privation in our society, it is not unreasonable to propose that schools play a crucial role. Background C H I L D A N D F A M I LY P O V E RT Y Government reports substantiate the large number of the U.S. population in poverty. According to the Census Bureau, 36.4 million individuals, or 13.8% of all Americans, were poor in 1995 (Weinberg, 1996a [see Figure 1, page 2]). A household is identified as “poor” if its yearly pre-tax cash income drops below a Also in This Issue ... 19 Presentations by SRCD members to the media at the biennial meeting in Washington, DC. ©1997 Society for Research in Child Development. All rights reserved. some segments of the population (blacks, the elderly [Holmes, 1996; Weinberg, 1996a]), analy45 ses by Zedlewski, Clark, Meier, 40 and Watson (1996) of the Urban 36.4 35 million Institute suggest the decrease 30 Number in poverty 32.4 million may be short-lived due to recent 25 welfare reform. The new law 20 mandates work and strict time Poverty rate 15 13.8% limits for welfare eligibility and 10 13.1% other income security programs. 5 Because it earmarks many of the 0 assistance programs for families 1959 1964 1970 1976 1982 1988 1995 with children, the economic Recessionary periods Source: U.S. Bureau of the Census, March Current Population Survey well-being of that category of persons is likely to be affected threshold set by the federal government to vary most. More children are expected to fall into by family size and composition: to cite two poverty, intensifying an already substantial child examples from 1995 figures, $12,158 for a fampoverty rate of 20.8% (in 1995), the highest of ily of three, $15,569 for a family of four any age group (Weinberg, 1996a; Zedlewski et (Weinberg, 1996a). al., 1996). Representing 27% of the total popuWhile the proportion of people falling lation, children comprise 40% of the poor below the poverty line has recently declined (Weinberg, 1996a). (from 14.5% in 1994), reaching historic lows for Fig. 1 Poverty: 1959–1995 Fig. 2 Share of Household Income: 1967–1995 Percent 60 Lowest 20% 60 50 50 40 40 30 30 20 20 10 10 0 1967 Highest 20% 60 * 1967 1995 60 50 50 40 40 30 30 20 20 10 10 0 1967 * Middle 60% 0 1967 1995 * 1995 * 1995 Highest 5% * Introduction of computer-assisted personal interviewing (CAPI) and increased reporting limits for selected sources of income of 1993 affect comparability. Source: U.S. Bureau of the Census, March Current Population Survey 2 income quintile (from 43.8% to 48.7%); and a declining share by the lowest (from 4.0% to 3.7%) and middle 60% (from 52.3% to 47.6% [Weinberg, 1996a]). Between 1994 and 1995, the bottom quintile gained in income distribution by 0.1%, a statistically significant change. No significant changes appeared for any of the other quintiles (“Money income,” 1997; Weinberg, 1996a). Figure 3 shows the share of aggregate household income by quintiles for three time periods: 1975, 1985, and 1995 (“Money income,” 1997), highlighting a long-term trend toward greater income inequality. Although the causes are not completely understood, a number of factors are cited as responsible for the rise in income disparity: I N E Q U A L I T Y: T H E G A P B E T W E E N R I C H AND POOR Historical data compiled by the U.S. Census Bureau reveal a long-term trend toward increased income inequality for more than the last quarter century. From 1947 to 1968, family income inequality (measured by the Gini index) declined by 7.4%, but then rose 22.4% during the years 1968 to 1994 [Weinberg, 1996b]). Between 1994 and 1995, overall income inequality did not change (“Money income,” 1997). Figure 2 presents a number of historical patterns in family income inequality (measured by shares of aggregate income) worthy of note: from 1967 to 1995, an increasing share of household income possessed by the highest Fig. 3 Share of Aggregate Household Income by Quintile: 1975–1995 (In percent) 48.7 45.3 43.2 24.8 17.1 24.6 23.3 16.3 15.2 4.4 4.0 9.7 9.1 15.9 17.0 21.0 3.7 1975 1985 1995 Lowest quintile 1975 1985 1995 Second quintile 1975 1985 1995 Third quintile Middle 60 percent Source: U.S. Bureau of the Census, March 1996 Current Population Survey 3 1975 1985 1995 Fourth quintile 1975 1985 1995 Highest quintile Top 5 percent 10.5 (1) increasingly more unequal wage distribution resulting from a shift away from less- to more-educated workers; (2) intensified global competition and immigration; (3) a decline in union membership; (4) the diminishing real value of the minimum wage; (5) a heightened need for computer skills; (6) an increased demand for temporary workers; (7) a shift away from married-couple toward single-parent and nonfamily households; and (8) the greater tendency for men with higher-than-average earnings to marry women of similar earning power (Weinberg, 1996b). Dewey conceived of the school as a place where children and teachers alike might attain personal fulfillment and social well-being (Jackson, 1990). To quote him directly: What the best and wisest parent wants for his own child, that must the community want for all of its children. Any other ideal for our schools is narrow and unlovely; acted upon, it destroys our democracy. All that society has accomplished for itself is put, through the agency of the school, at the disposal of its future members. . . . Here individualism and socialism are at one. Only by being true to the full growth of all the individuals who make it up, can society by any chance be true to itself. (Dewey, 1990, p. 7) Schools and Society: A Selective Review C R I T I C A L T H E O RY The next few sections of the report selectively review the writings of several people who have commented on the relationship between education and the economy: John Dewey, Samuel Bowles and Herbert Gintis, Michael Apple, and Henry Giroux. The latter four belong to a school of thought loosely called critical theory. While there may be no single “critical theory,” writings under that rubric reflect an attempt in common to critique capitalism and the social and economic inequality it has fostered, and to contemplate possibilities for change (Giroux, 1983). Dewey on democracy and education. One of the most influential educational theorists of his time, Dewey (1909, 1916, 1990) wrote extensively on the relationship of schools and society in the early part of the 20th century, when the capitalist system as we now know it was emerging. Briefly, he saw the school as a social institution capable of meeting the demands of the new order for economic growth and social integration. According to Dewey, the school should perform a profoundly democratic function: (1) nurture the child’s individual capacities, (2) inculcate a spirit of community among its members, and (3) prepare the young to adapt to as well as “shape and direct” society (Dewey, 1909). Any social organization assigning its young a position based on parental status or wealth and not ability was considered by Dewey to be undemocratic. Though he was aware that the economic system of his day was characterized by inequity, he assumed that the progressive educational philosophy he articulated should (and could) correct “unfair privilege . . . and deprivation.” In Dewey’s vision, schools transformed rather than perpetuated the status quo (Dewey, 1916). Alteration of the status quo was desirable so that diverse individual talents and abilities might be better employed for the sake of the common good. Schooling: Reproducing, resisting, and reconstructing an unequal society. Writing much later, Bowles and Gintis (1976) contended that schools were not “the great engines of democracy” that Dewey imagined them to be. They saw American society as politically autocratic and believed that rigid patterns of domination and subordination characteristic of capitalism could be found in schools. Drawing upon Marxist ideas and a variety of descriptive, historical, and statistical sources, they argued that schools reproduce the unequal distribution of power 4 that exists in society—a power that varies by race, ethnicity, gender, and class. According to Bowles and Gintis, schools cultivate forms of personal development and consciousness consistent with this objective, and they promote modes of social interaction similar to stratification in the workplace. Distinct social groups are socialized differently: for instance, blacks and other minorities experience coercive authority; those of the working class compliance with rules; and the more economically privileged internal standards of control (Bowles & Gintis, 1988a). In this way, these groups are positioned for their designated place in society and assigned low, medium, or high status. Schools ostensibly espouse equality of opportunity based on merit; but, in fact, they draw upon the concept to defend and reproduce economic disparity and to perpetuate the status quo. Bowles and Gintis’s theory (1976) has been criticized by some for being too mechanistic and for neglecting the role of human agency in opposing oppression (Apple, 1995; Bowles & Gintis, 1988b; Giroux, 1983). Apple (1990, 1993, 1995) claimed that by imparting knowledge, dispositions, and values that further the ideological superiority of certain groups, schools act as agents in the construction of a dominant culture that legitimates social and economic inequality. He believed that a complex power struggle between groups distinguished by class, race, and gender occurs in society, decides what’s legitimate, and results in “an organized assemblage of meanings and practices . . . values and actions which are lived” (Apple, 1990, p. 5). Employing various historical, ethnographic, economic, and cultural techniques and a neoMarxist framework, Apple progressed beyond the correspondence principle of Bowles and Gintis (1976) to conceive of the school as more than a microcosm of the economy and an institution defined by it. He sought to explain the mediations between the material conditions of society and the consciousness of its members, the mechanisms of domination, and their operation in the day-to-day life of schools. According to Apple, cultural incorporation arises through ideological transmission, but it represents a dynamic (not static) process. Those situated in schools (teachers, students) do not passively take in societal messages; rather, they “resist, alter, or mediate” them in some way (Apple, 1995, p. 144). And, these reconstructions possess transformative potential. Giroux (1983) similarly asserted that schools epitomize places where different cultural and economic groups vie for power. His conception of resistance distinguishes him from the other theorists cited and assumes a number of points: (1) a dialectical notion of human agency; (2) power exercised both as a mode of domination and defiance, and of cultural and creative expression; and (3) the possibility of radical transformation. According to Giroux, those experiencing repression do not always simply submit; sometimes they oppose and even alter school processes and practices. While the wider society presents formidable challenges, “social spaces” exist in schools (especially in classrooms) for opposition, although that opposition is unlikely to be entirely successful (Aronowitz & Bologh, 1983). He argued that earlier radical conceptions of schooling failed to illuminate the complex ways that various groups differing by class, race, and gender resist mechanisms of control. These conceptions epitomized a “blueprint for cynicism and despair” and neglected to offer any hope of social change (Giroux, 1983, p. 59). That was to be found in a radical pedagogy that encompassed