TASMANIAN QUALIFICATIONS AUTHORITY PLACE LABEL HERE Tasmanian Certificate of Education PSYCHOLOGY Senior Secondary Subject Code: BHP315111 External Assessment 2013 Writing Time: Three hours On the basis of your performance in this examination, the examiners will provide results on each of the following criteria taken from the course statement: Criterion 3 Use evidence to support a psychological view. Criterion 4 Analyse and evaluate ideas and information related to Psychology. Criterion 5 Display knowledge and understanding of psychological concepts and ideas. Pages: Questions: © 16 6 Copyright for part(s) of this examination may be held by individuals and/or organisations other than the Tasmanian Qualifications Authority. Psychology BLANK PAGE Page 2 Psychology CANDIDATE INSTRUCTIONS You MUST ensure that you have addressed ALL of the externally assessed criteria on this examination paper. You must answer a total of THREE questions, ONE question from each section. You must answer each question in a separate answer booklet. All written responses must be in English. Page 3 Psychology SECTION A – HUMAN LEARNING Answer ONE question from this section. You must answer ALL parts of the chosen question. It is recommended that you spend approximately 60 minutes on this section. Use a separate answer booklet for this section. This section assesses Criteria 3 and 5. Question 1 – Conditioning Examine the following stimulus items: Stimulus 1 – Fear of the dentist American and Asian adults who reported high rates of dental fears were asked when their fears began. About 66% replied that they acquired their dental fears in childhood or adolescence, often after a painful treatment (Milgrom et al., 1995; Poulton et al., 1997). These fearful adults reported that the more painful their childhood dental experiences had been, the greater was their fear (Milgrom et al., 1992). Researchers concluded that the majority of dental fears are acquired in childhood or adolescence, often through classical conditioning. In addition, these fears can keep individuals from asking for or receiving dental treatment for future but necessary dental problems (Abrahamsson et al., 2002). (Source: Plotnik, R. (2005) Introduction to Psychology. (7th ed.) Belmont, CA: Wadsworth Thomson Learning, p. 205) Question 1 continues opposite. Page 4 Psychology Question 1 (continued) Stimulus 2 – Learning to talk Day 1 doll duh dat Day 5 Word used doll duh dat Day 10 doll duh dat Day 20 doll duh dat 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Number of times This graph shows the results of an experiment in which a child who is learning to talk points to her doll and says either ‘doll’, ‘duh’ or ‘dat’ when she wants it. Day 1 shows the number of times the child uses each word to ask for the doll. At first, the child uses all three words interchangeably. After Day 1 the parents decide to give the child the doll only when she names it correctly. (Source: Adapted from Coon, D. and Mitterer, J.O. (2010) Introduction to Psychology: Gateways to Mind and Behaviour. (12th ed.) Belmont, CA: Wadsworth. p. 226) (a) Referring to the above stimuli and other relevant information, demonstrate your knowledge and understanding of the following concepts: • • • • (b) conditioned stimulus conditioned response reinforcement schedules of reinforcement Provide evidence to support explanations of how humans learn through classical and operant conditioning. Section A continues over the page. Page 5 Psychology Section A (continued) Question 2 – Observational/Social Cognitive learning Examine the following stimulus items: Stimulus 1 – Learning via imitation Observational learning is one of the most powerful mechanisms of learning. Children observe another person who serves as a model and then imitate what the model does, thus learning how to do something they didn’t know before. For example, a child may see an adult hammer nails into boards and try to duplicate the same feat. Many cultures explicitly encourage such imitative patterns as a way of inducting a child into adult ways. In one Central American society, young girls are presented with miniature replicas of a water jar, a broom, and a grinding stone. They observe how their mothers use the real objects and through constant imitation acquire the relevant skills themselves (Bandura and Walters, 1963). The modern West has a similar practice: toy stores are full of child-sized tools and miniature vacuum-cleaners, cooking sets and dolls that need feeding and nappy changing. We should note, though, that learning via imitation can be quite complicated. In some cases, much of the learning seems to occur quite a bit before the first performance of the learned behaviour takes place. (Source: Adapted from Gleitman, H., Fridlund, A.J., & Reisberg, D. (1999). Psychology. (5th ed.) New York: W.W. Norton & Company, p. 590) Question 2 continues opposite. Page 6 Psychology Question 2 (continued) Stimulus 2 – The link between television violence and aggression Child TV Violence Viewing 0.8 Low Adult Aggression 0.6 Medium High 0.4 0.2 0 –0.2 –0.4 Females Males This graph shows the results of a study investigating a link between the amount of violent television viewed by children and the level of aggression displayed by the same individuals in their adult years. The amount of violence viewed by children was categorised as low, medium or high. The measure of aggression is a composite score