Title Other Contributor(s) Author(s) The relationship between diadochokinetic rate and accuracy, reading, rate, and sentence intelligibility in Cantonese speakers with Parkinsonism University of Hong Kong Tsang, Suk-ling, Esther; 曾淑玲 Citation Issued Date URL Rights 2001 http://hdl.handle.net/10722/56247 This work is licensed under a Creative Commons AttributionNonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International License.; The author retains all proprietary rights, such as patent rights and the right to use in future works. The relationship between diadochokinetic rate and accuracy, reading rate, and sentence intelligibility in Cantonese speakers with Parkinsonism Tsang Suk Ling Esther A dissertation submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the Bachelor of Science (Speech and Hearing Sciences), The University of Hong Kong, May 4,2001. Abstract This study examined the relationship between nonspeech tasks (DDK rate and DDK accuracy) and speech tasks (reading rate and sentence intelligibility) in speakers with Parkinson's disease (PD). Eleven speakers with PD, aged between 43 and 78, served as speaker subjects. DDK rate and DDK accuracy were analyzed using perceptual and acoustical analysis. Reading rate was analyzed by calculating syllables per second. For the sentence intelligibility, six naive listeners orthographically transcribed sentences. The results showed a moderate correlation between DDK accuracy (as measured perceptually) and sentence intelligibility, and a moderate correlation between perceptual and acoustic measures of DDK accuracy All other coixelations between DDK rate, reading rate, sentence intelligibility and DDK accuracy (perceptual and acoustic) were nonsignificant. It appeared that the nonspeech tasks (DDK rate and DDK accuracy [as measured acoustically]) were not a very good predictor of speech tasks, at least for sentence intelligibility and reading rate. Possible explanations of thefindingsare discussed. i Introduction Parkinson's disease (PD) is a neurologic syndrome caused by changes in certain parts of the extrapyramidal system. The most obvious change is depigmentation of the substantia nigra, with a loss of dopamine (Darley, Aronson, & Brown, 1975). The major feature involved in extrapyramidal disease is hypokinesia, or limitations in movement. A decrease in the range, rate, and mobility of movements; the presence of rigidity and rest tremor; and a loss of automatic movement are characteristics of hypokinesia (Darley, et al., 1975). These abnormal movements in neuromuscular structures lead to the speech disorders found in these patients. The speech disorders associated with PD have been well-documented. Darley et al. described "monopitch, monoloudness, reduced stress, imprecise consonants and variable rate, etc." (1975, p. 193). Imprecise consonants and variable rate are the focuses of the current study. Canter (1965) and Darley, et al (1975) reported plosive consonants lacked precision, and a discoordination of articulatoiy activity in speakers with PD. Variable diadochokinetic (DDK) rate in speakers with PD was reported by Ackermann, Hertrich & Hehr (1995). Also, Canter (1963) found speakers with PD had variable reading rate. A variety of speech and nonspeech tasks are used in assessing motor speech disorders of neurological patients, including patients with PD. The role of nonspeech tasks in assessment is controversial. Nonspeech assessments sometimes are used to differentiate different neurological patientsfromnormals (Robin, Solomon, Moon, & Folkins, 1997). Also, nonspeech tasks have the advantage of investigating the performance of individual sensorimotor subsystems and the intactness of certain structures that produce speech without the complication of any linguistic demand (Robin et al., 1997). However, some researchers have doubted whether nonspeech i tasks can reflect natural speech production. Kreul (1972) claimed that motor control for speech may not be predicted by nonspeech movements. Also, Weismer & Liss (1991) stated that we cannot assume a relationship between performance in motoric subsystem (as measured by nonspeech task) and overall speech performance. However, Canter (1965) suggested that better neuromuscular performance in nonspeech tasks (e.g. rate of articulators) corresponds to better speech production. Also, Folkins, Moon, Luschei & Robin (1995), suggested that motoric nonspeech tasks may relate to natural speech. Therefore, the predictive value of nonspeech tasks to natural speech assessment is still unresolved (Robin, et al., 1997). Further investigation of the relationships between nonspeech and speech tasks is needed. In the present study, the nonspeech and speech tasks selected to investigate were related to the speech disorders seem in speakers with PD. As mentioned earlier, imprecise consonants and variable rates are two of the most salient speech features of Parkinson's disease (Darley et al, 1975). This study was intended to investigate the relationship between nonspeech tasks (including DDK) and speech tasks (including reading rate and sentence intelligibility), DDK tasks are considered a speech task by some people (e.g. Duffy, 1995) but a nonspeech task by the others (e.g. Robin et al, 1997). In the present study, we will follow the definition of Robin et al. (1997), in defining DDK as a nonspeech task, because DDK does not involve any linguistic content Also, it is believed that in the production of DDK trains, only one structure (e.g. labial) or maximally, the coordination of three structures (labial, tongue tip and tongue blade) are involved. Coordination is thus greatly simplified and is specified to certain movements. Diadochokinesis is defined as 'the maximum speed of movement with which a given reciprocating act can be produced" (Lundeen, 1950, p.54). DDK analysis may 3 include the measurement of rate and accuracy of placement (Forrest & Weismer, 1997) which will be discussed separately. DDK rate is considered a maximum performance test (MPT). It can reflect diminished coordination, range or rate of articulators in clinical neurology (Kent, Kent & Rosenbek, 1987). In the present study, both monosyllabic and trisyllabic DDK sequences were studied because both are widely used in clinical assessment of motor speech disorder, as supplements to the perceptualfindings(Wit, Maassen, Gabreels & Thoonen, 1993; Tjaden, 2000). Apartfromthe advantages of nonspeech task stated above, there are other advantages of using nonspeech tasks, specifically DDK rate. Andrews, Piatt, and Young (1977) reported DDK rate could predict articulatory performance. It might be because DDK rate is sensitive to oromotor deficits (Ackermann, Hertrich & Hehr, 1995). It is also a quantitative measurement which is objective and easy to be carried out. In the present study, DDK rate was analyzed both instrumentally and perceptually. DDK accuracy is defined as accuracy of articulation in DDK trains. In the present study, DDK accuracy involved two parameters: 1/ Target accuracy (TA), using perceptual analysis. 