W en .-.cC C'CI .c ...... J- CI) -D:: -::s ....I .-0 en c c:: LL .. -c:c c:: ....I ::) ca C E "C G) - LL ... +' G) .e 0 z 0 ~ '"0'"" ~~ ~ -~ 0 p:: ;.. ~ ~ z ~ CI) I .c ~ C'CI :.., 0 l- ::s ... W 0 .. :::E: >t- -Z CHAPTER 1 Everyday Inventions of Everyday Life T hink about the last time you were lied to. It may take you a minute. Most of the time when I ask people about when they were last deceived, they need a few moments before coming up with something. Eventually, they will recollect the story of the mechanic who overcharged for an unnecessary repair, or the date who promised to call the next day and was never heard from again. Lies that took some kind of emotional or financial toll are the ones that generally spring to mind when we think about the deception we encounter. This makes sense. Events that are painful or dramatic tend to be memorable, and tend also to shape our impressions of the circumstances-like being lied to-that surround them. My guess is that the lie you came up with as the one you most recently encountered involved a blow to either your heart or your wallet. The reality of deception, though, is very different from what such painful memories lead us to believe. Our relationship to lying is far more intimate than the occasional encounter with a duplicitous mechanic or dishonest lover. Think again, more carefully, about the last time you were lied to. Perhaps you 5 THE LIAR IN YOUR LIFE Everyday Inventions of Everyday Life picked up your dry cleaning, and when you thanked the per just as we smile when the man handing us our dry cleaning lies son handing it to you, he responded, "My pleasure"- although about the pleasure he took in laundcring oLlr clothes. there are both of you knew there was almost no chance that doing your lies society accepts, and even encourages. I ndccd, deception is laundry had given him any pleasure at all. Or maybe you 'vvere so deeply ingnlined in the functioning of our societ y that if we in line at the grocery store and you struck up a COJl\TrsatioIl removed it, we might not recognize the society that resulted. \\ith the woman in line ahead of you ..Maybe she told you shc'd \ \'c probably \\"Ouldn't be very comfortable living in it. either. never had to wait in line so long before. Really? The fact is that much of ,vhat we think we know about de Or perhaps the last time you checked your e-mail you were ception is, simply put, not true. Misconceptions too often mask offered a share of a Nigerian inheritance, which you could lying's prominence in our society and the ambiguities that sur claim for only a few thousand dollars in taxes. Or maybe you round its operation. So the first step to understanding the role watched television or listened to the radio, and heard about this of lying in our lives may be to consider the many ways we mis or that product's miraculous or lik-altering virtues. IVfaybe an understand it. in/(nnercial promised you savings, but only provided you "act Amanda and the Seven Dwarfs now. " The truth is, we are lied to frequently, even in the course 1\ lormer student of mine, whom I'll call Gary, moved to of a single day. Most of the lies we don't notice, or don't eveIl Santa Fe a kw years ago to pursue a career in rcal estate. (You'll consider to be deception. Spam e-mail, deceptive advertising, f()rgive me for fudging the names and other minor details in and disingenuous social niceties form almost an omnipresent this story, and in other personal anecdotes that appear in the white noise that we've lcamed to tune out. Regardless of what that lies are a typical feature of our everyday experience. That book. My only concern is to protect the innocent, as they say on Dragnet. The essentials, of course, are the truth, the whole truth, and so forth.) Gary is a devoted marathoner, and he joined a we do disregard so much deception only underlines how com recreational club of runners in his <lre<l. Another member of mon it really is. Lying is not limited to one aspect of our society, one type this club \vas a young woman named Amanda. She and Gary f(.1J in love. of person, or one kind of institution. As we'll sec, lying perme j\lllanda told Gary about her troubled upbriIlging. Her ates the way we get to know one another and the way \ve f(xnl mother had died when she was young, and she was estranged relationships. It is a part of how we educate our children and from her father. As a teenager, she had battled cancer. Gary was how we elect our leaders. It is essential to our economy, and it sympathetic, and impressed at the courage and toughness she is essential to t he media. showed in overcoming these hardships. After they'd dated for a we choose to accommodate or ignore, though, the fact remains lVIore strikingly, while lying sometimes occurs as an aberra little under a year, Gary proposed, and Amanda accepted. They tion in these and other arenas, often