that reflects the individuals’ self-ratings and ratings of them by others. Higher scores indicate higher levels of aggression. (Source: Gerrig, R.J. & Zimbardo, P.G. (2005). Psychology and Life. (17th ed.). Boston: Pearson, p. 201) (a) Referring to the above stimuli and other relevant information, demonstrate your knowledge and understanding of the following concepts: • • • • (b) modelling cognitive processes vicarious reinforcement latent learning Provide evidence to support explanations of how humans learn through observational and social cognitive learning. Page 7 Psychology SECTION B – REMEMBERING Answer ONE question from this section. You must answer ALL parts of the chosen question. It is recommended that you spend approximately 60 minutes on this section. Use a separate answer booklet for this section. This section assesses Criteria 3 and 4. Question 3 – Memory Examine the following stimulus items: Stimulus 1 – Baddeley’s working memory model Central Executive Phonological Loop Episodic Buffer Visuo-Spatial Sketch Pad (Source: http://cogpsylab.com/category/theory/iq-strategy) Question 3 continues opposite. Page 8 Psychology Question 3 (continued) Stimulus 2 – The accuracy of memory At first glance, our everyday experience strongly suggests that we can safely rely on our memories, because many of our recollections seem to be as crisp as scenes from films we are watching in real time. Do you remember where you were and what you were doing when you heard about the terrorist attacks on 11 September 2001? Many people say yes, and many say that, even today, they can ‘relive’ those frightening moments with astonishing clarity. Roger Brown and James Kulik (1977) referred to such recollections as flashbulb memories, emotional memories that seem so vivid that people seem able to recount them in remarkable detail. So do flashbulb memories suggest that our memories sometimes operate like video cameras after all? Maybe not. Consider the following story from Ulric Neisser (1982), himself a memory researcher. For several decades, Neisser repeatedly related a flashbulb memory of the bombing of Pearl Harbor in 1941. It was right before his thirteenth birthday, he recalled vividly, and he was listening with great interest to a baseball game on the radio. Suddenly, a newsman interrupted the game to announce that the Japanese had attacked the American naval base in Pearl Harbor, Hawaii. Neisser realised many years later that this memory could not have been correct, because the bombing of Pearl Harbor occurred in December, but the baseball season ends in October. (Source: Adapted from Lilienfeld, S.O., Lynn, S.J., Namy, L.L., Woolf, N.J., Jamieson, G., Haslam, N., and Slaughter, V. (2012). Psychology: From Inquiry to Understanding. Frenchs Forest: Pearson Australia, p. 328) (a) Referring to the above stimuli and other relevant information, analyse and evaluate evidence for at least two different explanations of how memories may be formed and stored. (b) Referring to the above stimuli and other relevant evidence, explain how false memories may be created. Section B continues over the page. Page 9 Psychology Section B (continued) Question 4 – Forgetting Examine the following stimulus items: Stimulus 1 – The influence of context Test environment 14 Land Underwater Mean words recalled 12 10 8 6 4 2 0 Studied on land Studied underwater The graph shows the results of Godden and Baddeley’s (1975) experiment into the effect of context on memory and forgetting. Scuba divers learned materials either while on land, or while underwater. They were then tested in the four possible combinations of learning/recall environments: • • • • Learned on land/tested on land Learned on land/tested underwater Learned underwater/tested on land Learned underwater/tested underwater (Source: Adapted from Reisberg, D. (2013). Cognition: Exploring the Science of the Mind. (5th ed.). New York: W.W.Norton & Company, p. 203) Question 4 continues opposite. Page 10 Psychology Question 4 (continued) Stimulus 2 – Decay Theory Some psychologists suggest that forgetting is a process of decay. Memories that are formed and not used begin to fade, become weaker with time, and eventually decay to the point where they can no longer be recollected – that is, they are forgotten. The concept of decay remains the main way in which forgetting is thought to occur in sensory memory and contributes to the loss of information from short term memory. However, the idea that decay explains the way in which information is forgotten from longterm memory is not as widely accepted. The decay of a memory over time may be the result of deterioration in the connections between the neurons that combine to make up a memory trace. Decay theory suggests that memories that are not retrieved or rehearsed dissipate over time as the synaptic connections between neurons that constitute the memory trace begin to degrade (Schacter, 1999). (Source: Adapted from Rawlings, M., & Skouteris, H. (2004) Psychology Two (2nd ed.) Port Melbourne, Vic.: Heinemann, p. 245) (a) Referring to Stimulus 1, as well as other evidence, explain how context-dependent and statedependent forgetting may occur. (b) Referring to the above stimuli, analyse and evaluate at least three explanations of how forgetting can occur. Include other relevant evidence and examples. Page 11 Psychology SECTION C – PSYCHOBIOLOGICAL PROCESSES Answer ONE question from this section. You must