2/ Complete occlusion (CO), using acoustic analysis. There are some advantages of using DDK accuracy, it measures undershooting of articulatory gestures, hence reflects the range of movement of articulators. DDK accuracy also provides invaluable information of dysarthric speech by differentiating neurological groupsfromnormal. Speakers with PD were reported to have significantly lower DDK accuracy than the normal (Ackermann, Hertrich, & Hehr, 1995; Ackermann, Grone, Hoch & Schonle, 1993). Speaking rate is "a measure of the amount of speech produced per unit time, it is frequently expressed in syllables per second" (Tijaden, 2000, p.997). Reading rate is similar to speaking rate but structured laboratory reading materials are used. 4 Reading rate and speaking rate have been studied in dysarthria (Kreul, 1972; Turner & Weismer, 1993). Although reading rate could not be considered the same as natural spontaneous speech (Canter, 1963), reading rate is more easy to control and measure as structured speech materials are used. It provides uniforai speech samples which allow comparsions between individual speakers and groups more easily and reliably (Canter, 1963; Tijaden, 2000). Intelligibility means "the understandability of speech" (Yorkston, Dowden and Beukelman, 1992, p.265). Speech intelligibility in words, sentences, and connected discourse has been widely used in evaluating dysarthric speech (Yorkston et al., 1992). Sentence intelligibility was the focus of this study as it is easier to measure than connected discourse but approximates natural speech more closely than single word production (Ackermann & Ziegler, 1991). In this study, the method of direct transcription was employed to evaluate intelligibility, rather than scaling procedures. Although direct transcription is time-consuming, it is considered more reliable and valid (Schiavetti, 1992). Investigation of the relationships between DDK rate, reading rate, DDK accuracy, and speech intelligibility have been carried out in several previous studies (Canter, 1965; Ackermann, & Ziegler, 1991). However, the results were inconsistent. As we know, DDK rate is widely used in clinical assessment (Darley et al, 1975), as it may predict articulatory competence. Correlations were obtained between DDK rate and speech intelligibility in several studies (Canter, 1965; Dworkin & Aronson, 1986; Buck & Cooper, 1956). In Canter (1965), a strong correlation (0.87) between DDK rate and "clarity of articulation'* (p.221) was obtamed. Moderate correlations between DDK rate and speech intelligibility (0.45-0.59) were reported by Dworkin & Aronson (1986). But in Buck & Cooper (1956), there was no significant correlation 5 between DDK rate and speech adequacy. Differences in the research methods across studies may account for the different results. First, speech intelligibility was defined differently. Buck & Cooper (1956) measured speech adequacy, which may involve many speech parameters, include prosody whereas Canter (1965) and Dworkin & Aronson (1986) measured clarity of articulation. Second, in Dworkin & Aronson (1956), dysarthric speakers with different neurological origins were investigated whereas in Canter (1965) and Buck & Cooper (1956), only Parkinson's patients were involved. As Luschei (1991) stated, it is not certain how DDK is related to speech task. The contradictory evidence arouses interest to further investigate the relationship between DDK rate and intelligibility. Inconsistent correlations between reading rate and DDK rate have also been found (Kreul, 1972; Tiffany, 1980). In Kreul (1972), there was a mild-to-moderate correlation between reading rate and DDK rate while in Tiffany (1980), no significant correlation was obtained between simple repetition rate (including DDK rate) and reading rate. It would seem that DDK rate and reading rates should correlate with each other as they appear measure the same parameter. However, reading rates involves complex factors, such as phonetics and linguistics while DDK rate, which is a kind of repetition rate, is a highly artificial test of simple motor movements (Tiffany, 1980). Given the inconsistency of previous results and the lack of recent investigations of speakers with PD. It seems valuable to further investigate the relationship between DDK rate and reading rate. In addition to DDK rate, DDK accuracy was a concern of the present study. Speakers with PD always undershoot articulator^ target or produce sustained /a/ instead of the target CV syllables than normals, it might be caused by fast DDK rates (Ackermann, Hertrich & Hehr, 1995; Ackermann, Grone, Hoch & Schonle, 1993), 6 Therefore, articulatory accuracy in DDK sequences may also be a valuable tool in assessment, especially in its possible relationship to DDK rate and sentence intelligibility. Ackermann, Hertrich & Hehr (1995) reported a tendency for speakers with PD to have normal DDK rate, but to undershot the articulatory target, while speakers with Friedreich's ataxia had slower DDK rate, but achieved the articulatory target more. This would suggest a relationship between DDK rate and DDK accuracy. However, a statistical of such a correlation has not been reported. On the other hand, a significant correlation (r=0.64) has been found between articulatory accuracy (CO) and severity of overall