both critical reflection and social action. Summary. In the views of these theorists, the process of schooling can only be understood by relating it to its broader socioeconomic context. Whereas Dewey saw that context as profoundly democratic in character and conducive to upward social mobility, the others conceived of it as just the opposite. For Bowles and Gintis, Apple, and Giroux, schools represent places of cultural and political subjugation, where children are taught certain ideological messages and are socialized for a predetermined position in 5 society. These meanings shape the form and content of what’s transmitted and serve a number of purposes: (1) they grant superiority to certain groups, and (2) they sustain a capitalist economy characterized by social and economic inequality. ety of school-related messages, those pertaining to work and play seemed to King to clarify best how schools connect with their larger socioeconomic context. Through observation and interviews focusing on a public school classroom thought to be exemplary, she documented through ethnographic study numerous aspects of daily activity. She found that the goals of learning “to share, to listen, to put things away, and to follow the classroom routine” were emphasized by the teacher from the very first weeks of school (Apple with King, 1990, p. 53). The use of materials was controlled by the teacher, who defined the meanings ascribed to them as “work” or “play.” Work constituted what children learned from the teacher, whereas play consisted of free-time activities bestowed as a reward for completing assigned tasks, with the former viewed as more important than the latter by both teachers and children. King pointed out that some of the meanings associated with work included (1) that it requires following instructions, (2) that it’s compulsory, and (3) that it must be done according to a prescribed procedure that results in the same end product by all. She equated the children’s experiences in this setting with the world of adult work, where one is expected to offer unquestioning obedience to authority and to exhibit qualities of diligence, cooperation, and perseverance, even though other attributes such as ingenuity and excellence may be compromised. Extending her research to the later years, King (1983) investigated the criteria used by children in grades 1 through 5 to distinguish work from play, as well as the relationship between the two constructs. Observations were conducted in 10 classrooms situated in a single school, and interviews were completed with 60 children (equally divided by gender) from predominantly white middle-class families. Invoking a personal standard, the children employed the term “play” to categorize all those activities not designated as “work”; King found the reverse to be true for the kindergartners she studied. The latter relied on the social context to define an activity. By fifth grade, more activities were THE HIDDEN (AND NOT SO HIDDEN) CURRICULUM Schools function as agents of socialization. As such, they draw upon two forms of curricula: one overt, another covert. The first specifies a formally articulated, explicit course of study; the other an informal, tacit (or hidden) one. The “hidden curriculum” may be defined as “those unstated norms, values, and beliefs” that are implicitly taught in schools (Giroux, 1983, p. 47). The concept has been assigned very different meanings and analyses, but that definition characterizes them all (Giroux, 1983). A variety of learnings may be imparted through the hidden curriculum. Some of these, drawn from six case studies of elementary and high school classrooms, include “Science is important”; “Some knowledge is objective; other has to be constructed”; and “Men can be caring parents” (McCutcheon, 1995). Empirical studies have also examined the hidden curriculum from a critical theory perspective, documenting its use in schools as a form of social reproduction and control. These investigations have carefully examined the construct from the perspective of curricula-in-use, teacher practices, and classroom relationships, and to a much lesser extent their effects on student behavior (Mickelson, 1987). This research has addressed diverse issues pertaining to class, race, and gender. Only a few studies on the hidden (and not so hidden) curriculum from a critical theory perspective are highlighted here. King’s ethnography of a kindergarten classroom. Selecting the kindergarten because it represents a “critical moment” in the socialization of children, King examined those social meanings necessary for success in school and later life (Apple with King, 1990). While children are exposed to a vari6 named “play,” and children increasingly employed adult perspectives to distinguish the two; that is, play was associated with something pleasurable, and work was devoid of it. With respect to the relation between the two, King found that play for the children in grades 1 through 5 sustained the school’s work orientation. Three forms of play present in the children’s responses (instrumental, recreation, illicit) provided opportunities for the extension or continuation of work, compensation for it, or opposition to the coercive rules and expectations of the classroom. From her findings she concluded that play provided a diversion from the monotony of work and permitted its endurance. As the researcher explained, inasmuch as it occurred in a “workplace” and was controlled by a teacher who limited its appearance to designated times and spaces, play represented “an escape from” and not “a confrontation” (King, 1983). Any promise play might have held for social critique was lost through the distortion of its capacity for autonomous personal expression. King’s work (Apple with King, 1990; King, 1983) has been criticized by Giroux (1983) for its failure to adequately address the complex interaction of culture and consciousness, and the ways that students differing by class, race, and gender oppose social control. Anyon’s content analysis of social studies texts. In a study now considered by some to be seminal, Anyon (1979a) investigated whether United States history textbooks presented an ideological bias that favored certain political, economic, or other groups. Seventeen well-known, secondary-school works served as the sample for the study; the texts appeared on lists of “Books Approved for Use” by boards of education in two large, urban school systems of the Northeast that were populated by large numbers of minority and white students from poor and workingclass families. Content selected for analysis consisted of economic and labor history from the Civil War to World War I. Anyon reasoned that the considerable conflict of interest and struggle for power occurring during that period (1865 to 1917) permitted an examination of these important issues: (1) whose knowledge the texts sanctioned, and (2) whether the prerogatives of any group more than another were privileged. The researcher scrutinized such topics as economic developments and solutions to economic problems, treatment of economic concepts and groups, industrialists and labor conflict, and labor unions. To cite only a few of her many findings, all the texts stressed certain economic developments (e.g., new inventions, expansion of the railroads) in a salutary fashion, whereas other developments (e.g., the concentration of “big business,” workers’ problems) were treated briefly and in a way supportive of certain groups. Governmental reform as a means of effecting economic change was underscored by every text examined, while other more radical forms (e.g. the adoption of socialism) were disavowed. Various economic groups (e.g., workers, managers, industrialists) were given disparate coverage. Anyon emphasized that contributions to economic development made by workers were omitted, obscured, or downplayed. The negative consequences of monopolies for some (e.g., small business owners, consumers) were described by all the texts, but effects on workers (e.g., low wages, control over their lives) were ignored by most books or given cursory treatment by a few. According to the researcher, the acquisition of wealth was dealt with in an ideological way: the depiction of industrialists of the period (e.g., Andrew Carnegie) by 12 of the books skirted the methods used for accumulating power. She inferred from her findings that, through the promotion of what might be characterized as a “rags to riches” success story, the texts sanctioned hard work as well as the status quo by implying that workers could achieve similar good fortune. Anyon also reported that the texts she examined provided a narrow discussion of labor history. The treatment cast more radical segments of the union movement in an unsympathetic light. Coverage in the books was brief, and 7 only a few of the more than 30,000 strikes occurring during this period were described. Fifteen textbooks discussed a successful strike; and the ones selected by 14 of them exemplified the failure of labor. The messages conveyed respectively were that success ensued only through government intervention, which is a more appropriate form of recourse, and that strikes were questionable as a valid approach to change. She found that strikes were discussed in such a way as “to avoid conflict and to facilitate consensus” (Anyon, 1979a). Anyon concluded from this analysis that business, as well as powerful labor unions empowered by business, profited from such a presentation; those whose circumstances were only marginally improved by labor, or who might alter the distribution of power, did not benefit. In sum, Anyon determined that the 17 textbooks she examined were biased in their presentation. Through a variety of means (e.g., omissions, subtle distinctions and emphases, distortions, and hidden assumptions), they predisposed the reader to the acceptance of certain values and, as Anyon put it, provided “boundaries to social choice” (Anyon, 1979a). They favored the interests, prerogatives, and points of view of the wealthy and powerful, whereas those of the working class and the poor were not represented. As she emphasized in summing up her findings: On a more sanguine note, she pointed out that the school curriculum, although it served as an agent of social control, could also be made to promote a very different purpose, namely, social change. Giroux (1983) has criticized Anyon’s work for the same reasons as King’s writings (Apple with King, 1990; King, 1983) cited earlier. Elsewhere, Anyon (1979b) has written that social studies curricula in the United States have highlighted five themes: (1) the manifestations of democracy; (2) political freedoms and opportunity; (3) the autonomy, possibility, and progress that the economy affords; (4) the importance of political consensus and progress, social harmony and compromise; and (5) intense “Americanism” or patriotism. These themes, according to Anyon, operate as curriculum organizing principles that legitimize social inequality. As she put it, “Schools and school knowledge are like they are because society is like it is” (Anyon, 1979b, p. 55). Sleeter and Grant’s study of elementary curricula. Sleeter and Grant (1991) examined four aspects of diversity (race, class, gender, and disability), as represented by four subject areas of the elementary school curriculum (reading and language arts, social studies, mathematics, and science). Forty-seven textbooks comprised the sample. At the time of the study, these texts (copyrighted between 1980 and 1988) were employed in grade 1 through 8 classrooms