Describing the values of Filipino adolescents

Journal of Tropical Psychology
C The Author(s) 2014, doi:10.1017/jtp.2014.2
Volume 4, e2, pp. 1–8, Describing the values of Filipino adolescents:
a comparison with pan-cultural norms
Allan B. I. Bernardo,1 Jose Antonio R. Clemente,2 and Gregory Arief D. Liem3
1
University of Macau
University of Macau and University of the Philippines, Diliman
3
National Institute of Education, Singapore
2
An etic approach was used to describe the values of Filipino adolescents and to show how pan-cultural
comparisons using a values survey can complement emic approaches to studying values. Participants were
752 adolescents who answered the Portrait Values Questionnaire (PVQ). The results indicate that PVQ
has structural validity and adequate internal consistency. Ranks of the value types were compared with
pan-cultural student norms; the results indicate that (a) Filipino adolescents’ value type hierarchies are
substantially similar (same top, same middle, same bottom ranked values), and (b) Filipino adolescents
gave higher rankings for hedonism and stimulation and lower ranking for achievement.
Keywords: values, adolescents, pan-cultural norms, Portrait Values Questionnaire, Philippines, etic-vs.-
emic approaches
Many societies worry about how each generation of adolescents seems to display a distinct set of behaviours, attitudes
and aspirations different from the previous generations and
from current mores of society. This is certainly true in the
Philippines where such worries are often framed in terms
of concerns about the ‘erosion of values’ the adolescents
have; and in this regard, there has been some effort to measure and understand the values of Filipino adolescents (McCann Erickson Philippines, 2001; Ogena, 1999). One way
of comprehensively describing what adolescents are like entails assessing the content, structure and hierarchy of their
values, defined here as beliefs about desirable end states
or behaviours that transcend specific situations and serve
as guiding principles in people’s lives (Schwartz & Bilsky,
1987). As Schwartz (2006) puts it, studying values directly
is an efficient way to capture and characterise groups or
cultures. Values emphasised in one community or culture
may constrain the behavioural choices that are available to
individuals in the group, particularly in cultures that have a
very stronger articulation of and conformity to value norms
(Hong & Phua, 2013). As values direct behaviour towards
certain goals instead of others, explicating the values of
adolescents may shed light on the motivations propelling
certain behaviours and experiences that are often associated
with this developmental stage. For instance, internalisation
of prosocial values (e.g., kindness) has been linked to behaving prosocially (e.g., doing things that benefit others with-
out expecting anything in return) (Hardy & Carlo, 2005),
and endorsement of moral values (e.g., truth, affiliation)
was inversely related to self-reported delinquency (Tarry &
Emler, 2007). In Asian contexts, conservation values are associated with adolescent students’ social-oriented achievement motives, whereas self-enhancement values are associated with individual-oriented achievement motives (Liem,
Martin, Porter, & Colmar, 2012; Liem & Nie, 2008). In the
domain of work, emphasis on intrinsic work values may
influence career choice (Duffy & Sedlacek, 2007), whereas
Confucian-related work values including self-discipline and
interpersonal ethics impact on individual and team performance in Chinese organizations (Huang, Liang, & Hsin,
2012).
ADDRESS FOR CORRESPONDENCE:
Allan B. I. Bernardo, Department
of Psychology, PLG334 Pearl Jubilee Building, University of
Macau, Macau SAR China; email: [email protected]
Author Notes: Preparation of this manusript was supported by a grant
from University of Macau (SRG14-FSH13-ABIB) and the
data-gathering and data-encoding activities were supported in part by a
grant from De La Salle University (Project #42 F U 3 11). We gratefully
acknowledge the research assistance provided by Karen Sison and Luis
Peralta, the support of all the school heads and teachers who facilitated
the data-gathering activities, and all the students who generously
participated in our survey.
1
Downloaded from https:/www.cambridge.org/core. IP address: 88.99.165.207, on 17 Jun 2017 at 07:27:25, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at
https:/www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/jtp.2014.2
Allan B. I. Bernardo, Jose Antonio R. Clemente, Gregory Arief D. Liem
Filipino Values
The present investigation aims to describe the value structure and hierarchy of Filipino adolescents. A quick survey
of the literature on value studies in the Philippine context reveals a focus on identifying values that are purportedly salient to most Filipinos. Much of the work on this
subject has mostly been descriptive, with scholars nominating their own set of values, which they deemed would
best describe the Filipino. For example, Ramirez (1997)
culled local leadership values based on her observations
of the events that transpired during the 1986 EDSA People
Power Revolution. She identified being maka-Diyos (belief in a transcendent force that is present within; a truth
that guides in life), maka-tao (being humane) and makabayan (being nationalistic) as values of authentic leadership. Talisayon (1994) synthesised nearly 100 studies on
Filipino values and identified five value clusters that comprise the Filipino value system: namely, relationship (e.g.,
respect for authority), inwardness (e.g., spirituality), social
(e.g., nationalism), livelihood (e.g., hard work) and optimism (e.g., joy and humour). Other oft-mentioned value
themes in previous studies include close family ties, selfand emotional control, courteous and friendly interactions,
concern and sharing with others, hospitality, courage and
endurance, and desire for economic progress (Church &
Katigbak, 2000). In an attempt to re-conceptualise Filipino
behaviour patterns and value structure (Pe-Pua & ProtacioMarcelino, 2000), Enriquez (2008) proposed a shared identity model (SIM) of Filipino values. The most important
assertion in the SIM is that the core value of the Filipino is
a sense of oneness or unity that emanates from the recognition of a shared identity or a notion of a shared inner self.