its manifestations are the bought an apartment together and started to plan their wedding. rule. \Ve tend to think of lying as something we censure. But In the J(Jllowing weeks, though, Amanda began to complain 6 7 Everyday I nventions of Everyday Life THE LIAR IN YOUR LIFE of feeling fatigued and lethargic. She showed little enthusiasm Now let's consider another incident of deception, one that ft))' putting together an invitation list or selecting a reception is also remarkable but that occurred in a br different context venue. Since this was wholly out of character for the energetic from Gary's broken engagement to Amanda. In 1991, seven top woman he knew, Gary encouraged her to see her ductor-a executives from leading American tobacco companies, among man Amanda trusted and whoIll she had been seeing since col them the CEOs of Philip Morris, Brown & \Villiamson, and lege. Because she was anxious about her doctor's \'isit, Gary U.S. 'I ()bacco, testified before the House Subcommittee on offered to go with her. Health and the Environment. The "seven dwarfs," as they were At the doctor's office, Gary and Amanda described Aman later dubbed, had been called as part of the committee's on da's symptoms. Amanda's doctor said that mononucleosis going efforts to bring attention to the health dangers of ciga sounded like a potential explanation, but he would have to ex rettes. All seven of these men were asked whether they believed amine her. Gary then asked whether a diagnosis of mono was nicotine to be addictive. And each man answered in turn, un more likely based on Amanda's history of cancer. As Gary de der oath, that he did not. The statement "1 believe that nicotine scribed it to me weeks later, the doctor looked at him blankly is not addictive" was repeated again and again. and asked, "Cancer?" \Vhat the seven dwarfs did not volunteer was research Big Amanda had never had cancer. Nor did she ha\,(-' mono. Tobacco itself had done, demonstrating that nicotine, in fact, ~or \Vhat she did have, or so it seems, is mythomania, more com is addicti\'(-'. did they mention that their companies had monly described as compulsive lying. Amanda wasn't estranged made efforts to enhance the addictive powers of the nicotine from her father, and her mother was vcry much alive. Amanda in the cigarettes they sold. Three months after their testimony, had hidden her fairly close relationship with her parents from the Justice Department opened a criminal investigation into Gary, along with a whole host of other facts about her basically whether they had committed per:iury with their "nicotine is not happy and completely healthy background. addictive" assertion. The relationship, and the lies, unraveled quickly but not The stories of Gary and Amanda, on the OJ1e hand, and the llcatly. Gary had to explain to all his friends and family why he sevcn dwarfs of Big Tobacco, on the other, would seem to have shan~ little ill common, other than the fact that they both have lying was canceling his wedding. He had to buyout Amallda's of the apartment, which he ewntually sold, at a loss. And he at their corc. But taken together, these stories provide a fairly had to start a big part of his life all over again. comprehensive picture of how most of us bclic\,(-' deception 1 wouldn't be surprised if you've heard stories similar to functions in our society. Those telling the lies in both stories Gary's. Perhaps you've personally encountered a compulsive seem generally representative of the kinds of people we typi liar and experienced tlw unnerving revelation that everything cally think of as liars: lying, as practiced by Amanda, was an you know about him or her might be f~dse. Compulsive lying is abnormal behavior, one indicative of mental imbalance. She not a common condition, but the anecdotes that accompany it may be an extreme casc, but liars are generally thought of as tend to be remembered. standing outside the norm for social behavior. They are more 8 9 Everyday Inventions of Everyday Life THE LIAR IN YOUR LIFE morally lax and more manipulative or simply more crazy many of the most notorious historical events the Greek infil titan an "ordinary" person. The seven dwarfs embody another tration or Troy, the Holocaust, the invasion of Iraq feature of the lying stereotype. \Ve can see these men in the deception on a grand scale. -involve ruthless liar mold, displaying a \\illingness to sacrifice the truth Yet the fact is that most of the lies to wh ieh we arc exposed in order to make a profit or to escape punishment. They aren't on a daily basis, the ones that influence us the most, do not fi t the pattern we find in the splashier examples of deception. crazy, but they are greedy or guilty enough to be dishonest. In both of these incidents of deception, too, we can identify Indeed, the lies that matler most to our everyday lives come innocent