Paper, presented at NERA’s 38th Congress, Malmö University, 11-13 March 2010 Susan Tetler & Kirsten Baltzer, DPU, University of Aarhus Students’ opinions about their classroom climate in inclusive settings Susan Tetler & Kirsten Baltzer, DPU, University of Aarhus In the Scandinavian countries we have for decades accepted the idea that our regular schools should include all learners – regardless of the type and degree of their disability. The question, however, is whether this is an ideal rather than an actual practice, as the practice seems often to fall short of the mark. Scandinavian research literature, at any rate, indicates several problematic experiences for students placed in inclusive settings (Emanuelsson, 1998; Dalen, 1999; Tetler, 2000; Nes, 2004; Marinosson, Ohna, and Tetler, 2007). Thus, the huge gap between ideology and reality – and the reasons for this gap - seems to be crucial for the outcome of the efforts of inclusion. It raises questions whether - and how - we judge the ‘quality’ of inclusive settings. Have for instance the efforts in inclusive settings been reduced to a matter of physical placement, depending on the child’s adaptability to the more or less standardised norms of the mainstream school? Or are the efforts characterized by flexibility and comprehensiveness, allowing students with disabilities to be involved as active participants on their own terms? This kind of questions implies a organizational perspective, as we know that the way in which the learning environment is organized for students with disabilities, affects the outcomes (academically, socially and personally) for that group of students (as for other groups of students) (Dyson, Howes, and Roberts, 2002; Ainscow, Booth, and Dyson, 2006; Black-Hawkins, Florian, and Rouse, 2007; Alborz, Pearson, Farrell, and Howes, 2009; Sydney, 2010). Farrell argues for a school-related concept of inclusion, containing four aspects: presence, acceptance, participation and achievement (2004), as he stresses: ‘It is not, for example, sufficient for children to simply be present in a school’ (ibid, p 8). Are schools to be characterized as inclusive, they should also welcome all students and accept them as valuable and active participants of their learning community. Furthermore, all students should be allowed to participate and contribute in all school activities, as well as to learn and to develop positive views of themselves. Thus, the research challenge is how to gain substantial knowledge about what constitutes a quality inclusive setting by examining the four aspects from teachers’, parents’ and students’ views. The study, reported on in this article, concerns the students’ own perceptions. How do they experience their outcomes of being students in inclusive settings, and how do they characterize the learning conditions, which they face? Research on students’ perspectives on learning In the research field of special education we see a lack of research projects taking the student perspective seriously. Gaining more substantial knowledge about increased problems in schools with disengagement, marginalisation and school failure, we need to ask students at risk, how they perceive their role in schools, and what they experience as characteristics of their learning environment. In order to deal effectively with that kind of problems we need to understand their motives, incentives and their will to learn. It is the everyday school life with all its multitude of interactions in learning situations that constitutes the conditions for taking courage, believing in oneself and learning to look upon oneself as important for the community. In this way, the student’s will to learn is related to conditions that would overall be described in terms of learning environment and classroom climate. 1 Paper, presented at NERA’s 38th Congress, Malmö University, 11-13 March 2010 Susan Tetler & Kirsten Baltzer, DPU, University of Aarhus Classroom climate, learning environment and learning management are related topics, which showed up on the educational (research) agenda in the 1960s. Since then, the theme has been a field of research of significant growth and has particularly focused on factors that are supportive of learning. According to a recent research meta-review, ‘there is clear evidence that the quality of the classroom climate is a significant determinant of learners’ achievement. They learn better when they have positive perceptions of the classroom environment’ (Mitchell, 2008, p 103). Three main factors of creating a climate that facilitates learning have been identified (ibid. p 103-104): 1) Relationships (the extent to which people in the classroom support and help each other). 2) Personal development (the extent to which personal growth and self-enhancement is facilitated). 3) System maintenance (the extent to which the classroom is orderly, and educators are clear in their expectations, maintain control and are responsive to change). Mitchell finds that learning environment and classroom climate depict nearly the same factors related to learning in schools, and this view is supported by a review from Danish Clearinghouse for Educational Research (Nordenbo, 2008). This review defines classroom climate as a construct including three indicators: 1) Student activity (the extent to which students are involved in decisions about classroom activities) and motivation (especially self- or inner motivation), 2) Supportive behaviour (the extent to which the teacher involves students in structuring the learning situation and supportive collaboration), and 3) Student/teacher relationship (the extent to which the teacher is supportive in relation to student initiatives and motivation). The wording is slightly different from Mitchell’s, but the content is in fact overlapping. Bronfenbrenner (1986) focuses upon