throughout the United States. An instrument was devised for use that incorporated a variety of techniques (e.g., picture, story, and language analysis). Only findings pertaining to social class are presented here. The researchers scrutinized 15 reading and language arts textbooks (by eight publishers) and found that they showed little diversity by class. They mainly highlighted people of middle-class; and they sometimes explicitly denied the existence of inequality and injustice. For example, a story appearing in a book featuring predominantly white characters depicted white and black figures of both genders in a variety of equal roles. The message thus transmitted, that The textbook reports of work, wealth, and the problems of industrial workers imply that we should regard the poor as responsible for their own poverty: poverty is a consequence of the failure of individuals, rather than of the failure of society to distribute economic resources universally. This ideology encourages education and other actions that attempt to change the individual, while leaving the unequal economic structures intact. (Anyon, 1979a, p. 383) 8 “anyone can be anything,” obfuscated, according to the researchers, the existence of discrimination as an important social issue (Sleeter & Grant, 1991). In the texts, white and lower-class characters of color were portrayed in a variety of ways: either realistically (e.g., pursuing a lowpaying job), sympathetically (e.g., desiring to make money and subsequently becoming more successful), or stereotypically (e.g., being uneducated). Sleeter and Grant indicated that their analysis of 14 social studies texts (by nine publishers) revealed that the treatment of socioeconomic diversity as an issue was virtually absent, with people in poverty not explicitly discussed. Eight mathematics textbooks (by five publishers) and a computer textbook were also examined, and the findings showed all nine works emphasizing middle-class characters; but a few financially well-off figures (whites, blacks, and Asians), and a few lower-class individuals (usually of color) were also presented. The researchers explained that the texts focused on material consumption through the inclusion of story problems involving the expenditure of money. Most problems involved sums appropriate for an average budget, but a few required a relatively large outlay. And finally, they reported that their examination of 10 science textbooks (by six publishers) disclosed that (1) the depiction of class in the pictures was difficult to ascertain; (2) the majority of the portrayals suggested middle- or upper-class settings and artifacts; and (3) those belonging to the under- or workingclass were rarely described. In interpreting their findings, Sleeter and Grant emphasized that the books imparted the image of a society unstratified by class, which was avoided by the texts and, as they pointed out, overlooked by most textbook analyses. They concluded that the texts they examined deemphasized or disregarded the interests of the poor (as well as people of color, females, and the disabled); depicted social constructions as being, in their words, “natural”; and sanctioned the status quo. And the books selectively incorporated some ideas (e.g., existing social relations characterized by harmony and equality) into the curriculum, while excluding others (e.g., the causes of poverty, questions about the distribution of wealth). Mickelson’s inquiry about student response to the hidden curriculum. Extending empirical work on the hidden curriculum and its role in social reproduction, Mickelson’s study (1987) is distinguishable from those just reviewed in two respects: (1) in considering how successfully ideological messages are transmitted in schools, and (2) in documenting empirically their link with human consciousness. The previously described studies examined curricula-in-use without analyzing student behavior in response. In Mickelson’s study, a questionnaire was administered to more than 1,800 seniors characterized by family, racial, and ethnic diversity, who were enrolled in over 60 social studies classrooms situated in nine comprehensive public high schools of the Los Angeles area. Students were asked to respond to forced-choice items requiring them to select one of two statements best reflecting their beliefs; on one of the items, the brackets for marking a reply were missing. Mickelson observed a class-based and track-specific pattern of responses to the questionnaire. He found that many working-class and lowertrack high schoolers (representing every race and ethnic background) requested clarification concerning the missing brackets; those of middle-class or higher-track status did not, but rather resolved the ambiguity independently. Furthermore, the latter challenged the forcedchoice format of the questions (by asking, “What if I believe both”), while the former complied without question. Mickelson interpreted these findings in terms of social reproduction theory, reasoning that the students responded in ways that predicted their future place in society: middle-class and upper-track students as professionals, managers, and future leaders, and working-class and lower-track students as docile workers. He reported that, although participation was voluntary, some students nonetheless displayed resis9 tance in ways that were class-based. Some middle-class or upper-track students demonstrated their opposition by openly declining to take part; some working-class students displayed sabotage-like actions. Mickelson equated the latter’s behavior with the reactions of assembly-line workers who resist monotony and oppression through subversion. Summary. Though this review covers only a few selected studies, the empirical evidence nonetheless documents the existence of a hidden (and not so hidden) curriculum, from kindergarten through high school, that imparts to children certain ideological messages about society. The dispositions cultivated (e.g., conformity), the concepts emphasized (e.g., the value of hard work), and the norms sanctioned (e.g., allegiance to the status quo) predispose the recipient toward the acceptance, and reproduction, of a system characterized by social and economic inequality. Suppositions that should be open to debate in a democratic society are presented as unquestioned truths. But, as Mickelson’s work also suggests, students do not always passively acquiesce to the messages that schools impart. (1) awareness of status differences between rich and poor, (2) causes of economic inequality, (3) judgments about it, and (4) sociodemographic variations in children’s and adults’ conceptions. Although cultural comparisons might be illuminating, this review is limited to studies conducted in the U.S. The adult views described come from comprehensive investigations with national samples completed over the past quarter century; those of children come from studies selected to coincide with the same time period. A WA R E N E S S O F S TAT U S D I F F E R E N C E S BETWEEN RICH AND POOR An interview study of children, aged 5 to 18 and from a variety of socioeconomic groups, shows a tentative developmental sequence of perceptions of poverty. The children responded to questions asking for their descriptions of rich and poor and explanations about how the two classes are alike and different. Their comments revealed a progression toward decentration— from peripheral, to psychological, to sociocentric conceptions. The youngest subjects (6 to 11 years) tended to focus on external, observable qualities (e.g., appearances); those at the next level (11 to 14 years) inferred internal states (e.g., traits); and the oldest (14 to 17 years) focused on relationships within society (e.g., class membership and its effect on class consciousness). With increasing age the children’s observations exhibited a more multiple-classificatory framework; they could recognize similarities as well as differences between rich and poor—“they’re both human beings” (Leahy, 1981, 1983b, 1990). Other research has explored the ideas of younger children. A study of white 3-, 4-, and 5year-olds (drawn from lower- and middle-SES families) found that preschoolers could make gross distinctions between rich and poor in response to a request that they separate and describe pictures of the two. A few of the children spontaneously commented on two class- Societal Conceptions of Poverty The few studies available on societal conceptions of poverty suggest that increasingly with age children justify and explain economic inequality as a matter of equity; they also regard privation as legitimate and inevitable. These beliefs reflect a capitalist ideology consistent with adult notions and the perspectives of critical theory cited earlier. Inasmuch as these beliefs attribute poverty to individual deficiencies, they down play other more important causes, hamper the design of antipoverty policy, and preclude meaningful social change on behalf of the poor. Earlier work by the author supporting these conclusions is briefly presented here (Chafel, 1995, 1997a, 1997b). Four themes structure this part of the discussion: 10 related attributes—job and attire. The data showed that the children in this study perceived rich and poor to be more different than alike (Ramsey, 1991). living in a community experiencing hard times, the results are consistent with other research (Furby, 1979; Leahy, 1983a, 1983b, 1990). In another study, white subjects from a middle- and upper middle-class locale cited equity increasingly with age. The 11th graders in that study drew upon the construct, “some people work hard and achieve,” with moderate frequency as a reason for the unequal distribution of wealth (i.e., ownership of more possessions). At this age, subjects, however, also showed increasing awareness of the inevitability of economic inequality arising from factors beyond individual control (e.g., the environment or chance). Adults in the study cited equity, circumstances beyond individual control, and inheritance as causes of inequality (Furby, 1979). Other evidence corroborates a developmental pattern of conceptions about the causes of inequality. At the stage of peripheral ideas (6 to 11 years), children were unable to think causally; rather they drew upon definitional criteria (e.g., money, ownership of possessions) to account for economic inequality. Those at the stages of psychological and sociocentric conceptions (11 to 17 years) cited equity; they displayed a belief that economic inequality resulted from differences in merit and cited a number of factors as responsible—effort, education, intelligence, and work. But responses of some subjects at the sociocentric level also revealed an awareness of class competition. Subjects rarely referred to external, structural factors (e.g., race) as causes of economic inequality (Leahy, 1983a, 1983b, 1990). The tendency of older subjects to invoke equity as an explanation led the author of the above study to say that they believed in “a just world where ‘the losers’ are viewed as obtaining their just due” (Leahy, 1990, p. 119). Increasingly with age, then, as confirmed by several studies, children employ equity as an explanation for economic inequality (Chafel, 1997b). Equity also appears in the thinking of adults as a salient cause of poverty. Two investigations conducted several years apart found that CAUSES OF ECONOMIC INEQUALITY The preschoolers in the study of younger children just described showed only a limited understanding of the nature and causes of economic inequality. The few who were able to