articulatory impairment (Ackermann & Ziegler, 1991). However, the analysis of undershooting of articulators was done in sentences, but not DDK trains. The present study was intended to extend the above studies. In addition to, perceptual judgment of DDK accuracy (TA) was added, which may be more accessible in clinical practice. Until now the relationships between all four of these factors (DDK rate, DDK accuracy, reading rate, sentence intelligibility) have not been explored. Determining the relationship between these four factors may be helpful in evaluating the value of simple nonspeech tasks (DDK rate and DDK accuracy) in the assessment of motor speech disorders. Nonspeech tasks are time-efficient and widely used by speech pathologists. However, their efficacy in predicting severity, type or even presence of motor speech disorders needs further study. The main objective of this study was to investigate DDK rate, DDK accuracy (TA & CO), reading rate, and intelligibility in speakers with PD. 7 Methodology Subjects The speech samples were previously obtainedfroma project investigating suprasegmental features in Cantonese dysarthric speakers (Whitehall, Ciocca & Yiu, 1999). Eleven speakers with PD (9 males, 2 females) who demonstrated speech problems based on perceptual judgment served as speaker subjects. The age range was 43 to 78 years (mean: 63.8 years). Ten speakers were native Cantonese speakers, whereas one speaker spoke Cantonese, Mandarin and Fukinese. This speaker's Cantonese was judged not to be affected by his other dialects. All speakers passed an aphasia screening test adaptedfromthe Cantonese Aphasia Battery (Yiu, 1992). All speakers could read Chinese and had adequate vision. Nine passed a hearing screening test presented at 20dB HL at frequencies of 500, Ik, 2k, & 4kHz. Two did not complete the hearing screening, but their hearing was judged to be sufficient for the experimental tasks. Six inexperienced listeners were recruited in the study for the sentence intelligibility task. The subjects had normal hearing, and had Cantonese as their primary language. Procedures Speaker Data collection a. DDK tasks The subjects were asked to produce three monosyllables, /pa/, /ta /, and /ka/, and the trisyllable /pataka/, as fast as possible on a single breath. Three trials of each task were taken. Based on perceptual judgment, the fastest and most accurate production among the three trials was selected for analysis (Wit et al., 1993). b. Reading task In the present study, we followed the definition of rate used by Turner & Weismer 8 (1993), in that both articulation time and pause time were included. The subjects were asked to read a standardized passage, the Barbara Streisand passage (gg&jfe Mff) at their habitual rate. The speakers repeated the words produced by the clinician if they did not know or missed a word. c. Sentence tasks Cantonese Sentence Intelligibility Test (Lo, 1999) was used to assess the speech intelligibility of the speakers. The speakers read aloud 22 sentences (two sentences each of 5 to 15 words) selected at random from the master pool of sentence. Recording The recording was done in a quiet room. The speech productions of the subjects were recorded using a digital audio tape (DAT) recorder (SONY TCD-D3 MK II) and a high quality microphone (Breul & Kjaer; type 4003) with microphone to mouth distance of 10cm. Editing Nonspeech tasks a. DDK rate Data of DDK rate was stored on a personal computer. Motor Speech Profile (ver. 1.0; model 5141, Kay Elemetrics, Lincoln Park, NJ) was used in the analysis. b. DDK accuracy (TA& CO) The speech data were stored in individualfileson a Apple Power Macintosh computer for editing. Soimdscope software (ver 1.44; GW Instruments, Cambridge, MA) was employed in editing. The data of DDK accuracy (TA) was then dubbed from the computer onto a MiniDisc recorder (SONY MDS-JB940) for perceptual analysis. DDK accuracy (CO) was analyzed by Soimdscope. Speech tasks 9 a. Reading rate and sentence intelligibility The speech data were stored in individualfileson a Apple Power Macintosh computer for editing. The speech samples were digitized at a sampling rate of 44.1kHz using Sound Designer software. The speech data were then dubbed onto a MiniDisc recorder (SONY MDS-JB940) separately for analysis in listening tasks. Analyses a. DDK rate Thefirstfive seconds were selected for analysis, with the exclusion of the first syllable, which might cause a speech onset effect, since it is usually produced with longer duration (Ackermann et al., 1995). The DDK rate of each DDK train was obtained by dividing the number of syllables produced by the elapsed time (5 sec). The number of syllables produced was obtained by counting the number of peak deflections from the visual display shown on the computer (one peak refers to one syllable), using The Motor Speech Profile (ver. 1.0; model 5141, Kay Elemetrics, Lincoln Park, NJ). Counting was supplemented by perceptual listening. For example, if the judge calculated three peaks based on visual display, the judge listened to the selected waveform portion of the three peaks to ensure three syllables were produced. The mean rates of the three monosyllables and one trisyllable were used to calculate the mean DDK rate. b. DDK accuracy Articulatory accuracy of stop consonant production during DDK trains involved two methods. The analysis involved only thefirstseven stop consonants of each monosyllabic train (Ackermann, Hertrich & Hehr, 1995), and thefirstthree trisyllables. /. DDK accuracy (TA) 10 DDK accuracy (TA) refers to the ratio of number of correct productions of target phonemes (/p, t, k/) to the total number of phonemes produced in monosyllabic and trisyllabic trains. Omissions or substitutions of target phonemes with the other phonemes were counted as incorrect productions. The perceptual analysis was done by two fourth-year students of speech and hearing sciences II DDK accuracy (CO) DDK accuracy (CO) refers to the ratio of number of complete closures of all phonemes to the total number of phonemes in monosyllabic and trisyllabic trains. Incomplete closures of stop consonants are thought to reflect undershooting of the articulatory