the relation between the learning context and acquisition of personal competences. A learning context is characterized by situational requirements and demands, and development of competence is a result of the interaction between the context and personal attributes (Wentzel, 2006). These are cognitive, self-monitoring, and self-regulating skills as well as personal goals and values. Student experiences are primarily influenced by proximal processes as relations, communication, and interactions (Bronfenbrenner, 1999), and positive experiences are related to the quality of proximal processes. Bronfenbrenner defines these processes as proximal, but they need further elaboration to serve as constructs for operationalization into research tools. The lesson to be learnt from Bronfenbrenner is that tools for our study should include indicators related to the learning context in a broader perspective, learning situations, social relationships between the teacher and student as well as among students, communication and interaction from the interpersonal as from the intrapersonal perspective. Mitchell’s and Nordenbo’s meta-reviews give hints about how to transform Bronfenbrenner’s concept of proximal processes into research tools. However, Nordenbo (2008) mentions the issue of the German traditions with focus on ‘Bildung’, when conducting meta-reviews on classroom management, following the guidelines presented in The Clearing House concept (2006). According to the German tradition, education is rooted in philosophy, and this tradition has been very influential in the Scandinavian countries including Denmark. Nordenbo argues that the ethics and morality included in the philosophical position of ‘Bildung’ and didactics is indeed a necessary perspective. Within this tradition there has been no focus on the consequences for management of the teaching and learning processes, and it is nearly impossible to find studies fulfilling the criteria for inclusion in a Clearing 2 Paper, presented at NERA’s 38th Congress, Malmö University, 11-13 March 2010 Susan Tetler & Kirsten Baltzer, DPU, University of Aarhus House review. The same criticism is relevant concerning Mitchell’s meta-review, as he focuses solely on the classroom’s psychological climate. This criticism has to be considered when creating a research tool based on Bronfenbrenner’s position. In other words, the tool has to reflect the Danish learning context inspired from both the German tradition for centuries and for the International tradition mainly rooted in AngloAmerican traditions and studies, which is an upcoming inspiration. As a consequence it was necessary to seek further inspiration from another meta-review conducted within a German context. The German researcher in didactics Hilbert Meyer (2004) has done a meta-review on good teaching. He finds ten characteristics of good teaching, which include more details about the structure of the teaching process, communication and differentiation. Meyer mentions that it has not been possible to find evidence for communication as characteristic for good teaching in the meta-review. However, he finds it necessary to include communication in the list. Allodi (2007) has prepared and tested a theory-based instrument for assessing the quality of learning environments in Swedish schools. There are many similarities between Mitchell’s definition based on a metareview and Allodi’s theory-based empirical model evaluated from a holistic perspective. However, the higher-order factors (Allodi) and the main factors (Mitchell) combine the lower order factors into different patterns. They fit into Bronfenbrenner’s views as well. Allodi finds that Self-affirmation (including Achievement, Efficacy and Safety) has a central position in evaluating the quality of learning environment. SelfAffirmation has an interpersonal as well as an intrapersonal dimension. Hence, it has been necessary to expand the theoretical perspectives. The concept ‘efficacy’ is rooted in Bandura’s social learning theory. According to Bandura (1994) ‘perceived self-efficacy is defined as people’s beliefs about their capabilities to produce designated levels of performance that exercise influence over events that affect their lives. Selfefficacy beliefs determine how people feel, think, motivate themselves and behave’ (1994, p 71). A strong sense of self-efficacy is a major motivational factor, in school settings as in other settings, where people conduct their lives. There are four main sources to a strong sense of efficacy: Mastery experiences, vicarious experiences provided by social models, social persuasion, and somatic and emotional states. It has to be mentioned that social persuasion is verbal persuasion and includes competence to structure situations for oneself as for other people - in ways bringing success. Part of this competence is to avoid situations or challenges where people are likely to fail. Bandura’s concept of self-efficacy is one of the cornerstones in his social learning theory. Bandura (1994) elaborates a model of personal and social dimensions which sets the stage for an understanding of motivation for learning as a synthesis of self-regulated processes taking place in social interactions. Later on Bandura (1997) developed the construct of collective efficacy which means that groups can develop shared efficacy beliefs. Efficacy is thus an important major dynamic source of motivation for self-monitoring, often mentioned as self-regulated learning (SRL) (McCaslin et al., 2006). Self-Regulated Learning is in focus of the theories about learning prepared by the Russian psychologist L.S. Vygotsky (ibid). Vygotsky has two important foci related to SRL. The first is the focus on the multiple functions of language: Communicative language is transforming thoughts into speech; in contrast language or speech directing the self transforms speech into thoughts – Vygotsky labels it inner speech. He finds that the capacity for self-regulation emerges from the interpersonal regulation mediated by language or speech. From this theoretical stance communication is a key-element of good teaching. The Zone of Proximal Development (ZPD) is a construct closely related to the understanding of self-regulation. ZPD recognizes what learners cannot do themselves, but are able to do with assistance or support, and is a potent target for learning. As a consequence the relationship between the participants in ZPD is central – be it peers, teachers or adult significant others. 