reply to the problem as posed expressed ideas reflecting concrete monetary transactions. In reply to a question about “why some people had more money than others,” the children indicated, for example, that those who were poor “forgot to go to the store to get their money” (Ramsey, 1991). In another study, 3rd- to 12th-grade children, from a largely black and somewhat skewed working-class sample, were asked about their notions of equality of opportunity, but not directly about the causes of poverty. Of the elementary school–aged children able to reply, 70% reacted negatively to the statement: “Do all kids in America have the same chance to grow up and get the good things in life?” The older children in the sample responded similarly. And 68% of the subjects able to distinguish the different types of children without equal opportunity specified that inequities were the result of ethnic, racial, or socioeconomic disadvantage (Simmons & Rosenberg, 1971). These findings differ from those obtained in a study of adults. In that study, adults communicated a belief that “everyone who works hard can get ahead” (Kluegel & Smith,1986). But this apparent discrepancy may be the result of different questions rather than differences in underlying beliefs (Chafel, 1997b). In several other studies, equity (that is, merit) was offered as an explanation for economic inequality. The construct was cited as the cause for wealth, poverty, and unemployment by many of the 8-, 11-, and 14-year-old middleclass children of one study (Harrah & Friedman, 1990). And although this particular sample was 11 subjects drawn from a variety of socioeconomic status groups viewed economic privation as emanating from individualistic factors, e.g., lack of effort, more than structural ones, e.g., low wages (Feagin, 1972, 1975; Kluegel & Smith, 1986). Using the same measure as the earlier investigation, the later work found that the ranking of reasons for poverty remained the same despite more than a decade between the two studies. The adult subjects in Furby’s study (1979), described earlier, differed from these findings in that they also highlighted external factors. This inconsistency may have arisen from the latter study’s more restricted sample (Chafel, 1997b). Viewed as a whole, the research cited suggests considerable societal agreement about the causes of poverty: to a large extent, the economically disadvantaged are blamed for their plight (Chafel, 1997b). others,” was the salient positive assessment for the 11th graders. Adult responses were more equally distributed between positive and negative evaluations. In their positive responses they mentioned the legitimate accumulation of possessions along with differential work and ability. They also expressed indifference to inequality more frequently than did the younger children. The findings on adults are consistent with other research (Kluegel & Smith, 1986). The way questions are phrased may explain the distribution of negative and positive replies in different studies (Chafel, 1995, 1997b). Explicit phrasing, “Do you think it is fair that some people own more things than other people?” evoked more positive replies in one study (Furby, Harter, & John, 1975) compared to a more general wording, “How do you feel about some people having a lot more things than other people?” in a later study, which resulted in more negative replies (Furby, 1979). Phrasing may also explain why subjects in the later study held more negative attitudes than those in the work about to be described (Leahy, 1983a, 1983b, 1990). The first study posed the query more generally, the latter more explicitly (Chafel, 1997b). In this next study, a developmental progression toward greater acceptance of economic equality was evident in the children’s replies. At the first level (ages 6 to 11), children expressed concern for consequences (e.g., “Poor should not suffer”) and used definitional criteria (e.g., “They should be rich because they have a lot of money”); at the second level (ages 11 to 14), children employed equity as a justification or equality principles as a challenge; and at the third level (ages 14 to 17) the respondents invoked equity and fatalistic ideas (e.g., “There will always be poorer people”) and class conflict. Other trends were discerned: a conception that the rich should help the poor declined with age (beyond 11 years). And equality challenges grew (at 14 years) and then decreased (at 17 years) (Leahy, 1983a, 1983b, 1990). The fatalism expressed about economic JUDGMENTS ABOUT ECONOMIC INEQUALITY Some of the investigations just described report findings concerning children’s judgments about economic inequality. Although judgments were not directly assessed in the study of 3-, 4-, and 5-year-olds, significantly more older than younger children did display a norm of sharing when questioned about “what rich and poor people might do together.” Yet only a small proportion of the preschoolers responded in this way (Ramsey, 1991). Kindergartners and second graders in an earlier study (Furby 1979) found it difficult to respond evaluatively, i.e., positively or negatively, to the question, “How do you feel about some people having a lot more things than other people?” Instead they disclosed various nonevaluative sentiments: for example, a norm of sharing. Fifth graders gave more evaluative judgments, although most of their replies were still nonevaluative. Eleventh graders provided even more. For both age groups, negative assessments outnumbered the positive. To illustrate briefly, a norm of equality appeared as a negative assessment for the 5th graders, and the idea of differential work, that “some people work harder than 12 inequality that increased by adolescence was, according to the author, “not based on a view that classes would actively resist change,” but instead upon “a view that human nature and complex society require stratification” (Leahy, 1990, pp. 116–117). The researcher concluded that the weight of the evidence, if somewhat mixed, was more indicative of subjects’ acceptance of the existing social system. Overall then, the findings just reviewed confirm that over the course of development children increasingly draw upon equity in explaining and judging economic inequality (Chafel, in 1997b). Children’s ideas about social mobility and change further substantiate the trend. A developmental progression in their thinking has been observed, beginning with (1) a conception of the wealthy helping the poor, which increased and then substantially declined; then (2) notions of equity (e.g., effort, work) became prominent, and the idea of social change arising from a sharing of wealth appeared; and finally (3) increasing references to societal change, class conflict, or its futility were communicated. Sociocentric challenges to economic inequality were rarely observed; equity characterized the responses of the oldest children (Leahy, 1983a, 1983b, 1990). Equity similarly appears in the thinking of adults about the justice of economic inequality. Whereas many of the adults sampled by Kluegel and Smith (1986) supported greater economic equality in fact, they did not in principle. In fact, they believed in a minimum and maximum level of income (in the case of the former, according to “need” to some degree), but in principle they ascribed to the following beliefs about economic inequality, that (1) it results from unequal ability and talents, (2) it inspires hard work, (3) human nature prescribes it, and (4) it is unalterable. Consistent with other research (Feagin, 1972), when subjects were asked about criteria for justly allocating income in principle, very few perceived strict equality as just. Substantially more cited “need,” and a large majority mentioned equity. These societal images of the poor affect support for social welfare policy (Blank, 1989; Chafel, 1997b; Cook & Barrett, 1992; Corcoran, Duncan, Gurin, & Gurin, 1985; Feagin, 1975; Hartman, 1984; Hendrickson & Axelson, 1985; Kluegel & Smith, 1986). It has been found that subjects who ascribed to structural views of poverty were more inclined than those with individualistic views to endorse measures to assist the poor (Feagin, 1972; Kluegel & Smith, 1986). In another study, subjects expressed a belief in a minimally acceptable standard of living for the poor, but levels of support were influenced by perceptions of deservingness. Greatest support was accorded those whose problems were characterized as extrapersonal or structural in nature (e.g., physical disability) and those who were depicted as seeking to improve themselves (Will, 1993). S O C I O D E M O G R A P H I C VA R I AT I O N S I N C H I L D R E N ’ S A N D A D U LT S ’ C O N C E P T I O N S The conceptions that children and adults hold about poverty vary with subjects’ sociodemographic characteristics. These findings have been summarized in detail elsewhere (Chafel, 1997b) and are only briefly described here. It has been found that in the case of children socioeconomic status does not have a large effect. Across groups, children agree more than they disagree (Leahy, 1981, 1983a, 1983b, 1990; Simmons & Rosenberg, 1971). Adults of lower socioeconomic status are less sanguine about the “system” than other groups, and although sociodemographic factors influence adults’ support for redistributive social policies on behalf of the poor, they do so less than the dominant ideology (Cook & Barrett, 1992; Feagin, 1972; Kluegel & Smith, 1986). A recently published study analyzed the attributions that 7th- to 12th-grade children from three communities (the inner city, the urban ring, and wealthy suburbs) made about poverty and other phenomena (Flanagan, Ingram, Gallay, & Gallay, 1997). Inner-city youth were more likely than those residing in 13 the urban ring or wealthy suburbs to attribute poverty to personal/dispositional factors (e.g., “They did not save their money”), whereas those from the urban ring and wealthy suburbs drew more upon systemic/structural attributions (e.g., “There are not enough jobs”) or a combination of the two. According to the researchers, the youth in poor urban communities perceived attenuated opportunities for success and so concluded that self-reliance provided a better way out of poverty than community support. Other findings confirmed that the study’s urban youth received more intense parental messages about the value of self-reliance and perceived a more alienating school environment than did their more privileged peers. Analyses of the adolescents’ attributions did not include results for the sample as a whole or by age, so it is difficult to compare these findings with the other research reviewed here. The frequency distribution does suggest, however, that systemic/structural factors may have figured more prominently than they did in earlier work (Leahy, 1983a, 1983b, 1990). This later study employed an adaptation of the earlier study’s measure and relied on a more restricted sample of subjects selected from a single metropolitan area, whereas the former sampled three different cities: New York, Boston, and Washington. It also utilized a much smaller pool of subjects encompassing a narrower age range. The finding that inner-city youth were more likely to possess personal/dispositional attributions than the more privileged subjects conflicts with other studies of adults showing a reverse pattern (Feagin, 1972, 1975; Kluegel & Smith, 1986). Whether these recent findings indicate a shift in thinking about the poor warrants further research; the weight of evidence reviewed here suggests otherwise. reform, and the