victims of the lies. Amanda tricked and emotionally not from compulsive liars or depraved business executives. Ex manipulated Gary for her own purposes, whatever they might traordinary liars may attract the most attention, but an accu have been. Cl 'he Big Tobacco executives perpetrated their fraud rate portrait of deceit reveals that most of the liars in our lives, against a congressional subcommittee and, more broadly, and the lies they tell, arc characterized mainly by how com against the American public in general. In other words, they monplace they really are. lied to us. Finally, and perhaps most importantly, in both cases the act of lying was a clear violation of conventional standards fix Everyday Inventions It is probably comforting to beliCH' that we encounter few ~lachiavellian behavior. Amanda's lies violated the bonds of trust she shared liars in our daily lives. Perhaps someune with a with her fiance, and so Gary broke ofT their engagement. The bent or with psychological problems like Amanda's might lie tobacco executives broke their oath to tell the truth, and le repeatedly to our face. Ordinary people, though, are less likely gal action followed. In broad terms, the lies in both cases were to do so. Or, at least, that's what we'd like to think. This assumption turns out to be wrong. A wealth of psy wron/", and were censured as such. In sum, tlwn, both of these stories, while different in their chological research has for decades provided evidence that it's particulars, fit our common conception of how lyiIlg works. not ollly the atypically immoral who lie frequelltly. Illdced, my One story played out in a private arena, the other in public, own research into deception, whieh has spall ned more than but in both cases the structure was the same: atypical individu four decades now, has repeatedly shown lying to be far more als employed deception to victimize innocent people and wnc cOIllmon in daily life than we think. To attempt to find out justly condemned for it. Indeed, because such stories arc so IT just how common, I (along with my studentsJames Forrest and markable and draw so much attention, they almost difine the BeI~amin Happ) conducted a study of more than a hundred stereotype of deceit. As with the lies touched on earlier, those people in ordinary social situations. CI 'he results of this study of the duplicitous mechanic and the dishonest date, when we have major implications fc)r how we understand the lying in our think about lying, these are the kinds of dramatic events that life -and garnered media attention that ranged far beyond the shape our impressions. They arc also consistent with the "big" circles of academia. dcccpt ions found throughout the history of civilization. I nckecL 10 The setup of the study was fairly simple Two at a time, I had 11 THE LIAR IN YOUR LIFE participants meet, and I asked these unacquainted individuals to spend ten minutes getting to know each other. I didn't tt'll Everyday Inventions of Everyday Life I did, though, introduce one other twist into the basic struc ture or the t'xperiment. I wanted to know if the frequency of the part icipants, though, that I was conducting a st ud) of ly lying might change with the specifics of the conversation. Per ing. I nstead, I said I was interested in investigating hO\\ people haps some social interactions were more prone to deception interact when they meet someone new. Something else I didn't than others. mention \vas that each pair's entire conversation would be se 'l() try to find out, I told some of the participants to attempt cretly videotaped. (Ironically, researchers who study deception to come off as very likable. In other cases, I told one of the par end up employing a good deal of it in their research.) ticipants to convince the other that he or she was very compe After the conversation finished, 1 revealed the secret sur tent. Everyone else I instructed simply to get to know the other veillance to the participants. I then asked one of each pair to re person. I reasoned that while assigning goals to certain people view the video \vith me. As the participant watched the video, I did introduce a slightly greater degree of artificiality into the asked him or her to identify, moment by moment, any instances experiment, social situations in which one person is trying to in which he or she said something that was "inaccurate."' (I pur demonstrate his or her charm or poise to another are very com posely didn't ask participants to report "lies," want ing to avoid mon. And again: none of the people in the study knew I was their becoming defensive or embarrassed and, consequently, even interested in lies. As far as the pairs of strangers were con not admitting to deception. The term "inaccurate" seemed less cerned, deception had no relevance whatsoever to the study. charged than "lie.") The conversations between