3 Paper, presented at NERA’s 38th Congress, Malmö University, 11-13 March 2010 Susan Tetler & Kirsten Baltzer, DPU, University of Aarhus Bandura and Vygotsky have developed important key-concepts which elaborates the understanding of the concept ‘learning environment’. Vygotsky adds richness to Bronfenbrenner’s concept of communication and interactions. Goals and values are mentioned in Bronfenbrenner’s list of personal attributes. Bandura adds richness to the understanding of goals. Following Bronfenbrenner, goals and values are embedded in the school context in curricula and syllabi. Somehow curricula and syllabi should be reflected in the research tool, because they set up a framework for the proximal processes: communication, interactions and relations. Finally, we have to bear in mind, that learning, development of motives and motivation for learning, collective efficacy and self-efficacy emerges intertwined in the work on school subjects within the framework set by curricula and syllabi. Hence, the research tools should reflect the specific school subjects for students in primary education, how these subjects and the social life within school relate to students’ everyday life and how teachers support students in connecting their experiences across important life contexts (Hedegaard and Chaiklin, 2005). Finally, it has to be stressed that the body of research on classroom climate has been investigations of various aspects of classroom climate and how they impact on academic achievement and affective learning, but as Mitchell states: He has ‘not been able to find any research that focuses on learners with special educational needs’ (Mitchell, 2008, p 108). However, the value of giving voice to young people with disabilities is evident, as they have provided many new insights about their experiences, including how they often feel deprived of influence on their own lives and living conditions. They also report loss of competence and opportunity for taking initiatives, making up one’s mind and acting self-dependently (e.g. Ringsmose and Buch-Hansen, 2004; Høgsbro et al., 1999). The challenge for education, then, is to counter processes, in which dependence, passiveness and helplessness seem to get internalized in children and youth with disabilities, and for that purpose it is necessary to learn more about the youngest students’ perceptions and school experiences. Methods The study, dealt with in this paper, has sought to gain a deeper understanding of the concept of educational inclusion in mainstream classrooms and the opinion held by the students involved. It is part of a larger research project, dealing with the learning experiences of 27 students with disabilities (specifically groups of students diagnosed with ADHD, autism, blindness, cerebral palsy, dyslexia, and learning difficulties) in their school settings. Fifteen students were included in mainstream classrooms, while the other 12 students were placed in more segregated settings such as special classes and special schools. All the students were in primary education. The research was funded by the Danish Ministry of Education and wished to explore the policy question of whether or not the resources being directed to special education services were ‘working’ (Egelund and Tetler, 2009). In order to study educational patterns required a systemic approach to capture classroom complexity in a variety of ways (Salomon, 1991). To this end, a wide range of methods was used including semi-structured interviews with focus students (students with disabilities), semi-structured interviews with their teachers and parents, semi-structured interviews about the classroom climate with the focus students as with their classmates (304 in all), collection of individual educational plans, collection of teachers’ diaries with success stories’ and observations of teaching and learning activities in each classroom. In all the research was con- 4 Paper, presented at NERA’s 38th Congress, Malmö University, 11-13 March 2010 Susan Tetler & Kirsten Baltzer, DPU, University of Aarhus ducted as a multi-site, multi-researcher qualitative project (in 24 classrooms across 23 schools distributed among 18 Danish municipalities) (Tetler et al., 2009; Ferguson and Tetler, 2009). This paper, then, deals with the insight gained through semi-structured interviews with those fifteen focus students, who were included in mainstream classrooms, and their class mates. First of all an interview guide was prepared for the diverse student group taking in account recent international meta-reviews about ‘good teaching’, which showed clear evidence that the classroom climate is a significant determinant of learners’ achievement. Especially three factors seem important: Relationships, personal development and system maintenance (Mitchell, 2008). However, when asking students about their own perceptions of their learning environment, meta-reviews can just be a starting point. It has been necessary to transform the content of the three main factors into questions related to everyday classroom practice and adapt these questions to student age and intellectual development. In this process we took inspiration