movements. According to Ackermann & Ziegler (1991), "incomplete closures resultingfromreduced extension of the mandibular, labial, or lingual movements orfromreduced occlusive force may be expected to result in an increase of sound pressure during stop realization" (p. 1094). Complete closures are represented by the appearance of decreased signal amplitude in the oscillogram together with the absence of an audible air-stream (Ackermann, Hertrich & Hehr, 1995, Weismer 1984). The method used in the present study was a modification of the method described by Ackermann, Hertrich & Hehr (1995) and Weismer (1984), where the use of spectrogram was used as well. Complete closures were defined as areas between the end of a syllable and the burst of the next syllable, when no energy was shown in the spectrogram between 2000Hz and 4000Hz within a 10ms period, and there was an obvious decrease of signal amplitude shown on the oscillogram during this 10ms period. This method was supplemented by perceptual judgment c. Reading rate Reading rate was calculated by dividing the number of syllables produced by the reading time (Turner & Weismer, 1993). Reading time was calculated by subtracting 11 the time of the onset offirstsyllable by the time of the offset of the last syllable, using a computer program (Sound Designer Software) to assist with time calculations. The number of syllables that the speaker produced was counted perceptually. Portions where the clinicians spoke were cut. Judges only had to identify the omissions and additions in readings and then adjusted the syllables count. d. Sentence intelligibility The Cantonese Sentence Intelligibility Test is similar in construction and administration to the CAIDS (Yorkston, Beukelman & Traynor 1984). Each listener heard two hundred and forty-two sentences (22 sentences eachfromeleven speakers). Three minidiscs were prepared for the listening task. Each minidisc was listened to by two listeners; speech samples were randomized manually for each disc. 24 sentences were repeated for intra-judge reliability. Before the task began, the listeners heard ten sample sentences which were not counted in the analysis, it was hoped this would decrease listeners' adjustment time to the task (Lo, 1999). The listening was done in a quite room. The intensity level of the speech samples was adjusted to a comfortable level by each listener. The listeners heard each sentence, then orthographically transcribed the sentences. Pausing to write down the sentences was allowed. The listeners could hear each sentence twice, but not more than two times. Guessing at the words that listeners were uncertain of was encouraged. The listeners were recommended to have a rest when necessary in order to maintain their concentration level Sentence intelligibility was calculated by dividing the number of words correctly transcribed by the total number of words for all sentences. The intelligibility scores were obtained by taking the mean scores across intelligibility scoresfromthe six listeners. Statistical analysis 12 The correlations between all measures were done either by Pearson's product moment or Spearman Rank Correlation Coefficients. The decision for using parametric or nonparametric statistical procedures was made separately for each correlation. Parametric statistical procedures (Pearson's correlation coefficients) were employed when heterogeneity of variance was acceptable, and when the criterion of normal distribution was met, as determined by Kolmogorov-Smirnov single-sample test. Results Reliability Pearson's correlation coefficient was used to determine all inter- and intra-judge reliabilities. For sentence intelligibility, interjudge reliability was obtained between pairs of judge's scores. The interjudge and intrajudge correlation coefficients for all pairs of judge were strongly significant (see Appendix A and B). Interjudge reliability coefficients ranged from 0.85 to 0.97. Intrajudge reliability was obtained by the listeners rewriting 24 sentences selected randomly from the samples. The correlation coefficients rangedfrom0.9 to 0.98. For DDK accuracy (TA), the inter-judge reliability was obtained by measuring all the speakers' samples whereas intra-judge reliability was obtained by calculating two random selected speakers' samples. Both reliabilities were high (above 0.99) (refer to Table 1 for details). The interjudge and intrajudge reliability of DDK rate, DDK accuracy (CO) and reading rate were calculated from two random selected speakers. For reading rate, both the inter- and intra-judge had 100% agreements. For the DDK rate and DDK accuracy (CO), both the inter- and intra-judge correlations were above 0.96 (refer to Table 1). 13 Table 1. Inter- and Intra-iudse reliabilities of DDK rate. DDK accuracy (TA). and DDK accuracy (CO) using Pearson's Correlation Coefficients DDK rate DDK accuracy (TA) DDK accuracy (CO) Intra-judge 0.99** 1.0** 0.97** Inter-judge 0.99** 0.99** 0.96* °**p<0.0001 *p<0.0005 ~~ "~ ""~~" — - DDK accuracy (TA) The mean DDK accuracy (TA) was 87.54% and the standard deviation was 18.09. The percentages scores ranged from 53.3 to 100 (refer to Table 4). However, nearly half of the speakers (5/11) had 100% accuracy. DDK accuracy (CO) The mean DDK accuracy (CO) was 44.67% and the standard deviation was 35.98. A large range of percentage scores was obtained which varied from 0 to 90% (refer to Table 4). Every speakers exhibited incomplete closures, and only two speakers had DDK accuracy (CO) above 90%. DDK rate All subjects produced the target DDK trains, except speaker 1 and 7, who did not produce DDK trains of/ka/ and /pataka/, respectively. The mean and standard deviation of duration of overall DDK trains were 4.84 and 1.79 respectively (refer to Table 2 & 4 for details). The subjects showed a large range of mean DDK rates varying from 1.47 to 7.81. Subject data was compared with normative data (Tiffany, 1980) for both monosyllables and trisyllables trains. The mean rates for most speakers (9/11) were lower than the mean rates for nondysarthric speakers. One subject (subject 9) had rate similar to the norms and one subject (subject 10) had a higher rate. Significant correlations between DDK rates of/pa/, Hat, /ka/ and /pataka/ were also found (refer to Table 3). This suggested a high degree of internal consistency in DDK rate performance; there is a tendency for those who have slow 14 rate on one syllable to have slow rate on the other syllables. Subject DDK rate DDK rate (ka) 1 (pa) 5.61 DDK rate (ta) 5.76 DDK rate (pataka) 5.81 Overall DDK rate 5.73 2 4.8 4.31 3.98 4.80 4.47 3 5.17 5.79 4.99 3.60 4.89 4 4.20 3.40 3.70 3.99 3.82 5 4.60 4.57 4.81 4.58 4.64 6 1.40 4.66 2.2 3.01 2.82 7 5.19 4.89 4.41 8 0.80 1.80 1.30 2.00 1.47 9 7.19 6.81 6.58 7.60 7.05 10 8.00 8.05 7.39 7.80 7.81 11 6.00 6.59 5.00 5.18 5.69 4.81 Mean rate (syllables/sec) MeanSJD. 