the participants generally un There is a difference worth emphasizing between this study folded as you might expect: some initial, tentative "How are and other laboratory studies of deception. lVlost studies of ly you?"s and "vVhere are you from?''s, followed by a more com ing, many of my own included, involve a fairly artificial setup. fortable exchange of personal details and opinions as the chat For example, a participant might be asked to watch a series got rolling. One conversation between a man and a woman of short video clips of people cit";cribing their upbringing and I'll call Tim and Allison was fairly typical. Tim was a college then identify in which clips participants had been told by tht' student with a laid-back disposition suggestive of someone at experimenter to lie. That sort of thing doesn't usually happen ease with himself. Once he and Allison had gotten a little bit in reallik. However, we meet new people all the time. ;"faybe acquainted, he told her about his band. the COl1\'(Tsations we have with strangers don't usually occur in a small room with a two-way mirror, but meeting and get TIM: ting to know someone else is a fundamental part of social life. ALLISON: vVhatever other conditions my study introduced, at its heart it TIM: vVe just signed to a record company, actually. Yeah, Epitaph. did seem to reproduce a typical, everyday experience. I hoped, ALLISON: then, that the data I gleaned would be particularly relevant to TIM: an exploration of deception in ordinary life. ALLISON: 12 Really? Do you sing or ... Yeah, I'm the lead singer. vVow! 13 THE LIAR IN YOUR LIFE Another participant, Natasha, also discussed with her part ner her musical background, talking about how she entered r ! ~ Everyday Inventions of Everyday Life "getting to know you" chat attracted national media attention. competitions as a pianist and toured the country with a cham The CBS Evening News, the BBC, the New York Times, and the Washington Post all covered the story. I was often asked whether ber group. She also managed to weave into the conversation that she'd been a member of her honor society in high school. my results could be explained by some circumstance unrelated It was all more or less what you'd expect from two strangers making small talk-one person, directly or just unconsciously, trying to impress another with his or her achievements, or just passing the time by discussing his or her life. would be easy-and maybe a little comforting-to conclude that the randomly selected participants in my study just hap pened to be unusually duplicitous. Or one might reason that What makes all of the above examples remarkable is that they are all lies. Natasha never toured the country, and never they normally would. But my later research on conversations between unacquainted strangers has shown, fairly consistently, quite managed to be inducted into the honor society. Tim's that they lie to each other about three times every ten minutes, band didn't sign with Epitaph. In fact, there's no band at all. both inside and outside the lab. With apologies to the media Tim's musical expertise is limited to, in his words, "a couple chords." And these are only a few examples of what I found to be an extraordinary pattern. Participants in my study didn't just lie occasionally. They lied a lot. to how people "actually lie" in the real world. Admittedly, it some factor in my study induced people to lie far more than outlets that shined a spotlight on my work, the results of this study really weren't extraordinary. They were typical. The ex traordinary thing is how much, it turns out, people lie to each other. Participants watching themselves on video confessed to lies High rates of lying are common even outside conversations that were big and small, rooted in the truth and fantastic, pre dictable and unexpected, relatively defensible and simply baf between strangers, who are unlikely to encounter each other in fling. Further, the lying was hardly limited to the participants the future. We might be able to accept that new acquaintances lie to each other with regularity, but we'd think that people to whom I had given a directive to appear likable or competent. These people lied with greater frequency, but even those with no specific agenda lied regularly. with any sort of established social bond would not. Yet this is All told, I found that most people lied three times in the course of a ten-minute conversation. Some lied as many as conversations between strangers, as in my study; in interactions between spouses and lovers; within families; and on the job. twelve times. Bear in mind, too, that after the fact, participants might have been reluctant to confess to their "inaccuracies." Lies occur regularly in all such contexts. The exact frequency This would only lead to an underreporting of the incidences of deception, though. In