from Norwegian, Swedish and German research related to classroom climate, learning environment and learning contexts (Nordahl, 2005; Westling Allodi, 2005, 2007; Meyer, 2004). Nordahl has done a literature review and learned that there are 8 important factors defining the learning environment. Some of these 8 factors are also mentioned in Mitchell’s model. Nordahl mentions school management and collaboration with parents, but these factors are not relevant for an interview guide for students. Allodi’s GAVIS questionnaire (GAVIS is an acronym for ‘The Goals, Attitudes and Values in School’) has provided inspiration to relate the factors ‘Relationships’ and ‘Personal development’ to classroom practice. Therefore, three of the themes (and the related questions from GAVIS) are included in our interview guides. The themes are ‘Participation’, ‘Responsibility’, and ‘Influence’, while the questions from ‘Helpfulness’ has provided inspiration to statements about ‘Collaboration’ in the interview guide. Mitchell’s factor ‘System maintenance’ is about didactical issues, and the inspiration to this section of the interview guide stems from a meta-review on characteristics of ‘good teaching’ (Meyer, 2004). Meyer found 10 characteristics of good teaching, and four of them are included in the interview guides as themes: ’Physical environment’, ’Clear structure’, ’Meaningful communication’ and ’Differentiation’ (see table 2). A closer look on the descriptions of these characteristics reveals an overlap with the factors ’Relations’ and ’Personal development’. The interviews were conducted as group interviews (and in some cases individual interviews), as they have been run as conversations about statements one by one. However, the answers have been collected on answering sheets designed in a questionnaire format to create a shortcut access to students’ views and opinions. This has been necessary, because some of the informants have no spoken language. The answering categories are inspired from ordinal scales e.g. Likert scales, but the interpretation has been processed according to qualitative methodology, rooted in a qualitative methodology focused on interpretivism (Ferguson and Ferguson, 1995). Findings Table 1 shows the answering patterns for focus students (15 in all) and their mainstream class mates (222 in all), calculated in % of interpretable answers within each group. Each column can be interpreted as below: 5 Paper, presented at NERA’s 38th Congress, Malmö University, 11-13 March 2010 Susan Tetler & Kirsten Baltzer, DPU, University of Aarhus Column A: Column: B Column: C Column: D Column: E Column: F Column: G Column: H Column: I Column: J Column K: Themes and Item numbers Theme and items Students Groups C and F Category 1 very positive answers n % Category 2 positive answers in % Category 1+2 Sum of positive answers in % Category 3 negative answers in % Category 4 very negative answers in % Category 3+4 Sum of negative answers in % Characterisation of answering pattern (See note page 9). Comparison between the C- and F population. Comparison categories similar or different Taking a closer look at as well the C- (class mates) as the F-(focus) students’ response pattern concerning their opinions on their learning environment, some characteristics stand out clearly (see Table 1). Four didactic themes are represented in this interview and the students’ statements are predominantly positive about the themes ‘Clear structure’ (statement 5-8) and ‘Meaningful communication’ (statements 9-12). For instance, except for one student, they all state that their teachers are good at explaining to them what to do (statement 8) and how to progress (statement 11). A more blurred pattern stems from the student responses concerning their physical environment. Especially, we will point out the F-students problems with doing different things without disturbing each other (73% negative answers at statement 3). This is corresponding with the lack of peace to work (statement 5) for nearly half of the students in both groups of class mates and focus students. The F-students are more positive concerning good space and order, and for the majority the situation is not that bad. The C-students have another meaning: they are positive about the space, but turns to the negative side concerning order in classrooms. Looking at the aspects of differentiation, the teaching pattern seems to be characterized by positive answers for both groups. The answers are positive in the interval from 58% to 100% (statements 13-16). Above all it is striking that all students state that it is OK in their learning communities not to be equally good at everything (statement 16). F-students also experience their teachers using a wide range of materials for the students to work with (statement 13), and they are even more positive about that than the C-students. For both groups there is a clear picture of having good relations to their teachers. The four themes ‘Collaboration’, ‘Influence’, Participation’ and Responsibility’ are related to the factors ‘Relationships’ and ‘Personal development’ mentioned in Mitchell’s model. Influence (statements 21-24) is about being able to decide, getting ideas appreciated and experiencing significant others’ attention of how you are doing in school, while participation (statements 25-28) is about being part of and being listened to. Influence seems to be quite a tricky issue for the teachers to leave room for (statements 21-24). Statements 21 and 22 turn to the negative side for F- as well as C- students. Statement 24 turns to the positive side for both groups, and at statement 23 turns the C-students to the positive and the F-students to the negative side. Both F- and C-students’ experiences on participation are more positive with responses on three statements 26-28 turning to the positive side. However, the answers on item 25: In my class some of my peers feel alone is a bit worrying, with only 49% to the positive side for C-students and 43% for the Fstudents. Concerning ‘Collaboration’ (statements 17-20) is the response pattern more positive for C- than for F students (especially on statement 17 and 18). However, statement 20 turns really high to the positive side for both groups. On ‘Responsibility’ (statements 29-32) both F- and C-students express positive experiences. On three statements both groups turn out very positive (29, 31, 32). They are also on the positive side concerning responsibility on what to do in reading and math, but not positive to the same high ranking as on the three other statement within the theme. 