2.15 5.15 4.44 4.84 4.84 1.72 1.82 1.86 1.79 7.1 6.2 7.5 Norms* 7.1 Notes: DDK=diadochokinesis 4.83 *from Tiffany (1980). Table 3. Relationship between individual monosyllables, trisyllables and mean DDK rate, using Pearson's Correlation Coefficients /pa/ /ta/ 0.86** /ka/ 0.98*** /pataka/ 0.92** Mean DDK 0.98*** rate *p<0.05 **p<0.001 /ta/ /ka/ 0.90** 0.84* 0.93*** 0.93*** A QQ**# ***p<0.0001 15 /pataka/ 0.96* * * Mean DDK rate Reading rate Readmg rate across 11 speakers ranged from 1.33 to 3.63 syllables per second. The mean reading rate (syllables per seconds) was 2.37 syllables per second and the standard deviations was 0.63 (refer to Table 4). Most speakers (8/11) had reading rates between 2 and 2.9 syllables per second; only a few exhibited significantly low or high rates. Sentence Intelligibility The mean intelligibility score was 90.08 with a standard deviation of 6.78. The speakers showed a limited range of severity with percentage scores ranging from 76.8 to 98.3% (refer to Table 4). About 64% (7/11) of speakers had intelligibility scores above 90%. Table 4. Skeleton table of the data collection Subject 1 MeanDDK Readmg rate DDKaccuracy DDKaccuracy Sentence rate (syll/sec) (syll/sec) (TA)(%) (CO) (%) intelligibility(%) 89.9 56.7 0 5.73 2.32 2 4.47 2.94 96.7 90 92.5 3 4.89 2.24 100 90 98.3 4 3.82 2.79 96.7 53.3 88.7 5 4.64 3.63 100 40 92.0 6 2.82 2.00 86.21 0 94.5 7 4.83 1.33 100 71.43 95.0 8 1.47 1.57 53.3 0 78.0 9 7.05 2.53 100 60 93.5 10 7.81 2.40 100 73.3 91.7 11 5.69 2.32 73.3 13.3 76.8 Mean 4.84 2.37 87.54 44.67 90.08 S.D. 1.79 0.63 18.09 35.98 6.78 Notes: DDK rate=diadochokinetic rate, DDK accuracy (TA)=target accuracy, DDK accuracy (CO)=complete occlusion. 16 Relationships between variables All the relationships between DDK rate, DDK accuracy (TA & CO), reading rate and sentence intelligibility were obtained using Spearman rank-order Correlation Coefficients. The results of all correlations are shown in Table 5. Relationship between rate measures There was no significant correlation between DDK rate and reading rate (rs = 0.17,p=0.61). Relationship between DDK measures There was no significant correlation between DDK rate and DDK accuracy (TA) (rs =0.43, p=0.19) or between DDK rate and DDK accuracy (CO) (rs = 0.33, p= 0.33) A moderate significant correlation was found between DDK accuracy (TA) and DDK accuracy (CO) (rs=0.74, p<0.01). Relationships with sentence intelligibility There was no significant correlation between sentence intelligibility and DDK rate (rs= 0.06, p=0.85). There was no significant correlation between sentence intelligibility and reading rates(rs=-0.18,p=0.59). No significant correlation between sentence intelligibility and DDK accuracy (CO) (rs=0.52, p=0.11) was found. A moderate correlation was found between sentence intelligibility and DDK accuracy (TA) (rs=0.65, p<0.05) Other Relationships There was no significant correlation between reading rate and DDK accuracy (TA) (rs=0.27, p=0.42) or between reading rate and DDK accuracy (CO) (rs=0.27, p=0.43) 17 Table 5: Spearman's Correlation Coefficients of DDK rate, reading rate. DDK accuracy (TA & CO) and sentence intelligibility DDK rate Reading DDK DDK Sentence rate accuracy accuracy intelligibility (TA) (CO) DDK rate Reading rate DDK accuracy (TA) DDK accuracy (CO) Sentence intelligibility p<0.05 p<0.01 0.17 0.43 0.33 0.06 0.27 0.27 -0.18 CV-C .'5 0.52 Discussion Folkins et al. (1995) stated that "we do not know which nonspeech tasks will generalize to the control of speech until we test them" (p. 142). This study showed that some nonspeech tasks can generalize to speech tasks but some cannot. Moderate significant correlations were obtained between sentence intelligibility & DDK accuracy (TA) (rs=0.65) and DDK accuracy (TA) & DDK accuracy (CO) (r, =0.74). However, no significant correlations were obtained between DDK rate & sentence intelligibility, DDK rate & reading rate, DDK rate & DDK accuracy (TA & CO), reading rate and sentence intelligibility, reading rate & DDK accuracy (TA & CO), & sentence intelligibility and DDK accuracy (CO). The results showed that the nonspeech tasks (DDK rate and DDK accuracy [CO]) might not be good predictors of speech tasks, at least for sentence intelligibility and reading rate. The present study can be compared with the other studies investigating similar correlations. No significant correlations were obtained between DDK rate and sentence intelligibility in the present study. The current result was consistent with Buck & Cooper (1956), who found no significant relationship between DDK rate and 18 articulator^ proficiency But the present results were different from those obtained by Canter (1965) and Dworkin & Aronson (1986). The differences in the results may be due to the use of different experimental methods. First, a scaling method was used in those studies to measure articulately proficiency or speech intelligibility, Sentence intelligibility using direct transcription may be more reliable and more sensitive to articulation deficits. Although those studies was also intended to rate intelligibility, whether the scores obtained from scaling procedures measuring articulatory proficiency is truly a measure of 'intelligibility5 is doubted. Potentially variables like suprasegmental factors (pitch, prosody) are difficult to control Scaling may represent a global judgment of severity of speech, unlike using direct transcription in measuring sentence intelligibility. seemed to have only mild intelligibility. Second, the