inevitability and justice of poverty are either explicitly or implicitly expressed (Chafel, 1997b). But viewing economic want in terms of individual psychological attributes exemplifies simplistic thinking (Chafel, 1997b). Poverty arises from myriad complex causes (Bianchi, 1993), and the dominant societal image of the poor as “less industrious” than the nonpoor is not supported by demographic data (Blank, 1989). Whereas individual personal factors may explain why some subset of the population is poor, a predisposition to “blame the victim” downplays other more important social-structural causes of poverty (e.g., barriers to opportunity), diverts attention away from those factors, and hampers the design of antipoverty policy (Blank, 1989; Chafel, 1997b; Feagin, 1975; Hendrickson & Axelson, 1985; Kluegel & Smith, 1986). Children are socialized via many channels: parents and siblings, schools, peers, books, the media, and more. None of the studies cited attempted to ascertain the source of children’s ideas about poverty, but it is not unreasonable to assume, from the diverse streams of research reviewed here, that schools acted as important agents of enculturation. Children enter schools at a young, impressionable age and spend many years there; the effect can be profound. The final section that follows presents discourse on the conduct and funding of policy-relevant research relating to the themes developed in this report. Such research would address (1) the messages conveyed to children by schools, and (2) children’s beliefs about poverty derived from other sources. S U M M A RY The several broad directions of research described here warrant funding initiatives by the federal government or private foundations. By examining existing assumptions about social and economic inequality and by suggesting pos- Implications for Policy-Relevant Research This review confirms the saliency of equity in adults’ and children’s thinking about economic privation. Notions of individualism and work, a perception of the poor as requiring moral 14 sible modes of intervention, research on these themes should generate information useful to policymakers (Chafel, 1995). From this work, pragmatic action-oriented recommendations can evolve. School messages. What messages do schools convey to children about social and economic inequality? Do schools function as agents of social control? In what ways, to what extent, and with what effects? What is taught to students of diverse groups, and how do they respond (Apple, 1990, 1993, 1995; Giroux, 1983)? In what ways are meritocratic values transmitted via taken-for-granted classroom routines, e.g., grouping practices, the use of labels (Apple, 1995, Giroux, 1983)? Three aspects of school life might be targeted for study: the basic day-today regularities of the classroom, the specific forms of curriculum knowledge transmitted, and the ideological perspectives implicit in educators’ pursuits (Apple, 1990). Issues as complex as these require diverse approaches. Empirical inquiries representative of a variety of disciplines, ideological persuasions, and paradigms should be funded. Only by viewing the above questions from different points of view might the complexity of the phenomena under study be grasped and a balanced picture of school life be obtained. Beliefs about poverty from other sources. Schools represent one avenue of socialization, but there are others as well. How do these other sources (e.g., peers, the media) mediate children’s understandings, and how do the latter interpret the messages they receive (Chafel, 1997b; Ramsey, 1991)? For example, Hall and Jose (1983) have asked “whether a child’s peers corroborate, oppose, or exert little influence on the acquisition of a coherent belief system about equality from the adult culture” (p. 254). And, to cite another example, what images of poverty are conveyed by children’s literature? To what extent do books for young people of different ages promote stereotypes and cultural conformity (Chafel, 1997b)? And how do children respond to these messages (Chafel, Fitzgibbons, Cutter, & Burke-Weiner, 1997)? And finally, in what ways do cognitive development and socialization interact to affect children’s knowledge (Chafel, 1997b; McLoyd, 1990)? For example, Leahy (1990) reported that children from 6 to 11 years of age displayed a norm of sharing, when thinking about rich and poor, a conception that diverges from mainstream thinking about poverty. How might such deviations from societal norms be explained (Chafel, 1997b; McLoyd, 1990)? Past work on children’s conceptions of poverty is not only sparse but also limited conceptually and methodologically (Chafel, 1995). Studies should be funded representing diverse theoretical frameworks and approaches to inquiry (Chafel, 1997b). Findings amassed from these sources might provide a more complex and up-to-date picture of child functioning than is presently available. In this way, deeper insights may emerge about issues having both developmental and societal significance, and steps may be taken to address conformist thinking (Chafel, 1995, 1997b). References Anyon, J. (1979a). Ideology and United States history textbooks. Harvard Educational Review, 49, 361–386. Anyon, J. (1979b). Education, social “structure” and the power of individuals. Theory and Research in Social Education, 7, 49–59. Apple, M. (1990). Ideology and curriculum. New York: Routledge. Apple, M. (1993). Official knowledge: Democratic education in a conservative age. New York: Routledge. Apple, M. (1995). Education and power. New York: Routledge. Apple, M., with King, N. (1990). Economics 15 and control in everyday school life. In M. Apple, Ideology and curriculum (pp. 43–60). New York: Routledge. Aronowitz, S., & Bologh, R. (1983). Introduction. In H. Giroux, Theory and resistance in education (pp. xi–xii). New York: Bergin & Garvey. Bianchi, S. (1993). Children of poverty: Why are they poor? In J. Chafel (Ed.), Child poverty and public policy (pp. 91–125). Washington, DC: The Urban Institute Press. Blank, R. (1989). Poverty and policy: The many faces of the poor. In C. R. Strain (Ed.), Prophetic visions and economic realities (pp. 156–168). Grand Rapids, MI: Eerdman’s Publishing Co. Bowles, S., & Gintis, H. (1976). Schooling in capitalist America. New York: Basic Books, Inc. Bowles, S., & Gintis, H. (1988a). Prologue: The correspondence principle. In M. Cole (Ed.), Bowles and Gintis revisited: Correspondence and contradiction in educational theory (pp. 1–4). London: Falmer Press. Bowles, S., & Gintis, H. (1988b). Schooling in capitalist America: Reply to our critics. In M. Cole (Ed.), Bowles and Gintis revisited: Correspondence and contradiction in educational theory (pp. 235–245). London: Falmer Press. Chafel, J. (1995). Children’s conceptions of poverty. In S. Reifel (Ed.), Advances in early education and day care, (Vol. 7, pp. 27–57). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. Chafel, J. (1997a). Children’s views of poverty: A review of research and implications for teaching. The Educational Forum, 61, 360–371. Chafel, J. (1997b). Societal images of poverty: Child and adult beliefs. Youth and Society, 28, 432–463. Chafel, J., Fitzgibbons, S., Cutter, L., & BurkeWeiner, K. (1997). Poverty in books for young children: A sociological analysis. Manuscript submitted for publication. Cook, F., & Barrett, E. (1992). Support for the American welfare state: The views of Congress and the public. New York: Columbia University Press. Corcoran, M., Duncan, G., Gurin, G., & Gurin, P. (1985). Myth and reality: The causes and persistence of poverty. Journal of Policy Analysis and Management, 4, 516–536. Dewey, J. (1909). Moral principles in education. Boston: Houghton Mifflin Company. Dewey, J. (1916). Democracy and education. New York: Free Press. Dewey, J. (1990). The school and society, and the child and the curriculum. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Feagin, J. (1972). Poverty: We still believe that God helps those who help themselves. Psychology Today, 6, 101–110, 129. Feagin, J. (1975). Subordinating the poor. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall, Inc. Flanagan, C., Ingram, P., Gallay, E., & Gallay, E. (1997). Why are people poor? Social conditions and adolescents’ interpretations of the social contract. In R. D. Taylor & M. C. Wang (Eds.), Social and emotional adjustment and family relations in ethnic minority families (pp. 53–62). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Furby, L. (1979). Inequalities in personal possessions: Explanations for and judgements about unequal distribution. Human Development, 22, 180–202. Furby, L., Harter, S., & John, K. (1975). The nature and development of possession and ownership: A cognitive-attitudinal study of 5- to 21-year-olds. Oreg. Res. Inst. Res. Monogr., 15(4). Giroux, H. (1983). Theory and resistance in education. New York: Bergin & Garvey. Hall, W., & Jose, P. (1983). Cultural effects on the development of equality and inequality. In R. Leahy (Ed.), The child’s construction of social inequality (pp. 253-285). New York: Academic Press. Harrah, J., & Friedman, M. (1990). Economic socialization in children in a Midwestern American community. Journal of Economic Psychology, 11, 495–513. 16 Hartman, R. (Ed.). (1984). Poverty and economic justice. New York: Paulist Press. Hendrickson, R., & Axelson, L. (1985). Middleclass attitudes toward the poor: Are they changing? Social Service Review, 59, 295–304. Holmes, S. (1996, September 27). U.S. census finds first income rise in past six years. The New York Times (National Edition), A1, A11. Jackson, P. (1990). Introduction. In J. Dewey, The school and society, and The child and the curriculum (pp. ix-xxxvii). Chicago: University of Chicago Press. King, N. (1983). Play in the workplace. In M. Apple & L. Weis (Eds.), Ideology and practice in schooling (pp. 262–280). Philadelphia: Temple University Press. Kluegel, J., & Smith, E. (1986). Beliefs about inequality: Americans’ views of what is and what ought to be. New York: Aldine De Gruyter. Leahy, R. (1981). The development of the conception of economic inequality: I. Descriptions and comparisons of rich and poor people. Child Development, 52, 523–532. Leahy, R. (1983a). Development of the conceptions of economic inequality: II. Explanations, justifications, and concepts of social mobility and change. Developmental Psychology, 19, 111–125. Leahy, R. (1983b). The development of the conception of social class. In R. Leahy (Ed.), The child’s construction of social inequality (pp. 79–107). New York: Academic Press, Inc. Leahy, R. (1990). The development of concepts of economic and social inequality. New Directions for Child Development, 46, 107–120. McCutcheon, G. (1995). Developing the curriculum: Solo and group deliberation. White Plains, NY: Longman Publishers USA. McLoyd, V. (1990). The impact of economic hardship on black families and children: Psychological distress, parenting, and socioemotional development. Child Development, 61, 311–346. Mickelson, R. (1987). The case of the missing brackets: Teachers and social reproduction. Journal of Education, 169, 78–88. Money income in the United States: 1995 (with separate data on valuation of noncash benefits) (P60–193). (1997, February 6) [on line]. Available on Netscape: http://www. census.gov/hhes/www/income95.html Ramsey, P. (1991). Young children’s awareness and understanding of social class differences. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 152, pp. 71–82. Simmons, R., & Rosenberg, M. (1971). Functions of children’s perceptions of the stratification system. American Sociological Review, 36, 235–249. Sleeter, C., & Grant, C. (1991). Race, class, gender, and disability in current textbooks. In M. Apple & L. Christian-Smith (Eds.), The politics of the textbook (pp. 78–110). New York: Routledge. Weinberg, D. (1996a, September 26). Press briefing on 1995 income, poverty, and health insurance estimates [on line]. Available on Netscape: http://www.census. gov/hhes/www/income95.html Weinberg, D. (1996b, June). A brief look at postwar U.S. income inequality [on line]. Available on Netscape: http://www. census.gov/pub/hhes/www/img/p60191.pdf Will, J. (1993). The dimensions of poverty: Public perceptions of the deserving poor. Social Science Research, 22, 312–332. Zedlewski, S., Clark, S., Meier, E., & Watson, K. (1996). Potential effects of Congressional welfare reform legislation on family incomes. Unpublished manuscript, The Urban Institute, Washington, DC. 17 Acknowledgement I would like to express my appreciation to Landon E. Beyer, Eric Bredo, Jesse Goodman, Aletha Huston, and Gail McCutcheon, for their helpful comments on this work. Special thanks are due to Nancy Thomas for her skillful editorial assistance. About the Author Judith A. Chafel is associate professor in the Department of Curriculum and Instruction in the School of Education at Indiana University, Bloomington. In 1989–90 she was an SRCD congressional science fellow with the U.S. House of Representatives Committee on Ways and Means. She is the editor of Child Poverty and Public Policy, published by the Urban Institute Press, and author of numerous other publications dealing with young children’s social cognition, child and family policy, and child poverty. 18 SRCD Presentations to the Media R Research on Adolescence) to prepare a series of “research briefs” of relevance to pending legislative debates. Research was summarized, and implications for policy were described in five key areas: teenage pregnancy, poverty and welfare, child care, nutrition, and violence prevention. The briefs were disseminated to program staff, program evaluators, policymakers, funders, and the general public. The goal was not to affect the legislative process but to assure that legislators and lay people understand current research findings. For the 1997 biennial meeting in Washington, DC, the Committee worked with Program Committee Cochair Nora Newcombe to plan media coverage of the conference. Media consultant Melissa Ludtke worked closely with Nora to identify topics, themes, and specific presentations of interest. Press packets were sent out, and a breakfast was held for journalists. On Friday morning of the meeting, five SRCD members were invited to present meeting highlights to the press. This briefing drew 35 to 40 reporters, including six television stations. Following are the comments of four of the five presenters: esearch on child development provides information about parenting and family decision-making that is critical to the design and evaluation of programs and policies serving children and families. Yet too frequently such information is relegated to academic journals and fails to reach the public, the staff of programs serving children, evaluators of such programs, policymakers, or funders and others who work on children’s behalf. Better dissemination has the potential not only to improve these efforts but to help maintain the nation’s commitment to the funding of developmental research. In response to this need and to the expressed concerns of SRCD members at the 1995 biennial meeting, the Committee on Child Development, Public Policy, and Public Information has been developing a dissemination strategy. To this end the Committee has secured grants from the Packard Foundation, Foundation for Child Development, and Kaufmann Foundation. The Committee has identified three goals: (1) To make research available to the public and to policymakers that is relevant to pending policy decisions; Aletha Huston (2) To promote the importance of research, both basic and applied, to the public and to policymakers in order to maintain its funding base; and My assignment today is to tell you briefly about research being presented at the conference on two topics: child care and television. These topics may seem unrelated, but they have two important things in common: (3) To instill a general public interest in developmental research by disseminating new and especially intriguing findings. (1) they are among the most important risk factors for young children’s development and, at the same time, they are socializing influences with enormous potential for promoting healthy development; As a first effort, the Committee joined in the spring of 1995 with three other professional associations (Division 7 of the American Psychological Association, the International Society for Infant Studies, and the Society for (2) the basic message about both is similar: It’s not how much but what kind. 19 On Child Care On Television Later today, the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development [NICHD] Study of Early Child Care1 will present research showing that the quality of child care in the first three years of life matters for children’s cognitive and language development and for the sensitivity of mothers’ interactions with their children. The study followed over 1,300 children from birth to age 3 in ten locations around the country. Children who were in nonmaternal care were observed in their child care settings. The study is distinctive not only because of large numbers but because family and child characteristics were taken into account in evaluating the importance of child care. There were two basic questions: Evidence has been accumulating for some time that children learn academic and social skills from educational TV and they learn aggression from violent TV. What is new in the research being presented tomorrow is that these effects last over many years into adolescence and adulthood.3 In one investigation, children who were frequent viewers of educational TV in preschool had higher high school grades in English, math, and science than infrequent viewers. They also had higher academic motivation and creativity. These patterns were not due to differences in parent education or early language ability between children who watched educational TV and those who did not. On the other hand, there were lasting relationships between watching violent TV in childhood and aggression. Children who watched violent TV as preschoolers and who carried TV themes into their play and conversation were more aggressive in high school. Another study in the same symposium found that aggressive attitudes engendered by watching violent TV in childhood carried over into adult life. Many stations are searching for educational programs in response to the new rule requiring them to broadcast at least three hours of informative programs for children every week. A discussion on Sunday morning4 makes it clear that effective educational programs don’t just happen from good intentions. Programs like Sesame Street, Mr. Rogers’ Neighborhood, and Gulla Gulla Island are carefully planned to achieve measurable educational goals by a team that includes expertise in child development and early education. They are based on research testing not only viewers interest but what children learn from them. Does the amount of child care matter? Does the quality of care matter? The amount of care did not predict cognitive and language development. There were no differences associated with the average number of hours in care; that is, children in no child care and children in full-time care were similar. Quality did matter. The important feature of quality was sensitive, responsive behavior by the caregiver and, more specifically, the amount of language stimulation—talking to and listening to the child—which predicted scores on tests of cognitive development and language as early as 15 months of age and on school readiness at age 3. The study also investigated the relation of child care to sensitive maternal interaction in a play session. In this case high amounts of care, especially during the first 6 months of life, were associated with slightly lower levels of sensitivity. But for children in care, quality was slightly associated with higher maternal sensitivity. Another symposium2 contains studies of school-age child care showing that the quality of care and after-school programs continues to be important after children enter school. 20 Nathan Fox child’s social and emotional development. The researchers at the SRCD meeting have gone to great lengths to document these effects with an eye toward affecting public policy. We will hear reports10 on a long-term project documenting exposure to violence and its effects among elementary school children in a New Orleans housing project. Differential effects of exposure in elementary and preschool children, depending upon whether the child was a witness or victim of the exposure will be presented.11 One symposium12 includes the first reports on a self-report measure for children that allows the child to report on the frequency with which he or she has been exposed to aggressive and violent acts. The VEX questionnaire provides information on whether the child was a witness or victim of the violence and important data on the context (home, school, or neighborhood) in which the violence takes place. In all the papers presented on this topic at the meeting, the theme is clear: Exposure to violence or trauma is bad for children’s physical and psychological health. On the Brain A good deal of research on the relation between brain development and behavior is being presented at the meeting of the Society for Research on Child Development. One of the invited speakers, Michael Posner,5 will discuss the different ways in which the brain encodes information. He will illustrate how certain areas of the brain are important in learning a task, whereas different areas are activated when this same task is initiated later on. He will address issues of brain plasticity and how the brain changes with development. Among the other work on brain development are studies of the effects of atypical environments on children, including work on the effects of maternal depression, trauma, and deprivation. One study6 illustrates the negative effects of maternal depression on infant’s brain activity; another7 reveals the negative effects of growing up in Romanian orphanages. An investigation of fetal neural development8 showed that activity level in utero is a strong predictor of temperamental difficulty in infancy. As well, maternal emotional state and stress seem to affect fetal responsivity in adverse ways. According to yet another report,9 inhibited children exhibit a specific pattern of brain electrical activity (EEG), which may reflect their inability to regulate negative affect. Numerous other papers at the meeting examine the intricate relations between brain activity and behavior with the message that context and environment greatly affect brain development. Jacquelynne Eccles On Middle Childhood It is much harder to intervene successfully in children’s lives to improve their chances during middle childhood: • More often than not, individual differences in school achievement increase over the elementary school years—so that discrepancies in performance linked to family and neighborhood income widen. On Effects of Exposure to Violence • More often than not, gains made by successful interventions in the preschool years disappear. Much of what is reported at the meeting