6 Paper, presented at NERA’s 38th Congress, Malmö University, 11-13 March 2010 Susan Tetler & Kirsten Baltzer, DPU, University of Aarhus Table 1. Students’ opinions about their learning environment A Item B C Students D YES E yes F D+E G no H NO I G+H J K Comparison: C vs. F Theme 1 Physical environment In my class there is plenty of space C F 29% 20% 57% 47% 86% 67% 13% 33% 1% 0% 14% 33% Pos. Mix. Different 2 In my class things are placed, making it easy for us to find C F 7% 7% 40% 53% 47% 60% 43% 27% 9% 13% 52% 40% Neg. Mix. Different 3 In my class we make different things without disturbing each other C F 7% 7% 39% 20% 46% 27% 45% 53% 7% 20% 52% 73% Neg. Neg. Similar 4 In my class is a computer, which we all can use C F 43% 50% 14% 7% 57% 57% 5% 7% 38% 36% 43% 43% Mix. Mix. Similar Theme 5 Clear structure In my class it’s possible to work without being disturbed C F 4% 20% 45% 20% 49% 40% 39% 7% 12% 53% 51% 60% Neg. Neg. Similar 6 In my class we have rules for how to behave, we have rules for how to behave C F 54% 64% 35% 29% 89% 93% 9% 0% 2% 7% 11% 7% Pos. Pos. Similar 7 In my class we are good listening to each other C F 14% 21% 70% 50% 84% 71% 12% 21% 4% 7% 16% 28% Pos. Mix. Different 8 In my class the teachers are good at explaining what to do C F 48% 64% 45% 29% 93% 93% 7% 7% 0% 0% 7% 7% Pos. Pos. Similar Theme 9 Meaningful communication In my class we speak nicely to each other C F 7% 20% 58% 40% 65% 60% 28% 7% 7% 33% 35% 40% Mix. Mix. Similar 10 In my class we are talking about what’s happening outside school C F 35% 33% 43% 40% 78% 73% 16% 20% 6% 7% 22% 27% Pos. Mix. Different 11 In my class teachers tell us how to get better in reading and math C F 37% 27% 45% 53% 82% 80% 15% 0% 2% 20% 17% 20% Pos. Pos. Similar 12 In my class teachers are good at making fun C F 19% 40% 38% 20% 57% 60% 24% 13% 19% 27% 43% 40% Mix. Mix. Similar Theme 13 Differentiation In my class we have varied materials and things to work with C F 23% 47% 41% 33% 64% 80% 31% 13% 5% 7% 36% 20% Mix. Pos. Different 14 In my class we can choose different tasks to work with C F 21% 29% 41% 29% 62% 58% 29% 29% 9% 14% 38% 43% Mix. Mix. Similar 15 In my class we have plenty of time to do our work C F 16% 20% 51% 40% 67% 60% 15% 7% 3% 13% 18% 20% Mix. Mix. Similar 16 In my class it is ok that we do differently on many things C F 72% 67% 25% 33% 97% 3% 0% 0% 0% 3% 0% Pos. Pos. Similar 100 7 Paper, presented at NERA’s 38th Congress, Malmö University, 11-13 March 2010 Susan Tetler & Kirsten Baltzer, DPU, University of Aarhus A Item B C Students D YES E yes F D+E G no H NO I G+ H J K Comparison: C vs. F Theme 17 Collaboration In my class we do project together C F 14% 20% 69% 33% 83% 53% 15% 33% 2% 13% 17% 46% Pos. Neg. Different 18 In my class we care for each other C F 21% 27% 62% 40% 83% 67% 14% 13% 3% 20% 17% 33% Pos. Mix. Different 19 In my class we are doing many things together in breaks/after school C F 52% 40% 41% 27% 73% 67% 6% 13% 1% 20% 7% 33% Mix. Mix. Similar 20 In my class we support each other C F 36% 33% 57% 60% 93% 93% 6% 0% 1% 7% 7% 7% Pos. Pos. Similar Theme 21 Influence In my class we are involved in decisions about important issues C F 9% 20% 22% 20% 31% 40% 32% 13% 37% 47% 69% 60% Neg. Neg. Similar 22 In my class are involved in decisions about many issues C F 6% 14% 32% 21% 38% 35% 41% 29% 22% 36% 63% 65% Neg. Neg. Similar 23 In my class we make up many ideas that our teachers like C F 12% 21% 44% 14% 56% 35% 35% 29% 9% 36% 44% 65% Mix. Neg. Different 24 In my class we often talk with teachers about how we feel C F 16% 27% 49% 47% 65% 74% 30% 27% 6% 0% 36% 27% Mix. Pos. Different Theme 25 Participation In my class some of my peers feel alone C F 14% 14% 35% 29% 49% 43% 27% 7% 10% 29% 37% 36% Neg. Neg. Similar 26 In my class everyone is feeling good C F 20% 29% 51% 36% 71% 65% 22% 7% 7% 29% 39% 36% Mix. Mix. Similar 27 In my class we all share experiences C F 50% 53% 33% 27% 83% 80% 18% 7% 3% 13% 21% 20% Pos. Pos. Similar 28 In my class we include each other, e.g. when playing C F 29% 33% 57% 40% 86% 73% 11% 20% 3% 7% 14% 27% Pos. Mix. Different Theme 29 Responsibility In my class we are responsible for what to do in class C F 49% 50% 30% 29% 79% 79% 16% 21% 5% 0% 21% 21% Pos. Pos. Similar 30 In my class we are responsible for what to do in reading/math C F 16% 21% 39% 43% 55% 64% 37% 29% 8% 7% 45% 36% Mix. Mix. Similar 31 In my class we share responsibility everyone is feeling good in school C F 33% 33% 47% 27% 80% 60% 15% 40% 5% 0% 20% 40% Pos. Mix. Different 32 In my class the teachers rely on us C F 39% 47% 42% 27% 81% 74% 14% 7% 6% 20% 20% 27% Pos. Pos. Similar 8 Paper, presented at NERA’s 38th Congress, Malmö University, 11-13 March 2010 Susan Tetler & Kirsten Baltzer, DPU, University of Aarhus Comparison between students with disabilities, included in mainstream settings and their class mates The overall aim of this paper is to compare the opinions about the learning environment between two groups of students: Students with disabilities included in mainstream classrooms (labelled Focus students in Table 1 & 2) and their class