Autism research news and opinion 22 July 2014 - 29 July 2014 For the latest news, visit SFARI.org on the Web Common mutations account for half of autism risk Jessica Wright 21 July 2014 Common gene variants that have minor effects may contribute about half the risk of developing autism, according to a study published Sunday in Nature Genetics1. Identifying these variants would require tens of thousands of samples. Genetic risk: Using multiple statistical models, researchers have attributed 59 percent of the risk of developing autism to genetics. Much of autism research so far has focused on rare, de novo mutations, which appear spontaneously in individuals with autism. These mutations often have strong effects and so can be straightforward to find — for example, by comparing the genomes of the affected individuals with those of their unaffected family members. By contrast, common variants are present in 5 percent or more of the population. Alone, each variant may have little effect on an individual, but taken together they can tip the scales toward a particular condition, such as autism. Because of their prevalence, definitively linking a particular variant to a disorder requires tens of thousands of samples. Rather than pinpoint individual variants, the new study looked at how much common variants contribute to autism risk overall. The results suggest that roughly 49 percent of the risk of developing autism can be attributed to common variants, versus 3 percent for rare, de novo variants. “De novo mutations are extraordinarily important, but we need to consider this other kind of inherited risk as a critical part of the [genetic] architecture,” says lead researcher Joseph Buxbaum, director of the Seaver Autism Center at the Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai in New York City. Family ties: Several studies have tried to define exactly how much of autism risk can be attributed to inheritance. These studies often analyze the genomes of twins because identical twins share the same DNA. However, they don’t always adequately account for the fact that twins also share other factors — such as the in utero environment, their homes or pediatricians. “In the world of heritability, which is something of a dark art, twin studies are the darkest of the arts,” says Stephan Sanders, assistant professor of psychiatry at the University of California, San Francisco. Perhaps as a result, estimates from twin studies of genetics’ contribution vary widely — ranging from 90 percent in twin studies in the 1980s to only 37 percent in a controversial 2011 twin study. The new study falls in the middle, estimating the contribution of genetics overall at around 59 percent. The researchers looked at autism risk across all children born in Sweden between 1982 and 2007, including 5,689 diagnosed with autism, in a total of 1.6 million families. The researchers pieced together pedigrees of extended families to an unprecedented level, from firstdegree relatives (parents, siblings) to ninth-degree distant cousins. By looking at autism recurrence across these families, they calculated that about 52 percent of autism risk is inherited. In the world of heritability, which is something of a dark art, twin studies are the darkest of the arts. The “magic” of this analysis is that the distant relatives — many of whom don’t even know each other — are less likely to share a home or other environmental confounds than siblings are, says Kathryn Roeder, professor of statistics at Carnegie Mellon University in Pittsburgh. To identify the role of common variants, the researchers looked at more than 500,000 variants shared among 3,046 unrelated individuals in the population. From this, they estimated that common variants contribute about 49 percent of autism risk overall. That suggests that only the remaining 3 percent of risk comes from rare, inherited mutations. These calculations are estimates, with sizable error rates. A study of Swedish twins published earlier this month similarly pegged the risk from inherited genetics at 54 percent, however, boosting the numbers’ credibility2. “By using a variety of different analytical approaches, the researchers still come up with the same figure of around 54 percent,” says Louise Gallagher, professor of child and adolescent psychiatry at Trinity College, Dublin, who was not involved in the study. “The strength [of the new study] is the large population sample.” Spontaneous risk: The new study also calculated the contribution of de novo mutations — which are not included in estimates of inherited risk — at about 3 percent. This calculation may be an underestimate, say experts. For example, the analysis includes only mutations found in the coding portion of the genome. Still, the relatively low contribution from de novo variants belies the importance the field has given to them so far, says Dan Arking, associate professor at the Institute of Genetic Medicine at John Hopkins University in Baltimore, who was not involved in the study. “This is bringing people back to the reality that common variation is explaining most of autism risk,” he says. Together, all these genetic factors (along with a previously published estimate of recessive mutations) add up to 59 percent of autism risk. Although other studies have designated the remainder as ‘environmental’ risk, the researchers say this category should be considered “unaccounted.” This is because the analysis does not account for interactions between risk factors — between common and rare variants, for example, or the influence of environment on gene expression. De novo mutations may in fact act as a ‘second hit’ that pushes a set of common variants toward