other words, it's possible that the fre quency oflies was even higher than three lies per conversation. My finding that most people told three lies in a ten-minute 14 not the case. Psychologists such as myselfhave conducted scores of studies examining interpersonal deception: as it plays out in is difficult to measure. A study like mine works fine to examine interactions between people who are meeting for the first time, but it's not as if this setup lends itself to, say, conversations be tween a husband and wife in bed. However, diary research studies, in which participants 15 THE LIAR IN YOUR LIFE Everyd ay I nventions of Everyday Life are asked to record their daily social interactions and indicatt' ":\ed" leads to a series of complications and, because it's a which contained lies, show that lying occurs regularly even in Hugh Grant movie, ends up helping \\'ill find love. The tropt' the most intimate relationships. Although deception occurs at is a lower levels bet\\cen those with a close bond and often involves lie-about having a passion for dance or a lucrative career- in lies meant to put anot her person at ease ("Of course you're not order to win the interest of some desirable romantic partner. f~lmiliar one in romantic comedies: the hero tells a giant putting on weight"), lying is still a routine part of the rapport \Ve probably don't spend much time vvondering why a to between spouses, lovers, close friends, and family members. No bacco executive would lie to a congressional subcommittee relationship has been found about the dangers of cigarettes. l'\or does it baffle us when a lO be immune to dishonesty. The weight of evidence is thus compelling and clear: people are lied to with great frequency in their daily lives. \Vhile the mechanic tells us a replacement part costs three times more than it actually does. While \\C' might question the character of extent and content of Amanda's deception of Gary may have the people who tell such lies, the reasons for the lies themselves been extreme, tlw bct that she lied regularly is not. ] ,ying in seem straightforward. Tobacco ext'cutives want to evade a truth our society is not "abnormal." The normal people who fill our that would harm their business and perhaps leave them legally lives do it all the time, in all sorts of ordinary interpersonal culpable; greedy mechanics hope to bilk ignorant customers situations. out of a kw dollars more. Profit, the avoidance of punishment: The question becomes, \Vhy? \Vhy do people fecI compelled th(~y these are the sorts of motivations we often associate with de don't belong to or honors they never ception. If we add Amanda's example into our considerations, earned:) \Vhat are the lies that oecur so regularly when we talk we might include mental imbalance as another conventionally with someone new- or sometimes even with a person we know understood motive for lies. to make up bands Yet greed, evasion of punishment, or some degree of insan well--all about? To put it simply: Why all the lies? To answer these questions, we'll need to reconsider some of ity arc not adequate explanations fiJI' much of the lying that oc our fundamental ideas ahout the motivations behind deception. curs in daily litc'. Unacquainted strangers usually have nothing Stories like those of Am an cia duping Gary or the seven dwarfs material to gain or any consequences to avoid through decep trying to dupe us tend to make us think about lying in terms tion. Nor is t hert' profit involved in many of the other inter of what it does to the person who is fooled. The central issue, personal interactions that make up atypical day, and which though, may be what lying doesjoT the person telling the lies. researchers have found to be riddled \vith lies. Conditions like mythomania are very rare; mental problems can account for "We Have So Much in Common" only a small portion oflics. BI!V, starring Hugh Grant and Ironically, the fiction of movies like Abuut a BI!Y may provide adapted from a l'\ick Hornby ]HWel, the thirty-something pro more of a clue for the motivations behind the bulk of the decep tagonist, \Vill, pretends to haH' a son so he can join a single tion we encounter than real-life incidents like t1w tobacco hear In the 2002 film About IZ parents' group to med \yomen. Predictably, the 16 in\'(~ntion of ing. In the I11m'ics, deceptive behavior is exaggerated and played 17 THE LIAR IN YOUR LIFE Everyday I nventions of Everyday Life for laughs. Yet using deception to win aflection may be so com here is not only that such behavior is common, as psychologi mon in the movies because it is so common in reality, too. cal research has shown, but also that mirroring of opinion to '['0 see what does motivate llluch interpersonal deception, avoid disagreement and demonstrate similarity is widely con let's examine more closely the lies told during my experiment. sielereel