mates (labelled C-students in Table 1 & 2). Also, the intention is to get a holistic picture of similarities and differences between those two groups of students. To pursue this aim it has been necessary to compile the comprehensive set of data, which has been done in a very pragmatic manner as described below. Therefore, the design of the compiling analysis is based on creating cuts that reveals differences between the two groups … and makes sense considering the big difference in population size and the interview with 32 items. This is done as described in the note below1. Table 2. The answering patterns for Focus students (with disabilities) and their Class mates. Student popu- C-students lation Answering Positive Mixed Negative category Positive FocusStudents Mixed Negative item 6, item 16 item 8 item 20 item 11 item 27 item 1 item 18 item 4 item 14 item 26 item 7 item 28 item 9 1tem 15 item 30 item 10 item31 item 12 item 19 item 17 item 29 item 13 item 24 item 32 item 23 item 2 item 3 item 22 item 5 item 25 item 21 1 For each item we have ascribed a characteristic of the answering pattern (please refer to Table 1, Comparison: C students vs. F students). Taking the small population in account, the pattern design is based on the frequency of positive and negative answers as registered within the F-population. Also we want to secure at least 1 student’s presence in every cell with a figure > 0. By compiling the answering categories into 2 groups we find only 1 cell with 0% in the F-row within the frequency cells. We have chosen to split the answering pattern for each of the item into 3 subgroups: positive pattern, negative pattern and mixed pattern. The cut-off score for the positive pattern is the most positive 5 scores from the F-population, the cut-off score for the most negative pattern is the 5 most negative scores from the F-population, and the mixed scores are placed in the interval in between. This is a very pragmatic mode of dividing into sub groups. The overall objective has been to find ‘a cut’ that reveals differences within the two populations. The chosen cut has this quality, as it matches the full set of 32 items in the sense that the answers are distributed on a scale from very positive to very negative, and furthermore, we provide the possibility to give answers at the extremes as well as near the middle of the scale. It matches the F-population with 14-15 answers on 30 of the 32 items. 5 most positive F student answers: 100-74% items with positive pattern (100, 93, 80, 79, 74%, including 10 items) 5 most negative F student answers: 0-53% items with negative pattern (27, 35, 40, 43, 53%, including 7 items) 15 mixed F student answers: 54-73% items with mixed pattern 9 Paper, presented at NERA’s 38th Congress, Malmö University, 11-13 March 2010 Susan Tetler & Kirsten Baltzer, DPU, University of Aarhus Taking the holistic perspective we find similar answering patterns for F- and C-students in the cells at the diagonal (grey shadow). Altogether we find 20 items in these three cells. The positive and mixed categories are of nearly the same size, but with fewer answers in the negative cell. That picture indicates major similarities in both F- and C-students opinions about their learning environment. Having positive, mixed or negative opinions is not just a simple matter of being in more or less complicated learning situations. Rather, it is a more complex issue, calling for further cross contextual investigations of the combined intra- and interpersonal dimensions of learning communities. In the cells below the diagonal we find 8 answers, i.e. F-students are less positive than C-students. The less positive answers are spread over the whole item set. However, it is a remarkable pattern that C-students are positive on item 17: ‘In my class we do project together’, while F-students are negative. Actually, more than the half part of the F students is negative about this issue. In the cells over the diagonal we find answers where C-students are less positive than F-students. There are three items on that side, which are item 13: ‘In my class we have varied materials and things to work with’, item 24: ‘In my class we often talk with teachers about how we feel’, and item 2: ‘In my class things are placed, making it easy for us to find’ with 52% students giving negative answers. The latter is very interesting and somehow unexpected. The methodology, however, does not allow us to interpret the findings in details. In general, this holistic picture of the learning opportunities for the young students, involved in this study, indicates some mainstream learning communities, which somehow succeed in creating positive student attitudes on a range of dimensions of the classroom climate. However, it should also be stressed that for 25% of the items F-students are less positive than C-students. All in all, looking at the eight thematic categories, no theme can be pointed out as specifically critical. Rather, we can talk about differences in degree, as the F- students concerning all eight interview themes are less positive than their class mates. Conclusion The study, reported on in this paper, concerns the students’ own perceptions of their learning environment. A major segment of a larger data set has been selected to illustrate how students with disabilities and their classmates construct the classroom climate, including their experiences of student membership. In general, (see Table 2) F- and C-students have similar opinions about their learning environment. Moreover, it is a diverse picture with positive, mixed and negative views on different dimensions of the classroom climate. The positive and mixed answers are dominant as well as within and across the F- and C- groups of students. However, the analysis also shows that F-students have a less positive answering