Thus, the self and the other are one and the same. Consequently, other values in this model (e.g., hiya/propriety,
lakas ng loob/courage, pakikiramdam/interpersonal sensitivity, pakikibaka/resistance against injustice, etc.) are related
values that are expressions of this core value.
As the SIM aimed to characterise Filipinos in general, recent studies have employed this model to specifically elucidate the value priorities of Filipino adolescents. For instance,
Clemente et al. (2008) showed that apart from the values
listed in the SIM, adolescents also deemed piety and respect
as important guiding principles in their lives. Notably, kindheartedness (kagandahang-loob) – defined as shared nobility
manifested by acts of generosity that spring spontaneously
from the goodness of the heart (Enriquez, 2008) – emerged
as the highest rated value in the adolescents’ lists of values.
In a review (Gastardo-Conaco, Jimenez, & Billedo, 2003),
the following values were identified as being particularly
important to Filipino adolescents: nationalism and pride
regarding identity, parental and family concerns, conformity, leadership and benevolence. Sex differences regarding
values were also noted. Young Filipino males valued family, religion, nation, social awareness and recognition more
than their female counterparts. On the other hand, young
2
Filipino females seem to value family attachment, social
mobility, friendships, autonomy, moral orientation, social
consciousness and benevolence more than the males. Note
that some of the ‘values’ listed are not strictly speaking values
(i.e, they are not beliefs about desirable end states that serve
as guiding principles); instead, some may be considered as
concepts deemed important or valuable to the adolescents.
The review ends with the observation of a trend that seems
to suggest the deterioration of the youth’s moral values as
social acceptance rather than principle becomes the norm
of behaviour. Similar observations regarding the shifting
values of Filipino adolescents have been noted in previous
surveys (Ogena, 1999).
These aforementioned investigations on Filipino values
often used indigenous approaches, which were assumed to
provide a less distorted image constructed from the perspective of Filipinos (Pe-Pua & Protacio-Marcelino, 2000). By articulating value structures using local concepts culled from
the language and emphasising an ‘insider’s point of view’
in the interpretation of the results, such studies have provided nuanced, contextualised and culturally-appropriate
descriptions of Filipinos, in general, and adolescents, in
particular. While we affirm the importance of such indigenous or emic approaches, we would also like to argue that
it would be beneficial to describe adolescents using an etic
perspective that refers to a more comprehensive value structure that is recognised across cultures, like Schwartz’ theory
of basic human values (Schwartz, 1992). Exemplars of this
cross-cultural approach go beyond interpreting value structures and hierarchies in isolation and afford a meaningful interpretation of the cultural-distinctiveness or culturalsimilarity of such value priorities. As demonstrated by Liem,
Martin, Nair, Bernardo and Prasetya (2011), for instance,
adolescents from four countries with diverse backgrounds
share a strikingly similar pattern of value configuration.
Aside from the establishment of similar value configurations across cultures, the etic perspective also allows for
cross-cultural comparison of value priorities and also comparisons with pan-cultural norms (Schwartz & Bardi, 2001).
Schwartz’s values theory and the use of norm-referenced
comparisons in values research are briefly discussed in the
next section.
Schwartz’s Value Types
Two main features of Schwartz’s values theory are relevant
in the current investigation. First, values are cognitive representations of desirable, trans-situational goals which aim
to meet three universal requirements of human existence –
biological needs, requisites of coordinated social interaction
and demands of group survival or functioning (Schwartz,
1992). Because these needs are universal, it is assumed that
people across cultures recognise and endorse more or less
similar values.
Schwartz (2006) defined ten value types that comprehensively capture nearly all the possible value categories. Each
JOURNAL OF TROPICAL PSYCHOLOGY
Downloaded from https:/www.cambridge.org/core. IP address: 88.99.165.207, on 17 Jun 2017 at 07:27:25, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at
https:/www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/jtp.2014.2
Values of Filipino adolescents
value type is motivationally distinct; that is, each value type
represents, manifests and expresses a central and general
motivational life goal. For instance, giving importance to the
value type of self-direction implies striving for independentthought and action across various domains in one’s life. To
make each value type more concrete, the theory also proposes that the content of each type is represented by a set
of specific values. Conceptually, this implies that the attainment of the specific values promotes the central goal of each
value type. Operationally, these sets of value items are used
to measure each value type, as in the case of the Schwartz
Value Survey (SVS), which assesses the importance of each
distinct value as a guiding principle in one’s life. For example, the life goal of pursuing independent thought and
action (i.e., self-direction) means giving importance to specific values such as creativity, freedom and choosing own
goals.