speakers in this study The use of direct transcription may not be sensitive enough in detecting subtle, small but real differences among the speakers (Yorkston & Beukelman, 1980). Different intelligibility measuring methods might be sensitive to different severity groups of patients (Yorkston & Beukelman, 1980). Therefore, it is believed different methods in rating intelligibility used in previous studies and in this study might contribute to the differing results. Third, the listeners employed were different in terms of experience. In the studies of Canter (1965) and Dworkin & Aronson (1986) study, intelligibility was rated by experienced speech pathologists. In the present study, naive speakers were employed. As Beukelman & Yorkston (1980) suggested, "speech pathologists may overestimate intelligibility scores because they are more familiar with the speech patterns of dysarthric speakers than unfamiliar speakers" (p.37). Therefore, the sentence intelligibility obtained in the present study might even be higher if rated by experienced speech pathologists. However, using naive listeners may have more value since the assessment of 19 intelligibility by them may be more representative of daily-life communication (Barkmeier, Jordan, Robin & Schum, 1991). No significant correlation was obtained between DDK accuracy (CO) and sentence intelligibility. Thisfindingwas differentfromthe study of Ackermann & Ziegler (1991), who found a significant correlation between perceived severity of overall articulatory impairment and undershooting of articulatory gestures. Both studies aimed tofindwhether articulatory imprecision correlates with intelligibility. The inconsistency in results may be for several reasons: First, as explained above, rating of articulatory performance, which is possibly less sensitive, was used by Ackermann & Ziegler (1991). Second, undershooting of articulatory gesture was measured in different contexts. Ackermann & Ziegler (1991) measured undershooting of consonants in sentence, which had variable phonetic contexts whereas the present study measured undershooting of consonants in DDK trains, the phonetic contexts was restricted to vowel /a/. Therefore, different nature of contexts, like the possibility of coarticulatory effects in sentences might affect articulatory precision which may cause less undershooting. Apartfromcontext, different rates might also account for the differences. Rate was controlled in the study of Ackermann & Ziegler (1991), where both undershooting and intelligibility were measured under the speaker's normal speaking rate. But rate was not controlled in the present study, the speakers needed to speak quickly in DDK task, but not required in reading sentences. No correlation was found between DDK rate and reading rate in the present study. Thefindingwas consistent with thefindingof Tiffany (1980) who found no significant correlations between maximum repetition rate (including DDK rate) and reading rate. In the present study, there was also no significant correlation between 20 DDK rate and reading rate (rs=0.17, p=0.61). Although Kreul (1972) found a significant correlation between DDK rate and normal reading rate, only mild correlation was found between DDK trains /pa/ and /ka/ with reading rates (r=0.48 to 0.50). In the present study, nonspeech tasks (DDK rate and DDK accuracy [CO]) had no significant correlations with speech tasks (sentence intelligibility and reading rate). Significant correlations between speech and nonspeech tasks were found only for DDK accuracy (TA) and sentence intelligibility. Canter (1965) suggested that if a subject performs a nonspeech task poorly, then it is reasonable to suspect there is a deficit in the motor control that will also affect speech production. However, in this study, it seems that subsystem motor unit control in nonspeech task does not necessary correspond with the control units of the speech system. This would support the view that "one cannot use nonspeech motor tasks as a window into speech motor control processes" (Weismer, cited in Folkins et al, 1995, p. 141). There are several reasons for poor generalization of most of our nonspeech tasks to the speech tasks. First, speech tasks involve many possible combinations of movements of many structures. Therefore, the movement of intact structures can usually compensate for deficits in the movements of impaired structures (Folkins et al., 1995). Moreover, deficits in the movements of impaired structures in the connected speech tasks can also be compensated by other cues, like contextual and suprasegmental cues. For example, Dongilli (1994) found that understandability of test words was better in sentences than in isolation, showing the presence of additional infomiation increases speech understandability. Also, the presence of various phonetic contexts in speech may provide other compensations, like coarticulations. In contrast, no compensation 21 of the impaired movement can be found in DDK tasks, either by intact movement of other structures or by contexts, because DDK only involves one (/pa, ta, ka/), or three movements (/pataka/). This may explains the nonsignificant correlations between DDK rate & sentence intelligibility and DDK accuracy (CO) & sentence intelligibility. As shown in table 4, some speakers with low DDK rate (e.g. speaker 2,3,4,5,6,7,8) and low DDK accuracy (CO) (speakers 1,4,5,6,8) still had fairly high intelligibility. Although DDK is claimed to reflect articulatory impairments and neutral intactness of the motor system (Robin et al., 1997), compensation in connected speech tasks may overcome the articulatory deficits. Second, the nonspeech tasks (DDK rate and DDK accuracy) are maximum performance tasks used to assess the maximum rate and range of repetitions in syllabic sequences. However, "very few speech task call forth full diadochokinetic ability "(Lundeen, 1950, p.58). Also, as Kent, Kent, & Rosenbek (1987) point out, maximum measures might not be relevant to natural speech since "speaking under ordinary circumstances does not tax the performance capabilities of the speech system" (p.382). Folkins et al. (1995) and Soloman, Robin & Luschei (2000) pointed