to be normal. If you later tell a ii'iend that you liked your As I mentioned, I gave some of the participants particular goals date but spent your dinner with her arguing over politics and fClr huw to present themselves: some of them were instructed to expressing disinterest in her hobbies, he might give you credit come off as likable, others as competent. And while participants for honesty, but he would also think you were a complete social with no particular goal lied frequently, tellingly, those who had moron. Deception is such a common technique for ingratiation a self-presentation agenda lied more. Tellingly, also, the kinds that its lack strikes us as aberrant. Now let's look at the participants in my study I told to try to oflies participants told varied based on their goals. Let's first consider the participants instructed to impress appear competent. The lies of this group reveal another pmver their partner with their likability. These people tended to tell I'ul lnoti\'ation for interpersonal deception, one more directly lies about their feelings. They distorted their true opinions and psychological than social. Participants instructed to come off as emotions, often in order to mirror those expressed by their competent tenckd to lie about their biographies. They invented partners. If their partner expressed a preference iClr Chinese achin'ements and plans that would enhance the way they were lewd, lelr instance, they would claim to like Chinese j()od, too, pcrcci\ed. 'I 'hey spoke of academic honor socie( ics they didn't even if they hated it. This kind of mirroring behavior is exactly belong to and of career ambitions they had no intention of ful the kind we sec enacted for comedic eflect in so many movies. filling. In other words, in order to impress their partners as In fact, psychologists have found the mirroring of opinions, competent, they made up stories about being competent. even at the cost of the truth, to be a very common strategy I want to emphasize again that I didn't encourage any of the for ingratiating oneself with others. The reasoning behind participants to lie. Indeed, when I spoke with participants with such behavior is rather straightforward. Most people shy away sc If~pres('ntatioll goals after their COllvcrsat iOlls, they reported from conflict and disagreement. They build relationships with that they hadn't felt encouraged to be deceptive. Deception was others based on fundamental things they have in In a strategy they selected on their own to achieve their goal. But order to itlrm a relationship with another person, then, one why would so many people lie to appear competent or likable? would want to avoid the areas or dissonance and emphasize the \ Vhy not 'just be yourself'? COIIIIIIOlI. \Ve can answer these questions if we consider what 'Just commonalities. Imagine yourself on a first date. If you find your date being yourseW' means. Often, when someone gives us this ad '~just charming and attractive, it would be an obvious tactic to try to vice, the implication is that at least gloss over your disgust for her preferred political party approach to any social situation. In fact, "just being and to maybe feign more interest than you actually have in her if we examine it closely, turns out to be a fairly complicated discussion of \Vest African folk music. What bears cmphasi.'i pruccss, invoking a complex balancing of a range of factors. 18 19 being yourself' is the simplest yourself~" THE LIAR IN YOUR LIFE To begin with, when we interact with another person, there I Everyday I nventions of Everyday Life we can see how it can easily slide into deception. Again, every is always a larger social context that shapes the encounter. If interaction involves decisions about which attributes to empha we see size and it friend at a party, for example, we will speak with him to minimize, which impulses to fCJllow and which to difft:rently than we would if we visited him in his cubicle at his ignore. At some point, we may not be choosing among our ac office. The din('rent standards f()r behavior in the different St't tual traits and ClUr sincere react ions. \ \'e may simply fabricate tings have to be taken into account in the ''self'' we present to the traits and reactions the social situation calls fl)r, or that we our friend. This self is further shaped by the content of the cn think it calls for. In other words, we might lie. counter, emotional or otherwise. If a friend has just lost his cat, I n addition to this, there is another complexity to 'Just be we wouldn't be much a friend if we didn't express sympathy; if ing yourself." We aren't always confident in ourselves. Insecu a friend announces her engagement, we wouldn't be much of rities are part of human nature. All of us, at times, question a friend if we didn't express joy. Such social standards fe)r how our own good