with regard to the effects of exposure to violence on young children is self-evident. There is no doubt that being exposed to acts of violence or aggression leave a lasting mark upon the young • This is a time when trajectories of children’s lives begin to crystallize—for 21 in managing their children’s lives: instance, academic failures in this period predict subsequent school failure and mental health problems, and the likelihood of a child who is having problems in elementary school shifting to a more positive pathway decreases dramatically as children move through this age period. • Parents of children in middle childhood who beat the odds spend a lot of time making sure their children get positive opportunities at school and in their communities. • They also work with the schools to help the school get the resources they feel their children and other children need. Understanding why these characteristics are true has been a central question for developmental psychologists for some time now. Several papers at this biennial meeting provide much richer answers and offer programs to change the patterns. As a society we have looked to the schools as our equalizing institution. Children from all backgrounds are suppose to get the kinds of training needed to give them all an equal chance at succeeding in this society. But this is not happening. The papers presented at this conference suggest why: First, schools can not do it alone—although several promising new curricular programs are reported, other papers document the impact of inequality in educational experiences across social class and ethnic group lines. In and of itself this is not new. But most importantly the papers highlight the interaction between school, family, and community influences. For example, papers reporting on the Baltimore Longitudinal School Study13 provide good evidence that school performance differences due to social class result more from out-of-school summertime experiences than from differences in learning rates during the school year. This work points to the importance of schools, families, and communities in supporting their children’s education all year long. Second, with regard to families, several pa14 pers highlight the ways in which parents facilitate school achievement through their impact on motivation and through the kinds of educational experience they provide in the home. But even more importantly, papers at this convention15 document the key role parents play • And if necessary, they actively resist school practices and decisions that are not in the best interest of their children. • They also seek out opportunities in their communities that will improve their children’s lives. Such family practices are especially important for poor and/or immigrant families.16 But they are also especially difficult for such families to actualize because of the other stresses in these families lives and because of insufficient knowledge about this culture, including where to find the opportunities that they think their children need. In addition, accessing these resources is made doubly difficult by discriminatory practices and poor information dissemination. Finally, such families have the added burden of having to socialize their children about prejudice and discrimination. Several papers17 addressing these concerns make it clear how critical a good connection between schools, families, and communities is for children in this age period. Programs like after-school day care can play especially important role in this connection if they support • the parents’ work involvement, • the school’s educational efforts, and • the children’s and family’s efforts to gain access to other community resources. 22 tions—the romantic relationship. A paper in this session19 reports that kindness and reciprocity come to surpass physical appearance and status in adolescents’ romantic relationships. In one study20 it was found that 8th-grade friends are more similar to one another than 6thgrade friends. Thus if the 8th grader’s friends have acting out problems, then the adolescent is likely also to have acting out problems. In another study21 it was concluded that for some adolescents social networks are “stepping stones” to more deviant groups. This is a twoway process—some adolescents both reject and are rejected by other adolescents and institutions—which occurs not just at the individual level but for stable peer groups. In yet another study,22 it was found that victims are often rejected by their peers—moreso than are aggressive students. Young adolescents appear to believe that victims bring their problems on themselves: they are “show-offs,” they ”bad mouth” others, and they “started it first.” Delinquent, drug-using peer groups may become organized street gangs, which are assessed in the Rochester Youth Development Study.23 The Chicago Youth Development Study24 reports that exposure to violence in the community has cumulative deleterious effects on both aggression and internalizing disorders. Most studies of delinquency focus on urban adolescents. But a study25 of delinquency in rural Iowa makes an important contribution. And in addition, another investigation26 of job loss in the sugar production industry in rural Hawaii found that delinquent activity is largely a function of age but also poor coping strategies, negative community climate, and family economic strain. Studies of delinquency have typically focused on males; studies of adolescent pregnancy have tended to focus on females. Prevention is the foremost goal in research on unplanned adolescent pregnancy. Toward that goal, President Clinton launched a national campaign in May to prevent early childbearing. A current report27 from a landmark 30-year follow- Some of the best such programs actively involve the adolescents from these families and communities as positive resources, doing such things as cross-age tutoring, mentoring, and problem-solving. Such programs provide very effective support for the healthy development of children in both middle childhood and adolescence. In contrast, family policies and programs that make it difficult for parents to play this managerial role in their children’s lives contribute to the widening gap in educational outcomes across social class and ethnic group lines. In sum, papers presented at this conference document programs that work—programs that can both sustain the gains made during early intervention programs, reduce the gaps in achievement that develop during this period, and increase the chances of positive outcomes for all American children. Such programs focus on coordinating efforts across family, school, and community resources. Such coordinated efforts are both especially important and maximally likely to be successful during this age period. Diane Scott-Jones On Adolescence Adolescence is a time of extraordinary change. In our society, when children reach biological or reproductive maturity, we place these newly mature persons in a holding pattern called adolescence, where they wait on the threshold of adulthood, preparing for a successful transition into adult life. The SRCD biennial program includes a number of studies of adolescent development, many of them longitudinal, that provide a window on normal developmental trajectories and also a view of events that might jeopardize adolescents’ movement into adulthood. I’ll mention briefly a few of these studies. Typically, peer relationships are a large part of adolescents’ social world. One symposium18 considers an understudied aspect of peer rela23 up of adolescent childbearers in Baltimore tells us that the adolescent mothers in this sample valued education greatly and often continued their education at the same time their children begin formal schooling. Adolescent childbearing influences not only the children but also the adolescents’ parents, who may be thrust into grandparenting earlier than they anticipated. One study28 of the relationship between adolescent mothers and their own mothers found the affective quality to be closer and warmer than when the adolescent was growing up. As with delinquency, studies typically focus on urban adolescent childbearers. But much needed data29 on childbearing in rural adolescents are also being presented. Attention to adolescent childbearing has increased in the recent past, although the highest rate of births to adolescents occurred during the 1950s. The meeting program includes a report30 on the history of adolescent pregnancy programs in the 20th century. Some high schools provide on-site child care to help adolescent childbearers stay in school. Reports from my own research program31 provide data from adolescents in these programs in Chicago and in Philadelphia. The prevention of early unplanned pregnancies and the prevention of HIV infection need to be considered in tandem. One symposium32 focuses on mothers’ and adolescents’ communication about sexuality and AIDS. Adolescent mothers have been singled out in current welfare reform; however, a study33 of mothers in low-income urban communities found that mothers’ age at first birth was not related to dependence on public assistance. Large percentages of both women who delayed childbearing until the early adult years and the adolescent childbearers did not fare well economically. Our meeting highlights many positive aspects of adolescence. Your fellow journalist, Mike Males, author of The Scapegoat Generation, points out that we tend to blame adolescents for society’s problems. But adolescents are much more than the sum of their problems—they are more than delinquents, more than adolescent parents. As developmental researchers and as concerned citizens, we can use the research findings reported at this meeting to increase the likelihood that children will move successfully through the second decade of life and become productive, responsible, and fulfilled adults. Notes 1 Owen, M. T., chair, “Mother-child interaction and cognitive outcomes associated with early child care: Results of the NICHD study.” Poster symposium, session 2-210. Marshall, N. L., & Friedman, S., chairs, “The impact of school-aged children’s after-school experiences on their socioemotional adjustment.” Poster symposium, session 2-162. 2 3 Wright, J. C., chair, “The long-term effects of television viewing.” Paper symposium, session 3-097. Santomero, A., & Wilder, A. A., chairs, “The role of child development knowledge and theory in the design and production of children’s television.” Discussion hour, session 4-066. 4 Posner, M. I., Cognitive neuroscience and the development of attention. Invited address, session 1-096. 5 Dawson, G., Frey, K. S., Panagiotides, H., Self, J., Hessl, D., & Yamada, E. M. Atypical frontal brain activity in infants of depressed mothers: The role of maternal behavior. In poster symposium 1-020. 6 Carlson, M., Earls, F. T., & Gunnar, M. R. Cortisol levels associated with physical, mental, and motor development in institutionally and home-reared Romanian children. In poster symposium 1-020. 7 DiPietro, J. A. Maternal affect and fetal neurobehavioral development. In paper symposium 1-059. 8 Schmidt, L. A. A multiple psychophysiological measure approach to understanding childhood shyness. In paper symposium 3-055. 9 Osofsky, J. D. The impact of community and domestic violence exposure on children. In invited paper symposium 2-002. 10 Shahinfar, A. Patterns of violence exposure and symptomatology: The effect of variations in context of exposure on preschool children’s behavior. In paper symposium 1-069. 