answer ALL parts of the chosen question. It is recommended that you spend approximately 60 minutes on this section. Use a separate answer booklet for this section. This section assesses Criteria 4 and 5. Question 5 – Perception Examine the following stimulus items: Stimulus 1 – Viewing objects and people When we see someone walking away from us, the image of that person projected onto our retina gets smaller. However, we know that the person is not really changing in size. This phenomenon is called size constancy. Objects also appear to change shape as they rotate through space or as we move around them. Shape constancy refers to our ability to recognise that the actual shape of the object is not changing despite changes in the shape of the retinal image. These constancies are important because they enable children to maintain a stable perceptual world as they move about the environment and their retinal images change. Without size constancy, for example, a child watching his mother walk away from him might have trouble recognising her – because she will have appeared to have changed in size. Children would also be very poor at grasping and catching objects without size constancy. Fortunately, much evidence suggests that size constancy emerges soon after birth (Bower, 1965). (Source: Adapted from White, F.A., Hayes, B., and Livesey, D. (2013). Developmental Psychology: From Infancy to Adulthood. (3rd ed.). Frenchs Forest: Pearson Australia, p. 117) Question 5 continues opposite. Page 12 Psychology Question 5 (continued) Stimulus 2 – The Necker Cube B A The top cube can be perceived as if viewed from above (in which case it is a transparent version of Cube A) or as if viewed from below (in which case it is a transparent version of Cube B). Both perceptions fit perfectly well with the information received by your eyes, and so the Necker Cube is compatible with either of these interpretations. Your perception of the cube, however, is not neutral. Instead, you perceive the cube as being similar to either Cube A or Cube B. In other words, your perception goes beyond the information given in the drawing, by specifying an arrangement in depth. (Source: Adapted from Reisberg, D. (2013). Cognition: Exploring the Science of the Mind. (5th ed.). New York: W.W.Norton & Company, p. 78) (a) Referring to the above stimuli and other relevant information, demonstrate your knowledge and understanding of the following psychological concepts: • • • (b) perception perceptual constancies distance and depth cues Analyse and evaluate explanations of how principles of perceptual organisation and interpretation influence an individual’s view of the world. Section C continues over the page. Page 13 Psychology Section C (continued) Question 6 – Consciousness Examine the following stimulus items: Stimulus 1 – Example of circadian rhythms Asleep Awake Asleep Awake Asleep 80 Alertness 40 0 Core body temperature (˚C) 37 36 15 Secretion of 10 growth hormone (nanograms/ml) 5 0 6 o’clock 12 Noon 6 12 Midnight 6 12 Noon 6 12 Midnight Time of day (hours) These graphs show how alertness, core body temperature and the secretion of growth hormone typically fluctuate in a 24-hour rhythm. *nanogram – one billionth of a gram *ml – a millilitre; one thousandth of a litre (Source: Adapted from Coleman, R. (1986) cited in Weiten, W. (2004). Psychology: Themes and Variations. (6th ed.) Belmont, CA: Thomson Wadsworth, p. 180) Question 6 continues opposite. Page 14 Psychology Question 6 (continued) Stimulus 2 – A biological view Some dream researchers argue that dreams are biological phenomena with no meaning at all. According to one such theory, dreams represent interpretations of neural signals initiated in the midbrain during REM sleep (Hobson, 1988; Hobson & McCarley, 1977). Because the initial signals are essentially random, the interpretations rarely make logical sense. Many dream researchers criticise this view, arguing that the presence of dreams during NREM sleep challenges this explanation of dreaming. More recently, biologically oriented researchers have offered another view that emphasises the role of sleep and dreaming in learning and memory. If they are right, the next time you are tempted to stay up all night to prepare for a big exam, think again. Sleep appears to be involved in the consolidation of memory. Memories for newly learned material are stronger after eight hours of sleep than after eight hours of wakefulness. The data suggests that during NREM sleep, the hippocampus ‘replays’ what it has ‘learned’ during the day and activates relevant parts of the cortex to consolidate the memory (Chrobak & Buzsaki, 1994; Wilson & McNaughton, 1994). (Source: Adapted from Westen, D., Burton, L. & Kowalski, R. (2006). Psychology: Australian and New Zealand Edition. Milton: John Wiley & Sons.) (a) Referring to the above stimuli and other relevant information, demonstrate your knowledge and understanding of the following concepts: • • • (b) characteristics of normal waking consciousness measurements of consciousness characteristics of sleep as an altered state of consciousness Analyse and evaluate explanations of why humans dream. Page 15 Psychology This question paper and any materials associated with this examination (including answer booklets, cover sheets, rough note paper, or information sheets) remain the property of the Tasmanian Qualifications Authority. Page 16
© Copyright 2025 Paperzz