out that subjects with poor performance in nonspeech tasks may still perform many speech tasks accurately since the normal ranges of oromotor structures and functions in speech is much smaller than in nonspeech tasks. It is interesting to note that one speaker (speaker 8) who had the slowest DDK rate and lowest DDK accuracy (CO) had comparatively low intelligibility score and reading rate. Perhaps significant problems in connected speech could only be observed when a critical level of deficit in the performance of nonspeech tasks (DDK rate and DDK accuracy [CO]) is obtained. However, two speakers (1 & 6) with very low DDK accuracy (CO) (0%) but comparatively faster DDK rate (compared with the slowest DDK rate) had high 22 intelligibility. Perhaps poor performance in intelligibility may only occur when there is both slow DDK rate and low DDK accuracy (CO). This might explain why poor intelligibility is seen in speaker 8, but not speakers 1 & 6. Multivariate analysis techniques may be important in studying the relationship between speech and nonspeech tasks. Third, DDK is different from natural speech in that DDK trains are composed of repetitive syllables or trisyllables. Speech is not just composed of simple sequences of sound, but consists of always changing prosodies involving complex, simultaneous articulations (Tiffany 1980). This might further account for the nonsignificant correlations between DDK rate and reading rate. Fourth, the nonspeech tasks used in this study (DDK rate and DDK accuracy) may not tap the control processes of speech tasks (sentence intelligibility and reading rate). Moore, Smith & Ringel (1986) proposed that the control strategies for orofacial structures in nonspeech tasks may be different from those required for speech. More specifically, they reported that there were different EMG activation patterns ofjaw activity in nonspeech and speech tasks. It is believed nonspeech tasks that mimic the control processes of speech are more likely to generalize to speech tasks (FolMns et al, 1995; Barlow & Netsell, 1986). Therefore, the significant correlation of DDK accuracy (TA) with sentence intelligibility may be explained by the motor process in these two tasks being closer. Clinical Implications DDK, has been regarded as useful nonspeech assessment tool in motor speech disorders. In the present study, we have found no significant correlation between DDK and the speech tasks of sentence intelligibility and reading rate. In contrast, DDK accuracy (TA) had a positive correlation with sentence intelligibility. 23 Increasing attention should be placed on the use of DDK accuracy (TA) as an assessment tool in motor speech disorders in clinical practice. Although DDK rate and DDK accuracy (CO) have little predictive value for sentence intelligibility and reading rate, it still has its importance in the diagnosis of the motor speech disorders (Canter, 1965; Ackermann, Hertrich & Hehr, 1991). Also, it aids in examining the subsystem of speech motor system, it helps tofindout the underlying primary motor impairment, eliminating the possibility of compensation of the impaired structure by the other structures in speech tasks (Wit, Maassen, Gabreels & Thoonen, 1993). In sum, the combined use of nonspeech and speech tasks in the assessment of motor speech disorder is essential to understand the integrity of speech motor system. Further Investigation In this study, as most of the speakers had high intelligibility scores, the results are more likely to be representative for the mild group of speakers with PD only. Therefore, it is recommended that a wider range of severity of speakers should be examined in the future studies. Further investigation between other nonspeech tasks and other speech tasks in a variety of neurological groups is suggested. In order to find more stronger correlations, selection of nonspeech tasks that are closer to the motor requirements of the speech task is suggested (Folkins et al, 1995; Barlow & Netsell, 1986). For example, modifications of DDK tasks can be made with producing the movement at rate and force similar to speech-tasks. Also, different kinds of intelligibility, such as word intelligibility is suggested. In this way, the factor of compensation by providing contextual cues in sentence intelligibility can then be eliminated. Finally, ongoing investigation on the 'threshold5 of weakness in nonspeech task, together with multivariate analysis, is suggested, with the hope that simple nonspeech 24 motor tasks may make great contributions to our understanding of assessment in motor speech disorder in the future. References Ackermann, ft, Grone, B.F., Hoch,G, & Schonle, RW. (1993). Speech freezing in Parkinson's Disease: A kinematic analysis of orofacial movement by means of electromagnetic articulography. Folia Phoniatrica, 45, 84-89. Ackermann, H., Hertrich, L, & Hehr, T. (1995). Oral diadochokinesis in neurological dysarthrias. Folia Phoniatrica et Logopaedica, 47,15-23. Ackermann, H., & Ziegler, W. (1991). Articulatory deficits in Parkinsonian dysarthria: An acoustic analysis. Journal ofNeurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 45, 1093-1098. Andrews, G., Piatt, L.I, & Young, G. (1977). Factors affecting the intelligibility of cerebral palsied speech to the average listeners. Folia Phoniatrica, 29, 292-301. Barkmeier, J., Jordan, L.S., Robin, D.A., & Schum, R.L. (1991). Inexperienced listener ratings of dysarthric speaker intelligibility and physical appearance. In C. Moore, K.M. Yorkston & D.R. Beukelman (Eds.), Dysarthria andapraxia ofspeech: Perspectives on management (pp. 15-27). Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes. Barlow, S. W., & Netsell, R. (1986). Differential fine force control of the upper and lower lip. Journal ofSpeech and Hearing Research, 29, 163-169. Beukelman, D.R., &Yorkston, K.M. (1980). Influence of passage familiarity on intelligibility estimates of dysarthric speech. Journal of Communication Disorders, 13, 33-41. Buck, I E , & Cooper, IS. (1956). Speech problems in parkinsonian patients undergoing anterior choroidal artery occlusion or chemopallidectomy. Journal ofthe American Geriatric Society, 4, 1285-1290. Canter, G.J. (1963). Speech characteristics of patients with parkinson's disease: 26 I. intensity, pitch, and duration. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 28, 221-229 Canter, G.J. (1965). Speech characteristics of patients with Parkinson's disease: HI. Articulation, diadochokinesis, and over-all speech adequacy. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 30, 217-224. Darley, F.L., Aronson, A.E., & Brown, J.R. (1975). Motor Speech Disorders. Philadelphia: WB Saunders. Dongilli, P.A, Jr. (1994). Semantic context and speech intelligibility. In J. Till, K.M. Yorkston, & D.R. Beukelman. (Eds.), Motor speech disorders: Advances in assessment and treatment (pp. 175-191). Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes. Duffy, J.R. (1995), Motor speech disorders: substrates, differential diagnosis and management. St Louis: Mosby. Dworkin, P.J., & Aronson, A.E. (1986). Tongue strength and alternate motion rates in normal and dysarthric subjects. Journal of Communication Disorders, 19, 115432. Folkins, J.W., Moon, J.B., Luschei, E.S., Robin, D.A., Tye-Murray, N. & Moll, KX. (1995). What can nonspeech tasks tell us about speech motor disorders. Journal of Phonetics, 23, 139-147. Forrest, K., & Weismer, G. (1997). Acoustic analysis of dysarthric speech. In M.R. McNeil (Ed.), Clinical management of sensorimotor speech disorders (pp. 63-80). New York: Thieme. Kent, R.D., Kent, J.R, & Rosenbek, J.C. (1987). Maximum performance tests of speech production. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 52, 387-387, Kreul, EX (1972). Neuromuscular control examination (NMC) for parkinsonism: Vowel prolongations and diadochokinetic and reading rates. Journal 27 of Speech and Hearing Research, 15, 72-83. Lo, A.C-Y (1999). Intelligibility and acceptability measurement of Cantonese dysarthric speech. Unpublished undergraduate dissertation. Hong Kong: The University of Hong Kong, Department of Speech and Hearing Sciences. Ludlow, C.L., Connor, N.P., & Bassich, C J. (1987). Speech timing in Parkinson's and Huntington's disease. Brain and Language, 32, 195-214. Lundeen, D.J.(1950). The relationship of diadochokinesis to various speech sounds. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 15, 54-49. Luschei, E.S. (1991). Development of objective standards of nonspeech oral strength and performance: An advocate's views. In C. Moore, K.M. Yorkston, & D.R. Beukelman (Eds.), Dysarthria andapraxia of speech: perspectives on management (pp 15-27). Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes. Moore, C.A., Smith, A., & Ringel, R.L. (1988). Task-specific organization of activity in human jaw muscles. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 31, 670-680. Robin, D.A., Solomon, N.R, Moon, J.B., & Folkins, J.W. (1997). Nonspeech assessment of the speech production mechanism. In M.R. McNeil (Ed), Clinical management of sensorimotor speech disorders (pp. 49-62). New York: Thieme. Schiavetti, N. (1992). intelligibility. Scaling procedures for the measurement of speech In R.D. Kent (Ed.), Intelligibility in speech disorders. Philadelphia: John Benjamins Publishing Company. Solomon, N.P., Robin, D.A., & Luschei, E.S. (2000). Strength, endurance, and stability of the tongue and hand in parkinson disease. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 43, 256-267, Tiffany, W.R. (1980). The effects of syllable structure on diadochokinetic and 28 reading rates. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 23, 894-908. Tjaden, K. (2000). A preliminary study of factors influencing perception of articulatory rate in parkinson disease. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research 43, 9974010. Turner, G.S., & Weismer, G. (1993). Characterises of speaking rate in the dysarthria associated with Amyotrophic Lateral Sclerosis, Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 36, 1134-1144. Weismer, G. (1984). Articulatory characteristics of parkinsonian dysarthria: Segmental and phrase-level timing, spirantization, and glottal supraglottal coordination. In M.R. McNeil, J.C. Rosenbek & A.E. Aronson (Eds.), Dysarthrias: Physiology, acoustics, perception, management (pp 101-129). San Diego: College-Hill Press. Weismer, G., & Liss, J.M. (1991). Reductionism is a dead-end in speech research: Perspectives on a new direction. In C. Moore, K.M. Yorkston & D.R. Beukelman (Eds.), Dysarthria andapraxia of speech: Perspectives on management (ppl5-27). Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes. Whitehill, T.L., Ciocca, V, & Yiu, E.M-L. (1999). Suprasegmental contributors to intelligibility and acceptability judgments of Cantonese dysarthric speech. Hong Kong Research Grants Council. Wit, J., Maassen, B., Gabreels, FJ.M.& Thoonen, G. (1993). Maximum performance tests in children with developmental spastic dysarthria. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 36, 452-459. Yiu, E.M-L. (1992). Linguistic assessment of Chinese-speaking aphasics: Development of a Cantonese Aphasia Battery. Journal ofNeurolinguistics, 7, 379-424. 29 Yorkston, K M , Beukelman, D.R. & Traynor, CD (1984). Computerized Assessment of Intelligibility of Dysarthric Speech. Austin, TX: Pro-ED. Yorkston, KM, Dowden, P., & Beukelman, D.R. (1992). Intelligibility measurement as a tool in the clinical management of dydarthric speakers. In R.D. Kent (Ed.), Intelligibility in speech disorders: Theory, measurement, and management (pp. 265-285). Philadelphia: John Benjamins. Yorkston, K.M. & Beukelman, D.R. (1980). A clinician-judge technique for quantifying dysarthric speech based on single-word intelligibility. Journal of Communication Disorders, 13,15-31. 30 Appendix A: Interiudge reliability of sentence intelligibility using Pearson's Correlation Coefficients Judges 1 1 2 3 4 5 6 2 3 4 5 6 Mean scores 0.69 0.86 0.72 0.77 0.77 0.88 0.76 0.96 0.79 0.81 0.80 0.85 0.81 0.94 0.91 0.85, p<0.001 0.94,p<0.0001 0.88,p<0.0005 0.92, p<0.0001 0.97, pO.OOOl 0.96, pO.0001 AppendixB: Intrajudge reliability of sentence intelligibility scores using Pearson's Correlation Coefficients Judges 1 1 2 3 4 5 0.97 2 3 4 5 6 0.92 0.97 0.98 0.90 6 0.98 All correlations significant at p<0.0001 31
© Copyright 2025 Paperzz