autism, says Buxbaum. “The inter-relationship between common and rare variation and inherited and de novo variation is going to be the big thing for the next few years,” he says. In any case, it’s clear that large numbers of samples can transform autism research — as they have in work on schizophrenia. Just five years ago, schizophrenia researchers had access to only a few thousand genomes — enough to show that common variants are important in the disorder, but not enough to identify any particular variant — says Benjamin Neale, assistant professor of analytic and translational genetics at Massachusetts General Hospital. But after analyzing nearly 40,000 schizophrenia genomes, an international consortium of researchers identified more than 100 common variants associated with schizophrenia — 83 of which are new links to the disorder. The researchers published their results yesterday in Nature3. “The kind of trajectory we’ve seen in schizophrenia suggests what the future of autism genetics may hold — if we make a strong commitment as a community to increasing sample sizes,” says Neale. News and Opinion articles on SFARI.org are editorially independent of the Simons Foundation. References: 1: Gaugler T. et al. Nat. Genetics Epub ahead of print (2014) PubMed 2: Sandin S. et al. JAMA 311, 1770-1777 (2014) PubMed 3: Schizophrenia Working Group of the Psychiatric Genomics Consortium. Nature Epub ahead of print (2014) Abstract Autism gene affects brain function early in development Jessica Wright 28 July 2014 Mutations in a gene linked to intellectual disability and sometimes autism may lead to a permanent boost in brain activity beginning early in development, according to a study published 18 June in Neuron1. Signal strength: Brain slices from mice lacking one copy of the SYNGAP1 gene (bottom) are more excitable (red) than those from controls (top). The gene, called SYNGAP1, prevents connections that send activating signals in the brain from maturing too early during the first few weeks of fetal development. Mutations that inactivate one of two SYNGAP1 gene copies are a relatively common cause of intellectual disability but also frequently lead to epilepsy and, in about 30 percent of cases, autism. In 2012, the same researchers showed that loss of one copy of this gene results in abnormally strong signals and overexcitable brains in mice2. The boost in activating signals may lead to an imbalance between inhibitory and excitatory activity, which some researchers have suggested as a cause of autism and epilepsy. The new study suggests that mutations in SYNGAP1 cause permanent damage to the brain in utero. Restoring SYNGAP1 in adulthood does not improve symptoms in mice missing a copy of the gene. Conversely, removing it in adulthood does little damage to their brains and behavior. “You can lose one copy of SYNGAP1 in adulthood, that’s fine,” says lead researcher Gavin Rumbaugh, associate professor of neuroscience at the Scripps Research Institute in Jupiter, Florida. “But you need full expression of the protein in the developmental critical period in order to have cognition develop and emerge properly.” The results may seem discouraging for developing treatments. But they suggest that repairing the mutation in utero may completely mitigate its effects. “If you had a compound that enhanced SYNGAP1 back up to normal in the critical period, you would essentially cure the disorder,” says Rumbaugh — although he notes that putting this strategy into practice is decades away. Excitatory effects: The study also looked at where in the brain a SYNGAP1 mutation might cause the most damage. Pyramidal neurons, which send excitatory signals in the forebrain — a part of the brain responsible for most cognitive function — are most affected. “Understanding which neuron types are important will help us to understand mechanism and how these disease genes are working in specific neurons,” says Kimberly Huber, professor of neuroscience at the University of Texas, Southwestern, who was not involved in the study. SYNGAP1 has other links to autism as well. For example, it regulates expression of the autism-linked gene MEF1. And it is a member of a family of genes that dampen activity of the RAS pathway, many of which, when mutated, also have links to autism. To home in on the specific effects of SYNGAP1 mutations, the researchers engineered a series of mouse models that lack one copy of SYNGAP1 either in certain cells or at certain times in development. Deleting one copy of SYNGAP1 in the entire mouse brain leads to social deficits, seizures and problems with learning and memory. Deleting the copy only in pyramidal neurons has the same effect, underscoring the importance of these neurons. By contrast, mice missing one copy of SYNGAP1 in inhibitory neurons of the forebrain look normal. “This is a nice, simple demonstration that you don’t need to have deficits in inhibition to have excess of excitatory activity,” says Vikaas Sohal, assistant professor of psychiatry at the University of California, San Francisco. Being able to focus only on excitatory circuits gives researchers a defined target when it comes to treatments, he adds. The researchers also engineered mice that express normal levels of SYNGAP1 in pyramidal neurons but are missing one copy in the rest of the brain. This alleviates the mice’s problems with memory and learning but not their seizures. This finding is intriguing because it is often difficult to know whether cognitive deficits are a symptom in their own right or are the result of frequent seizures, says Huber. “As far as I know, that’s the first time someone has been able to dissociate a seizure phenotype from cognitive