qualities-whether we are really smart enough, we ought to act are weighed against expressions of our "true" attractive enough, capable enough. Lying can allow us to navi f(~elillgS. gate social situations in which we don't feel we quite measure Finally, working in coruunction with our sensitivity to con up. Like the participants in my study who attempted to appear text is an almost constant interest in projecting a positive image competent by making up stories of their competence, if we kar of ourselves. :\0 matter where we are or who we arc talking we don't embody the qualities a particular situatiun calls for, to, we generally want to show our better qualities. \Vhich of we can substilllt e a fiction that docs. these qualities we express is again illl1uenced by the nature of Think back to Amanda and the lies she told Gary about the interaction we are involved in. For example, our excellent her deceased parent and her battle with cancer. I t's probably sense of humor isn't much use at funerals, but our caring, sensi not a 'vvorthwhile exercise to try to draw thirclhand conclusions tive nature is. On the other hand, our scnse of humor might be about why she did \\hat she did. But if we assume that there precisely \vhat we want to show on a first date, or to a prospec was at least tive client. miration, of attempting to present }wrself in a \vay that was SOflH-' clement of wanting to win sympathy and ad 'just being yourself," it turns out, takes effort. Indeed, psy more appealing to Gary than she believed the facts allowed for, chologists have long discussed it as something that takes creative we can identify elements of conventional social behavior in her effort. Our expression of who we are involves choices that re mythomania. Hect social and interpersonal context, our mood, our personal ity, our need to maintain our sclf~image, .[he key difkn-'nce, of course, is that Amanda man ipulated and on and on. The Cary practically all t he way to the altar. Common interpersonal fact that we are able to perform this process with little con deception is usually far less menacing. \Vithout a doubt, every scious clTort every day and in nearly every conversation speaks lie, by definition, involves deceit and implicit manipulation to our vast intelligence as social beings. but that cloes not mean that every lie is employed in the service Still, if' W(c consider self~prcscntatinll 20 as a creative process, of deception and manipulation. \Yhen Tim told Allison about 21 r THE LIAR IN YOUR LIFE Everyday Inventions of Everyday Life his nonexistent band and his nonexistent record contract, he it can be argued that an ability to lie is a valuable social skill. did so vvithout any larger agencb of fooling her into doing this Consider whether you would really want to form a relationship or thinking that. For all he knew, he would never see Allison with someone who pointed out every area of disagreement or again. Tim's lies seemed to involve Allison only secondarily. could not comince you of his feigned interest in your hobbies. His primary goal seemed to be fostering his own persona or In mallY ways, deception is not so difkrcnt from tact; indeed, addressing his own insecurities \vhen meeting a new person. To one could make a case that sometimes they arc act uany one put it simply, Tim's lies were about 7 im. and the same. \Ve now see how another aspect of the conventional concep Regardless of the terminology we apply to interpersonal tion oflying is debunked by reality. Stories like that of Amanda deception, it's important to recognize that it is both a common and of the seven dwarf, of Big Tobacco make us think of lying and an accepted feature of ordinary conversation. Strict hon as a tool used to victimize or exploit ordinary people. More esty is often directly opposed to what we consider to be stan commonly, though, ordinary people use lying to quell their own darel social behavior. Lying, as strange as it sounds, is more insecurities, to build a friendsh ip, or to avoid a disagreement. normal. Sometimes, too, lying can be used within a conversation to But if lying is more frequently used to keep a conversation benefit t he conversation itself. There are times in any interac mO\'ing than to cheat or manipulate, this raises an important tion wlwn a strict adherence to the truth would only interrupt question: Does lying matter as much as we think? Perhaps, its natural How, probably needlessly. \Vhen a friend wants to tell since there arc no obvious victims to much deception, its fre you about the great time he had in Montauk over the weekend, quency in our culture should not be so alarming. Maybe lying, and asks you, "You know where Montauk is, right?," it borders outside of the kind practiced by Amanda or the seven dwarfs of on the pointless, not to mention the tedious, to stop his story Big Tobacco, doesn't really matter so