11 Fox, N. A., & Leavitt, L. A., chairs, “Measuring children’s exposure to violence: Cross-national and cross-cultural use of the Violence Exposure Questionnaire (VEX).” Paper symposium, session 4-054. 12 Dauber, S., Alexander, K. L., & Entwisle, D. R. To college or not to college? The construction of early educational expectations. 13 24 poster session 3-039. Schulz, M. S. Parental work conditions and children’s school adaptation: Identifying familial moderators and pathways. In poster session 3-039. In paper symposium 1-109. Grolnick, W. S., Gehl, K., & Manzo, C. Resources and school performance. In paper symposium 1-110. Cowan, P. A., & Heming, G. A. Family factors in children’s school adaptation: The development of a “mega-model.” In poster session 3-309. Brody, G. H., & Flor, D. Parenting efficacy beliefs, development goals, parenting, and competence among rural African American children. In paper symposium 2-005. Molfese, V. J., chair, “Environment, parenting social conditions: Family variables influence cognitive and social development of risk and normal children.” Paper symposium, session 4-013. Halle, T. G., Kurtz-Costes, B., & Meece, J. L. Influences of parents and family on academic skills and achievement. In poster session 2218. Ellenberger, K. M., Wisdom, J. M., & Killian, C. M. Early parental aspirations, home environments, and children’s intelligence: A developmental model of academic achievement. In poster session 2-218. 14 Furman, W. C., chair, “Emerging perspectives on adolescent romantic relationships.” Paper symposium, session 4-001. 18 Laursen, B., & Jensen-Campbell, L. A. The nature and function of social exchange in adolescent romantic relationships. In paper symposium 4-001. 19 Buhrmester, D., & Yin, J. A longitudinal study of friends’ influence on adolescents’ adjustment. In poster session 2-227. 20 Cairns, B. D. Adolescent networks: Affiliation, rejection, and aggression. In paper symposium 1-115. 21 Graham, S. H., & Juvonen, J. Ethnicity, perceived control, and peer victimization: A view from middle school. In paper symposium 3-050. 22 15 Kerns, K. A., & Gentzler, A. L. Parent-child supervision in middle childhood. In paper symposium 2-907. Slaughter Defoe, D., Howard, E., & Cooper, R. Parent education and ethnicity as cultural mediators of school adaptation. In paper symposium 1-112. Ramey, S. L., Gaines, K. R., & Webb, M. B. The family ecology from an insider’s view: Intergenerational perspectives on helping children succeed in school. In paper symposium 1-112. Blair, C. B. School adjustment: Risk and resilience in the early elementary grades. In poster session 2089. Nadon, I., & Normandeau, S. Can parents’ involvement with homework moderate the relation between children’s cognitive abilities and school achievement? In poster session 2-218. Thornberry, T. P., & Krohn, M. D. The impact of association with delinquent peers and of membership in street gangs on violent delinquency. In paper symposium 4-017. 23 24 Richards, M. H., chair, “Dangerous neighborhoods, family, and psychosocial adjustment of urban minority youth.” Paper symposium, session 3-017. Scaramella, L. V., & Bryant, C. M. Parental protective influences and gender-specific increases in adolescent delinquency. Chao, W. Family structure, economic pressure, community effect, and adolescent adjustment. Both in paper symposium 2-065. 25 DeBaryshe, B. D. Resiliency factors in youth affected by parental job loss: Coping strategies, parenting practices, and social support. In poster session 3-041 26 Garcia-Sellers, M. J., chair, “School adaptation of Hispanic immigrant children.” Poster symposium, session 2-020. Cardenas, A. M., & Minnett, A. M. Mexican American parents’ expectations: Possible predictors of parental school involvement and children’s academic attitudes. In poster session 2-030. Serpell, R., Sonnenschein, S., & Baker, L., chairs, “Patterns of emerging competence and sociocultural context in the early appropriation of literacy.” Poster symposium 2-163. Mueller, D. P., & Gozali-Lee, E. M. Early school performance of Hmong children in comparative context. In poster session 2092. Wang, B. Family environment, motivation, and self-perceived competence of American and Chinese children. In poster session 2-218. 16 Furstenberg, F. F., & Weiss, C. C. Schooling together: Mutual influences on educational success of teenage mothers and their children. In paper symposium 1-109. 27 Caldwell, C., & Brown, E. Relationship affect among teenage mothers and grandmothers: A test of the intergenerational stake hypothesis. In paper symposium 1-010. 28 Scaramella, L. V., & Conger, R. Early parenthood in a rural cohort of adolescents. In paper symposium 2-194. 29 Rupert, M. L., & Rubovits, D. S. A history of adolescent pregnancy programs in the twentieth century. In paper session 4-067. 30 Marshall, N. L., & Friedman, S., chairs, “The impact of school-aged children’s after-school experiences on their socioemotional adjustment.” Poster symposium, session 2-162. Furstenberg, F. F., & Weiss, C. C. Schooling together: Mutual influences on educational success of teenage mothers and their children. In paper symposium 1-109. Neuman, S. B. “Getting books in children’s hands”: A study of access to literacy. In paper symposium 1-108. Christian, K. Predicting kindergarten academic performance: Interactions among time in child care, SES, and family literacy environment. In poster session 2-218. Brooks, H. S. Promoting academic and social competence in low-income AfricanAmerican children: An after-school intervention project report. In 17 Sangster, N. A. Family support and financial responsibility as predictors of adjustment in adolescent mothers. In poster session 2-040. Sangster, N. A. Adolescent-child care provider relationships as sources of support for adolescent mothers. In poster session 1-140. 31 Sigman, M., chair, “Mother-adolescent communication about sexuality and AIDS.” Paper symposium, session 2-155. 32 Dannhausen-Brun, C. A., Shalowitz, M. U., & Berry, C. A. Challenging the assumptions: Teen moms and public policy. In poster session 2-047. 33 25 About Social Policy Report Social Policy Report (ISSN 10757031) is published four times a year by the Society for Research in Child Development. Its purpose is twofold: (1) to provide policymakers with objective reviews of research findings on topics of current national interest, and (2) to inform the SRCD membership about current policy issues relating to children and about the state of relevant research. PROCEDURES FOR SUBMISSION AND M A N U S C R I P T P R E PA R AT I O N Articles originate from a variety of sources. Some are solicited, but authors interested in submitting a manuscript are urged to propose timely topics to the editor. Manuscripts vary in length ranging from 20 to 30 pages of doublespaced text (approximately 8,000 to 14,000 words) plus references. Authors are asked to submit hard copy and a disk, including text, references, and a brief biographical statement limited to the author’s current position and special activities related to the topic. Three or four reviews are obtained from academic or policy specialists with relevant expertise and different perspectives. Authors then make revisions based on these reviews and the editor’s queries, working closely with the editor to arrive at the final form for publication. The Committee on Child Development, Public Policy, and Public Information, which founded the Report, serves as an advisory body to all activities related to its publication. CONTENT The Report provides a forum for scholarly reviews and discussions of developmental research and its implications for policies affecting children. The Society recognizes that few policy issues are noncontroversial, that authors may well have a “point of view,” but the Report is not intended to be a vehicle for authors to advocate particular positions on issues. Presentations should be balanced, accurate, and inclusive. The publication nonetheless includes the disclaimer that the views expressed do not necessarily reflect those of the Society or the editor. 26 Past Issues Volume VII (1993) No. 1 Canadian special education policies: Children with learning disabilities in a bilingual and multicultural society. Linda S. Siegel & Judith Wiener No. 2 Using research and theory to justify and inform Head Start expansion. Edward Zigler & Sally J. Styfco No. 3 Child witnesses: Translating research into policy. Stephen J. Ceci & Maggie Bruck No. 4 Integrating science and ethics in research with high-risk children and youth. Celia B. Fisher Volume VIII (1994) No. 1 Children’s changing access to resources: A historical perspective. Donald J. Hernandez No. 2 Children in poverty: Designing research to affect policy. Aletha C. Huston No. 3 Developmental effects of lead exposure in children. Johanna Rich Tesman & Amanda Hills No. 4 Resiliency research: Implications for schools and policy. Marc A. Zimmerman & Revathy Arunkumar Volume IX (1995) No. 1 Escaping poverty: The promise of higher education. Erika Kates No. 2 The U.N. Convention on the Rights of the Child: Its relevance for social scientists. Susan P. Limber & Målfrid Grude Flekkøy No. 3 Children who witness violence: The invisible victims. Joy D. Osofsky Volume X (1996) No. 1 Latin American immigration and U.S. Schools. Claude Goldenberg Nos. 2 & 3 Is the emperor wearing clothes? Social policy and the empirical support for full inclusion of children with disabilities in the preschool and early elementary grades. Bryna Siegel Inclusion at the preschool level: An ecological systems analysis. Samuel L. Odom, Charles A. Peck, Marci Hanson, Paula J. Beckman, Ann P. Kaiser, Joan Lieber, William H. Brown, Eva M. Horn, & Ilene S. Schwartz No. 4 Building research and policy connections: Training and career options for developmental scientists. Amy R. Susman-Stillman, Joshua L. Brown, Emma K. Adam, Clancy Blair, Robin Gaines, Rachel A. Gordon, Ann Marie White, & Sheri R. Wynn No. 5 A reconceptualization of the effects of undernutrition on children’s biological, psychosocial, and behavioral development. Ernesto Pollitt, Mari Golub, Kathleen Gorman, Sally GranthamMcGregor, David Levitsky, Beat Schürch, Barbara Strupp, & Theodore Wachs Volume XI (in press) No. 1 Schooling, the hidden curriculum, and children’s conceptions of poverty. Judith A. Chafel No. 2 Fathers’ involvement with children: Perspectives from developing countries. Patrice L. Engle & Cynthia Breaux No. 3 Investigating child care subsidy: What are we buying? Helen Raikes No. 4 Training the applied developmental scientist: Three current models. Celia B. Fisher, Joy Osofsky, Rachel A. Gordon, & Lindsay Chase-Lansdale 27 Social Policy Report is a quarterly publication of the Society for Research in Child Development. The Report provides a forum for scholarly reviews and discussions of developmental research and its implications for the policies affecting children. Copyright of the articles published in the Report is maintained by SRCD. Statements appearing in the Report are the views of the author and do not imply endorsement by the Editor or by SRCD. Editor: Nancy G. Thomas phone: (970) 925-5516 · fax: (970) 544-0662 e-mail: [email protected] Subscriptions available at $12.50 to nonmembers of SRCD, single issues at $4.00, and multiple copies at reduced rates. Write or phone: SRCD Executive Office • University of Michigan • 505 East Huron, Suite 301 • Ann Arbor, MI 48104-1522 (313) 998-6578 SRCD Executive Office University of Michigan 300 N. Ingalls, 10th Floor Ann Arbor, MI 48109-0406 SOCIAL POLICY REPORT Nonprofit Org. U.S. Postage PAID Permit No. 587 Ann Arbor, MI
© Copyright 2025 Paperzz