phenotypes,” she says. “This suggests there are different circuits that are mediating these two different types of behaviors.” The study also found that enhanced signaling in pyramidal neurons tracks perfectly with memory problems in the mice. This suggests that researchers should correct this brain signal instead of watching for changes in behavior, which are harder to assess, says Rumbaugh. “Now the next phase of the project is to try and figure out how to make these animals better.” News and Opinion articles on SFARI.org are editorially independent of the Simons Foundation. References: 1: Ozkan E.D. et al. Neuron 82, 1317-1333 (2014) PubMed 2: Clement J.P. et al. Cell 151, 709-723 (2012) PubMed Severity metric helps studies address autism's variability Kate Yandell 23 July 2014 A new method for measuring severity in disorders such as autism can help researchers correct for the widely varying autism symptoms among study participants, according to a paper published 2 May in NeuroImage1. The method, called population characterization of heterogeneity (PUNCH), lets scientists combine the results of multiple tests into a single severity score for each individual. Using severity scores, they can divide their samples in new ways, allowing them to detect subgroup characteristics that might otherwise remain hidden. Clearer contrast: Differences between the brains of people with autism and those of controls become more apparent as autism severity in the sample increases (from left to right). Researchers first created PUNCH as part of a larger project to understand differences in brain images between people with autism and controls. They hoped to find new ways to parse their sample, the better to reveal differences between the groups. People with autism have widely varying intelligence quotients, language abilities and levels of repetitive behavior and social difficulties, among other core autism symptoms. This can make it difficult to home in on the exact differences between people with autism and controls. For PUNCH’s proof of concept, the researchers analyzed unpublished test results collected in the broader research program. They used results from nine tests measuring 50 traits, including social responsiveness, communication, attention and anxiety, in 370 adolescent males with autism and 118 controls. They converted the scores into percentiles of performance in comparison with that of others. The researchers then calculated how well the scores for each trait distinguish between people with autism and controls, and weighted them accordingly to produce an overall score. A person who has more traits that distinguish well between autism and typical development gets a higher overall severity score than a person with traits that are less distinctive. The researchers next used PUNCH scores to help analyze brain images from 69 people with autism and 54 controls. They found that brain differences between people with autism and controls intensify with autism severity. When the researchers discarded data from people with the lowest PUNCH scores, or the least severity, they saw more differences than when they looked at the whole autism group. The researchers say that using the new severity metric as a filter for their analyses will allow them to see trends that might otherwise be obscured by mild cases of the disorder. They are applying PUNCH to a much larger collection of brain images. News and Opinion articles on SFARI.org are editorially independent of the Simons Foundation. References: 1. Tunc B. et al. Neuroimage 98, 50-60 (2014) PubMed Why inferring autism's causes from epidemiology is dangerous Mayada Elsabbagh 29 July 2014 In the past few years, I’ve seen several reports that suggest that health disparities in autism are related to social factors such as culture, race, ethnicity and socioeconomic status. Misleading clues: An early study of autism in Africa looked only at people of high socioeconomic status, but some researchers mistakenly assumed that only this group was susceptible to the disorder. For example, claims abound that a mother’s ethnicity is associated with genetic differences that predispose her children to autism. Or that her country of birth means she was exposed to harmful environmental factors that may lead to autism in her children. Or even that the stress associated with immigration increases her risk of having a child with autism. Most recently, a study published earlier this month reported that in Los Angeles County, children born to mothers who immigrated from Central or South America, the Philippines or Vietnam have an elevated risk of autism. It also concludes that African-American and Hispanic children have higher rates of autism than do Caucasian children1. At first glance, these ‘just-so’ stories resemble well-established associations between some single-gene disorders and certain races or ethnic groups — for example, the link between Tay-Sachs disease and the Jewish population. But it is far more challenging to identify and explain such disparities in conditions such as autism, which result from multiple genetic and environmental risk factors. While working on a World Health Organization-commissioned review of the global prevalence of autism, I came across many other just-so stories linking factors such as ethnicity, nativity and race to the prevalence of the condition2. But association does not imply causality, and prevalence data cannot be used to infer underlying genetic, biological or environmental differences. Out of Africa: I was particularly struck by a claim in the review that autism is a rare or even nonexistent condition in Africa. I was even more surprised to find the origins of this