much. and find out exactly where the town might be. The conversa This line of thinking may be comforting, but unfortunately tion gcws much more smoothly if you nod, and say, "Oh, sure, it is also misleading. While many lies don't cost us money or Montauk." I call such deception "lies of social convenience." happiness, they do cost us something. The idea that some don't Tlwse lies grease the wheels of social discourse. They are not is surprisingly common, though. Many of us like to think that about /c}oling someone or achin'ing illicit gain. They arc a tac there are lies that are so insignificant, they aren't even really tic to make communication easier, or sometimes even possible. lies at all. As we will discuss more fully in future chapters, psycholo gists have found an association between socially successful peo.. The Myth of the Little White Lie pie and skill at deception. In other words, popular people, for We have seen how deception is pervasive in daily life, but whatever reason, tend to be good liars. vVhik it is easy to exag not deception as we commonly think of it. A coworker may genlte the meaning of this finding (we can't say, for instance, claim to like the Yankees in order to steer clear of an argument that popular people are popular because they are good liars), about the Red Sox. The woman in line ahead of 22 23 lIS at Star THE LIAR IN YOUR LIFE Everyday I nventions of Everyday Life bucks may kign a familiarity with our company to avoid the less warm, less intirnate, less comf(Jrtable than those that are inconvenience or saying that she's never heard of it. more honest. \Vhen I tell people that ten-minute conversations often in The common lies in everyday life may not hurt us in an clude three lies, and sometimes more, they are usually shocked. easily measurable way, then. But they have the efrect or making But when I cxplai n the !lmct ion or tllCse Iics, the shock gi\cs nlTyclay way to a kind of ambivalence. "But th()sc are just little white a society so prone to cleceit in so many of its aspects: Ol1r lies," I've been told more than ()nce' aiJout some of the more oftcn smudged. 'I 'he accumulation of' these many smudges can common forms of interpersonal deception. erode our trust in one another, it can make us cynical about our This is what I call the Myth of the Little Vlhitc Lie. Accord ing to this myth, "little white lies" are not the same thing as "real" lies. "Little' white lies," like a lie of social conveni(~nce lif(~ that much less friendlv. This is the cost ofliving in lif(~ is media and government, it can make us generally less attent ive to the world around us. vVhatever our particular response to or the lies in our lives, tlw fact is that we han' one. All lies have an the "My pleasure" we hear on receiving our dry cleaning, arc impact. It's probably EliI' to say that some have a greater impact so negligible--or so the myth says--that they shouldn't even than others, but given their volume, not even the smallest lie be grouped with lies like those of the seven dwarfs. "Real" lies can be wholly ignored. arc bad: they cost money or cause pain. "Real" lies arc morally furthermore, when people find out they arc being lied to, wrong. "Little vvhite lies," by (somewhat circular) deflnitioll, the ('f"fect is immediate and almost ahvays negative. In a study don't hurt anyonf'. ] conducted in which participants learned that they'd been lied Unfortunately, the Myth or the Little \Vhite Lie is basically to during a eonnTsation they'd just held with another person, a fairy tale. Although "little white lies" may be less egregious these participants immediately formed a negative impression of' than "real" lies, they still-like all deception---involve some the individual who had lied to them. Their conversational part degree of \·ictimization. If a lie sllcceeds, someone is always n(']" was seen as unt1"llstworthy, unlikable, and generally more j()oled. And, crucially, even if the target ofthc lie doesn't (kvious. But here's t he most insidious consequence: those who kllCl\\ were duped typically began to increase the level of their lies in a this, the liar docs. Bella DePaulo, a n'scarcher at the University of California, subsequent conversation with tl](' person who lied to them. In Santa Barbara, and her stuclents have found repeated and con short, lies -- even the tiny white Iies of everyday conversation- sistent cvideno' that lying beget more lies. even "white" Iving--takes a toll on the teller or the lie. Lying call cause a "twinge of distress," in DePaulo's words, making liars je'e1 a little worse than they did bel'ore they told a lie. further, this coloring of mood can last even aftcr the conversation has returned to more honest terri tory. The sum efIect of th is is what DePaulo calls an emotional "smudge" on the interaction. Conversat ions involving Iies arc 24 25
© Copyright 2025 Paperzz