pattern, compared with their class mates. Taking a thematic perspective we find the most positive answers on the four didactic themes: ‘Physical environment’, ‘Clear structure’, ‘Meaningful communication’, and ‘Differentiation’, and it holds for both student groups. Also the Themes ‘Collaboration’, ‘Participation’, and ‘Responsibility’ are dominantly positive, while ‘Influence’ seems to be a more tricky issue. Looking on the findings more theoretically (following Bronfenbrenner, 1986; Bandura, 1997; Almquist et al., 2004) the students’ quite positive attitudes towards their school experiences concerning the didactic and social dimensions of schooling are results or outcomes of reciprocal dynamic relationship between personal and environmental factors. The overall picture of environmental dimensions like physical environment, 10 Paper, presented at NERA’s 38th Congress, Malmö University, 11-13 March 2010 Susan Tetler & Kirsten Baltzer, DPU, University of Aarhus clear structure, meaningful communication, differentiation, collaboration, participation and responsibility is positive. This is somehow in line with the theoretical views behind the study, as Bronfenbrenner’s proximal processes (relations, communication and interactions) are included in these environmental dimensions. Both Bronfenbrenner and Bandura pinpoint that beliefs and efficacy is context specific, e.g. characterized by situational requirements and demands. Seen from the perspective of the students is the outcome of their school experiences up to grade 4/5 that the mainstream classroom contexts (set by the school subjects, physical surroundings, need of support from teachers and classmates etc.) have met F- and Cstudents requirements and demands for support. Discussion As mentioned earlier, mainstream school situations of students with disabilities relate to a wide range of issues dealing with disengagement, marginalization and school failure. This seems not to be the case among students in this study, as they have mostly positive experiences of their learning environments. However, this finding should be reflected in conjunction with our students' age, as they are early in their schooling. The question is whether (and in which aspects) students’ opinions on their learning environment will change during schooling. Therefore, it will be obvious (and desirable) to repeat this semi-structured interview for the same student group at three-yearly intervals. Indeed, the research field of educational inclusion is in need of that kind of longitudinal studies, which look more closely at the nature of processes of inclusion and exclusion over the years. Even though, in general, the students with disabilities, included in mainstream class rooms, involved in this study, show up quite positive attitudes towards their learning communities, it also becomes clear, that they are less positive, compared with their mainstream class mates (without disabilities). First and foremost, they seem to lack positive experiences of influence and appreciation. In this way, it is an evident challenge for schools to create learning situations and processes, based on values such as ‘student influence’ and ‘educational participation’, if they are to be characterized as inclusive (see also Farrell, 2004). It is not enough to be cared for by caring professionals; they also need to be active and valuable contributors to the student learning communities, to which they belong; e.g. doing project together with their class mates. It is worrying that two thirds of the students with disabilities do not agree with the statement that ‘in my class we make up many ideas that our teachers like’. When influence on own learning processes (and a lack of appreciation) are withdrawn from students with disabilities, they tend to be treated like objects, rather than subjects – and are likely to develop passiveness and helplessness. As a consequence, successively, they will suppress own wishes and motives … and will present themselves as disengaged and passive. Later on, in schooling or adult life, when asked for active participation and independent making up one’s mind, they have lost competence in acting like that long time ago. Is this trend to be reversed, we must take the educational challenge seriously, as phrased by Per Lorentzen (1998, p. 27): To work with children’s motives and will to learn. If we are going to facilitate for students’ development of autonomy and engagement in own learning processes, we need to involve them in our teaching planning and evaluation, even if they have difficulties in expressing their wishes verbally or putting forward their requests. It requires a lot of creativity, perseverance and empathy to succeed in grasping the students’ will to learn and in transforming it to a sustainable practice. Not least, it requires time for reflections on how to balance so inconsistent processes as support 11 Paper, presented at NERA’s 38th Congress, Malmö University, 11-13 March 2010 Susan Tetler & Kirsten Baltzer, DPU, University of Aarhus and challenges to each individual student, protection and autonomy, security and risky situations (Tetler, 2002). Thus, we need research to further explore the concept of educational participation and the meaning, held by the students involved. References: Ainscow, M., Booth, T. and Dyson, A. (2006) Improving Schools, Developing Inclusion. (London: Routledge). Alborz, A., Pearson, D., Farrell, P. and Howes, A. (2009) The impact of adult support staff on pupils and mainstream schools. In Research Evidence in Education Library. (London EPPI-Centre, Social Science Research Unit, Institute of Education, University of London). Bandura, A. (1994) Self-efficacy. In V.S. Ramachaudran (Ed.), Encyclopedia of human behaviour (Vol. 4, pp 71-81). (New York: Academic Press). Bandura, A. (1997) Self-efficacy: The exercise of Control. (New York: W.H. Freeman). Black-Hawkins, K., Florian, L. and Rouse, M. (2007) Achievement and Inclusion in Schools. (London: Routledge). Bronfenbrenner, U. (1986) Ecological Systems Theory. In R. Vasta (ed.), Annals of child development. (Vol 6, pp 187-250) (Greenwich CT: JAI Press). Bronfenbrenner, U. (1999) Environments in developmental perspective: theoretical and operational models. In S.L. Friedman and T. Wachs (eds.), Measuring environments across the lifespan. (APA: Washington DC). Dalen, M. (1999) Den inkluderende skole – idealer og realiteter. In: En skole for alle I Norden. Et festskrift I anledning af den 20. nordiske kongres i Stavanger: ’100 års nordisk samarbejde omkring undervisning af elever med særlige behov’. Dansk Clearinghouse for Uddannelsesforskning (2006) Dansk Clearinghouse for Uddannelsesforskning /konceptnotat. http://www.dpu.dk/Clearinghouse Dyson, A., Howes, A. and Roberts, B. (2002) A systematic review of the effectiveness of school-level actions for promoting participation by all students. (EPPI-Centre Review, version 1.1) In: Research Evidence in Education Library. London: EPPI-Centre, Social Science Research Unit, Institute of Education). Egelund, N. and Tetler, S. (eds.), Effekter af specialundervisningen. (København: Danmarks Pædagogiske Universitetsforlag). Emanuelsson, I. (1998) Integration and segregation – inclusion and exclusion. International Journal of Educational Research, 2, 95-105. Farrell, P. (2004) School Psychologists: Making Inclusion a Reality for All. School Psychology International; 25; 5. Downloaded from http://spi.sagepub.com at Aarhus Universitets Biblioteker / Aarhus University Libraries on June 24, 2009. Ferguson, D.L. and Ferguson, P.M. (1995) The Interpretivist View of Special Education and Disability: The Value of Telling Stories. In T.M. Skrtic (ed.), Disability and Democracy: Reconstructing (Special) Education for Postmodernity. (New York: Teachers College Press). Ferguson, D.L. and Tetler, S. (2009) Meeting the Challenge of Multi-Site, Multi-Researcher Interpretivist Research. Paper presented at ECER, Vienna, September 28, 2009. Hedegaard, M. and Chaiklin, S. (2005) Radical-Local Teaching and Learning. (Aarhus: Aarhus University Press). Høgsbro, K., Kirkebæk, B., Blom S. V. and Danø, E. (1999) Ungdom, udvikling og handicap. (Frederiksberg: Samfundslitteratur). Lorentzen, P. (1998) Språk og handling. (Otta: Tano Aschehoug). 12 Paper, presented at NERA’s 38th Congress, Malmö University, 11-13 March 2010 Susan Tetler & Kirsten Baltzer, DPU, University of Aarhus Marinosson G., Ohna S.E. and Tetler, S. (2007) Delagtighedens pædagogik. Psykologisk Pædagogisk Rådgivning, 44, 236-263 McCaslin, M., Bozack, R.A., Napoleon, L., Thomas, A., Vasquez, V. Wayman, V. and Zhang, J. (2006) SelfRegulated Learning and Classroom Management: Theory, Research, and Considerations for Classroom Practice. In C.M. Evertson and C.S. Weinstein (eds.), Handbook of Classroom Management. (London: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates). Meyer, H. (2004) Was ist guter Unterricht? (Berlin: Cornelsen Verlag Scriptor). Mitchell, D. (2008) What Really Works in Special and Inclusive Education – Using Evidence based teaching Strategies. (London: Routledge). Nes, K. (2004) Hvor inkluderende er L97-skolen? In K.J. Solstad and T.O. Engen (Eds.) En likeverdig skole for alle? Om enhet og mangfold i grunnskolen. (Oslo: Universitetsforlaget). Nordahl, T. (2005) Læringsmiljø og pedagogisk analyse. En beskrivelse og evaluering af LP-modellen. NOVA rapport 19/05. Norsk Institutt for forskning om oppvekst, velferd og aldring. (Oslo: Nova). Nordenbo, S.E. (2008) En Clearingshouse-undersøgelse. Om regelledelse og relationskompetence. In J.B. Krejsler and L. Moos (eds.), Klasseledelse – magtkampe I praksis, pædagogik og politik. (Frederikshavn: Dafolo). Ringsmose, C. & Buch-Hansen, L. (2004) VUL – Voksne - Udviklingshæmmede – Livsvilkår. (København: Danmarks Pædagogiske Universitetsforlag). Salomon, G. (1991) Transcending the Qualitative-Quantitative Debate: The Analytic and Systemic Approaches to Educational Research. Educational Researcher, 20, 10-18. Sydney, A. (2010) A Handbook for Inclusion Managers. (London: Routledge). Tetler, S. (2000) Den inkluderende skole – fra vision til virkelighed. (København: Gyldendal). Tetler, S. (2002) Skolelivskvalitet i den inkluderende skole. Kognition & Pædagogik, 12, 32-44. Tetler, S., Baltzer, K., Hedegaard-Sørensen, L., Boye, C. and Andersen, G.L. (2009) Pædagogiske vilkår … for elever i komplicerede læringssituationer. In N. Egelund and S. Tetler (eds.), Effekter af specialundervisningen. (København: Danmarks Pædagogiske Universitetsforlag). Wentzel, K.R. (2006) A Social Motivation Perspective for Classroom Management. In C.M. Evertson and C.S. Weinstein (Eds.), Handbook of Classroom Management. (London: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates). Westling Allodi, M. (2005) Specialpedagogik i en skola för alla. Individ, omvärld och lärande. Forskning nr.27. Institutionen för Individ, omvärld och lärande. (Lärerhögskolan: Stockholm). Allodi, Westling M. (2007) Assessing the quality of learning environments in Swedish Schools: Development and analysis of a theory-based instrument. Learning Environments Research, 10, 157-175 Keywords: Educational inclusion, participation, Student voices, Classroom climate Susan Tetler (ph.d.) is Director of the Research Program ‘Inclusive Education and Social Pedagogy’ at the Danish School of Education, Aarhus University, Tuborgvej 164, DK-2400 Copenhagen NV, Denmark. Kirsten Baltzer (ph.d.) is external associate professor at the Danish School of Education, Aarhus University, Tuborgvej 164, DK-2400 Copenhagen NV, Denmark. Correspondence should be addressed to Susan Tetler: email: [email protected] 13
© Copyright 2025 Paperzz