claim in a misinterpretation of Victor Lotter’s pioneering and insightful case series in Africa3. A well-known epidemiologist in the 1970s, Lotter traveled to a number of African countries and described cases of autism that looked remarkably similar to what he had seen in his home country, the U.K. Lotter is often misquoted as suggesting that there is a lower prevalence of autism in Africa than in the U.K., or that autism is associated with high socioeconomic status. He was in fact open about the fact that he had looked at only a small group of people of high socioeconomic status. He attributed this to the fact that these families were probably more likely than others to seek help in urban clinics. As we put together the puzzle pieces of race, culture and biology as risk factors for autism, it is worth making sure that we aren’t perpetuating this type of misunderstanding. If we do, we risk repeating the past mistakes of social Darwinism, which attributes individual differences in intelligence, personality and cultural and social characteristics to a genetic basis. For example, African-Americans as a group may score consistently lower on tests of intelligence than other ethnic groups, even after controlling for a wide range of social and economic confounds4. This has led some people to attribute these results to innate (genetic) differences between races. But prominent critics of this perspective have challenged not only the quality of the evidence on which these claims are based but also their underlying assumptions5. Mistakenly attributing racial differences [in autism risk] to biology offers a convenient excuse for political apathy. Complex social phenomena cannot be reduced to measurable concepts such as intelligence quotients. And the association of these measures with race does not imply causality. The lower test scores could be a result instead of biases in the tests themselves, which in the case of IQ tend to reflect the person’s ability to take tests in general. What’s more, the skills measured in these tests are by no means culturally universal. Mistakenly attributing racial differences to biology offers a convenient excuse for political apathy, in lieu of sustained efforts to eliminate health and social disparities where possible. In autism, many studies have similarly tried to link autism risk or prevalence to race, ethnicity and country of origin. The stories often lead in two curious directions: Mothers take the lion’s share of the blame. And the increase in autism risk and severity is seen mostly in non-Caucasians, or those who are born outside the U.S. or Northern Europe. In the past decade, social and advocacy pressures have revealed many disparities in autism prevalence. Indeed, in our global review we found that prevalence estimates are highly variable across geography and culture. Others’ findings suggest differences in severity of symptoms or in functioning across racial groups. We and others have attributed this variability to a range of social factors that influence the measurement of autism prevalence. These include broadening of the diagnostic criteria of the condition, the rise in awareness, improved identification and stronger advocacy, alongside the many methodological differences in prevalence studies. If differences in prevalence across diverse groups were truly the result of genetics, they would be immutable. Instead, prevalence estimates are highly variable and amount to snapshots within a given community at a certain time period. False assumptions: The most comprehensive evidence from the U.S. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention confirms that prevalence across racial groups in the U.S. is a moving target. The pattern of change suggests a ‘catch up’ in diagnosis in groups of individuals who were initially underdiagnosed. This makes prevalence estimates powerful advocacy tools to signal the unmet needs of various subgroups. Further problems with claims about differences in prevalence relate to the validity of their constructs. For example, how is ‘foreign birth’ a biologically meaningful construct? Who or what is the person foreign to? Where did she come from? Did she choose to leave her home country or was she driven out by natural or political circumstance? Similarly, U.S.-defined race categories are limited in capturing the complexity of individual differences both in biology and in culture. For example, ‘black’ encompasses African-Americans alongside immigrants from African countries who are a socioculturally distinct group. Neighboring Canada has dozens of government-recognized ethnic categories that are not collapsible into the U.S. categories, despite the overlap in ethnic origins between the populations of the two countries. More often than not, ‘U.S.-born Caucasian’ is used as a reference group of convenience, rather than one that is logically or statistically justified. Rather than measuring individual differences, we tend to measure how different everyone else is from this ‘prototypical’ Caucasian Anglo-American or European group. What’s more, disparities in access to care and clinician biases are documented phenomena that may be driving the reports of differences in prevalence. And the lack of a typical comparison group in most studies leaves open the possibility that tests used to measure IQ or language skills underestimate these skills in children from certain racial and ethnic groups. If we accept that race and ethnicity are signs of biological differences in autism, this also opens to the door to using a child’s skin color to decide his or her prognosis or treatment. We should instead focus on making access to care more equitable for all children. Rather than relying on frequency counts, studies investigating questions of culture and ethnicity need to formulate solid hypotheses based on well-grounded assumptions and the highest-quality data. For now, evidence suggests that where these disparities exist, they are unlikely to relate to underlying causes. As with social Darwinism, our epidemiological just-so stories may inadvertently have socially and ethically questionable implications. The reality is that what underlies these stories is probably as complex as life itself. References: 1: Becerra T.A. et al. Pediatrics 134, e63-71 (2014) PubMed 2: Elsabbagh M. et al. Autism Res. 5, 160-179 (2012) PubMed 3: Lotter V. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 19, 231-244 (1978) PubMed 4: Herrnstein R. and C. Murray (1994) The Bell Curve: Intelligence and Class Structure in American Life New York: Free Press 5: Gould S.J. (1981) The Mismeasure of Man New York: W.W. Norton Guest blog: Schizophrenia milestone holds lessons for autism Alan Packer 21 July 2014 A long-awaited report on the largest-ever study to link common variants to the risk of developing schizophrenia appeared today in Nature1. This so-called genome-wide association study (GWAS) confirms what some investigators have been arguing for years: Given cases and controls numbering in the tens of thousands, researchers can identify a substantial number of common genetic risk factors for a complex neuropsychiatric disorder. This is a milestone in the study of schizophrenia, and holds several lessons for autism researchers as well. The results are reasonably straightforward. A consortium of dozens of investigators pooled DNA from nearly 37,000 people with schizophrenia and 113,000 controls and looked for associations of 9.5 million single-nucleotide variants across the genome. They found that 128 variants reach genome-wide significance, a level of statistical support that makes false-positive associations unlikely. Of these, 108 map to distinct genomic regions, and 83 are completely new to schizophrenia risk. Jonathan Flint and Marcus Munafò, authors of an accompanying News & Views article2, regard this as an advance “of the sort that rewrites textbooks” — and I agree. Notably, the researchers titled their paper, ‘Biological insights from 108 schizophrenia-associated genetic loci,’ underscoring the point that the genetic findings are only important insofar as they enable a better understanding of the underlying biology of the disorder. So what are the genes and what do they tell us? The researchers highlight pathways that regulate neuronal function, such as glutamate-dependent neurotransmission and voltage-gated calcium channels. They also implicate genes involved in immune functions. In this latter category, the most significant association by far is in the major histocompatibility complex, which encodes proteins that enable the immune system to recognize foreign molecules. Others have made this observation before, but it remains largely unexplained in terms of the biology of schizophrenia risk3. Genes involved in the immune response to pathogens are also on the list, implicating this process in the development of the disorder. Finally, it’s worth noting that many of the associated variants actually lower the risk of developing schizophrenia, which makes these of particular interest as potential drug targets. Additive effects: The subject of drug targets raises the issue that these common variants have small effects, each altering the risk of developing schizophrenia by 10 percent or less. This raises the question of whether these really would be good targets for new drugs. The researchers have one good argument on their side, which is that the list of genome-wide significant hits includes the type 2 dopamine receptor (DRD2). DRD2 is the target of all effective antipsychotic drugs, suggesting that even modest genetic associations can identify drug targets of significant clinical impact. As for autism, the contrast is clear. Although it is apparent that common variation is an important component of autism risk4, 5, we can count the number of variants implicated with a high level of statistical support on one hand. The number of genotyped autism samples is only in the range of 5,000 to 6,000, and if the genetic architecture of autism is anything like that of schizophrenia, this lack of statistical power by itself explains why autism research lags behind in this area. Fortunately, efforts are ongoing to close this gap. The good news from the schizophrenia GWAS is that cases ascertained by physicians rather than research-based assessment using time-consuming methods allowed for a boost in power without introducing what the researchers call “a crippling degree of heterogeneity.” A similarly broad approach to ascertainment of autism samples should speed the accumulation of the sample sizes needed for gene discovery. Finally, the schizophrenia study reveals significant overlap between genes implicated by both common and rare variant approaches. If this turns out to be true for autism as well, it may help us to solve a puzzle: that rare, spontaneous — or de novo — mutations in autism are largely restricted to individuals with below-average intelligence quotients (IQs). As such, despite the success in identifying rare mutations in autism, we still know almost nothing about the genetic underpinnings of the disorder in individuals who have average or above-average IQs. Susceptibility to ‘high-functioning’ autism may be driven by particular constellations of common variants of weak effect. But we’ll only know that for sure if the field examines groups of the scale that schizophrenia researchers have so painstakingly assembled. Alan Packer is senior scientist at the Simons Foundation Autism Research Initiative. News and Opinion articles on SFARI.org are editorially independent of the Simons Foundation. References: 1: Schizophrenia Working Group of the Psychiatric Genomics Consortium. Nature Epub ahead of print (2014) Abstract 2: Flint J. and M. Munafò Nature Epub ahead of print (2014) Abstract 3: Stefansson H. et al. Nature 460, 744-747 (2009) PubMed 4: Gaugler T. et al. Nat. Genet. Epub ahead of print (2014) Abstract 5: Klei L. et al. Mol. Autism 3, 9 (2012) PubMed Guest blog: Rett outcome is improving with time Alan K. Percy 25 July 2014 I first learned about Rett syndrome in 1983, when I read the first Englishlanguage description of 35 cases1,2. So much has changed since then, and I see no reason to think it won’t keep changing. The 1983 paper led to a flurry of clinical studies aiming to understand the specific features of this disorder. In 1999, researchers identified the genetic basis of Rett as mutations in MeCP23. That discovery created a surge in research to identify the fundamental neural underpinnings and to seek potential therapies. An Australian study published in the Orphanet Journal of Rare Diseases in June supports a series of similar previous studies that are revealing remarkable changes in the outcome of women with the disorder, including greater life expectancy and improved overall health4. This is the result of rising awareness of the medical problems that accompany the disorder. And it has led to specific therapies, such as physical, occupational and communication strategies, along with improved nutrition, for these girls and women. For example, we have better information now about symptoms such as impaired growth, gastrointestinal problems such as acid reflux and constipation, and a high risk of choking, or aspiration. This has led clinicians to focus on proper nutrition for women with the syndrome and to aggressively treat their gastrointestinal problems. Before this change in clinical practice, a 1997 study of 805 participants reported that nearly half of the deaths in Rett syndrome occur in women described as frail or debilitated and having frequent aspiration5. The researchers could not identify an immediate cause of death for one-quarter of the participants. However, it is likely that it might have been the result of a sudden catastrophic event such as aspiration or seizure disorder, because of the common occurrence of these problems in this population. (This is purely speculative on my part; the authors do not go into much more detail, but they suggest the same reason.) Better lives: Since then, researchers have looked in detail at the long-term survival of women with Rett syndrome who live in the U.S., Canada, Austria and Australia: The bottom line is that survival rates have improved everywhere. In 2010, we published a study that included nearly 2,000 participants with Rett syndrome from the U.S. and Canada6. At least half of these women lived past 50 years of age. Since 2006, we have an ongoing study now involving more than 900 participants with classic Rett syndrome, whom we examine at least once every year. This project is part of the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development’s Rett Syndrome Natural History Study, which aims to gather natural history data in preparation for clinical trials. In addition to the individuals with classic Rett syndrome, the study includes 166 girls who have atypical Rett syndrome, meaning that they meet only some of the criteria for a Rett syndrome diagnosis. By contrast, a 2010 study looked at the outcome of the original 22 cases diagnosed by Andy Rett in 1966. The probability of survival to age 25 was 21 percent: 19 of 22 Austrian women with Rett syndrome had died by 25 years of age7. However, the same report found that 71 percent of more than 332 Australian women had survived past 25 years — results that echo our findings from the U.S. and Canada. The Australian study published this month, which includes nearly 400 participants followed for up to 20 years, found 71.5 percent survival past 25 years of age — virtually identical results to the previous Australian findings4. From our own Natural History Study, we have identified 35 deaths among 855 participants with classic Rett syndrome and 6 of 157 people with atypical Rett. Only one of these deaths was related to frailty or poor nutrition, yet the possibility of aspiration pneumonia remains. These results strongly support the need for parents to continue demanding aggressive therapeutic approaches in order to preserve optimal health — for example, maintaining proper nutrition, treating gastrointestinal issues and optimizing physical and occupational therapies. It is equally crucial to remain alert to potential medical issues, to continue implementation of strong therapies and to maintain a proper level of engagement with family and peers. Alan K. Percy is professor of pediatrics and neurology at the University of Alabama at Birmingham School of Medicine. News and Opinion articles on SFARI.org are editorially independent of the Simons Foundation. References: 1. Rett A. Wien. Med. Wochenschr. 116, 723-726 (1966) PubMed 2. Hagberg B. et al. Ann. Neurol. 14, 471-479 (1983) PubMed 3. Amir R.E. et al. Nat. Genet. 23, 185-188 (1999) PubMed 4: Anderson A. et al. Orphanet J. Rare Dis. 9, 87 (2014) PubMed 5: Kerr A.M. et al. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 6 Suppl 1, 71-74 (1997) PubMed 6: Kirby R.S. et al. J. Pediatr. 156, 135-138 (2010) PubMed 7: Freilinger M. et al. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 52, 962-965 (2010) PubMed
© Copyright 2025 Paperzz