As described by Schwartz (2006), aside from selfdirection, other value types include: stimulation (the pursuit
of excitement and novelty, represented by values such as a
varied life and daring); hedonism (the quest for sensuous
gratification for oneself, exemplified by pleasure and enjoying life); achievement (the life goal of attaining personal
success by demonstrating competence in accordance with
society’s standards, represented by values such as ambitious
and capable); power (aiming for prestige, social status and
control over people and resources, characterized by valuing
wealth and preserving public image); security (attainment
of safety, harmony and stability of self, relationships and
society, represented by social order and reciprocation of
favours); conformity (the goal of restraining action likely to
upset or harm others and violate social norms, embodied
by giving importance to politeness and honoring parents
and elders); tradition (motivated by respect, commitment
and acceptance of customs, represented by humble, devout
and accepting my portion in life); benevolence (the pursuit of
enhancing and preserving the welfare of one’s ‘in-group,’ exemplified by honest, loyal and forgiving); and, universalism
(the life goal of understanding, appreciating and protecting
the welfare of all people and nature, represented by social
justice, a world at peace and unity with nature).
The second feature of Schwartz’s values theory is that
values are ordered by relative importance to one another.
Individuals and groups may vary in the importance they attribute to the aforementioned value types. Although much
of the cross-cultural studies on values have focused on how
nations differ, research shows that there is a high degree
of consensus regarding the value priorities of individuals
across nations. Certain values are consistently ranked as especially important (e.g., benevolence) while others are consistently regarded as less important (e.g., power) (Schwartz
& Bardi, 2001). Using data sets of representative or nearrepresentative samples across 13 nations, teacher samples
from 56 nations and student samples from 54 nations, they
derived pan-cultural norms or averages of value hierarchies
which were used to analyse the value priorities of any nation,
sample of teachers and sample of students respectively. This
average value hierarchy is the basis for the establishment
of pan-cultural norms against which specific groups (e.g.,
adolescents) and societies can be compared.
Using pan-cultural norms, the interpretation of value
endorsement scores and ranks in a particular country of interest (e.g., Philippines) becomes more meaningful because
ratings are not considered in isolation. The comparative approach can shed light on which values are more emphasised
(or less emphasised) in a particular group when compared
to the normative baseline. To illustrate, Schwartz and Bardi
(2001) compared value hierarchies of Singaporean teachers
against the pan-cultural ranking. Their analysis suggests that
in Singapore, there is much more emphasis on stability of
relationships and an aversion for violating societal norms;
security and conformity were ranked highly compared to
the pan-cultural average. Consistent with this description
of the Singaporean sample, self-direction was also found
to be emphasised less, suggesting a relatively less importance given to supporting independent thought and action.
Using pan-cultural norms can also reveal whether value hierarchies are either culturally distinct or highly congruent
with the average of the comparative group. As a case in
point, the value hierarchies of Black African samples seem
to deviate from the pan-cultural average, with correlations
relatively lower when compared to the degree of consensus
of the majority of the nations or regions with the normative
baseline (Schwartz & Bardi, 2001). In relation to this, the
comparative approach can provide insight as to which factors possibly influence any significant deviations from these
norms. Schwartz and Bardi posit that nations have more or
less congruent value hierarchies because this reflects similar
priorities that nations have in accordance with their goals of
preserving relationships among group members and supporting individual members’ self-interests. Any deviations
from the pan-cultural pattern may reflect cultural particularities that help shape an individual’s value hierarchy.
As the present study focuses on describing the value profile of Filipino adolescents using Schwartz’s values theory,
it is important to note that the theory has been validated
in diverse samples of adolescents from countries such as
Germany (e.g., Bubeck & Bilsky, 2004), Chile and Portugal
(e.g., Bilsky, Niemann, Schmitz & Rose, 2005), South Africa
(e.g., Schwartz et al., 2001; Study 2), Uganda (e.g., Schwartz
et al., 2001; Study 3), China and Indonesia (Liem et al., 2012;
Liem & Nie, 2008), Singapore, Australia and the Philippines
(e.g., Liem et al., 2011). Most of these studies have focused
on confirming the theory’s value structure (i.e., whether the
ten value types were positioned in the same space within
the circular model, similar to the proposition of the original
theory). At the same time, these studies tested the stability of the value structure among children and adolescent
samples, since the proposed circular model and most subsequent studies validating it were often derived from adult
samples. In this study, however, we intended to compare
and interpret the value hierarchy of Filipino adolescents
JOURNAL OF TROPICAL PSYCHOLOGY
Downloaded from https:/www.cambridge.org/core. IP address: 88.99.165.207, on 17 Jun 2017 at 07:27:25, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at
https:/www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/jtp.2014.2
3
Allan B. I. Bernardo, Jose Antonio R. Clemente, Gregory Arief D. Liem
TABLE 1
Summary of means and ranks of value types for Filipino adolescents and pan-cultural
student norms
Filipino adolescents (n = 745)
Pan-cultural student Norms
Cronbach’s ␣
Mean rank
Mean rating (SD)
Mean rank
Mean rating∗ (SD)
Self-direction
.63
1
4.83 (0.68)
2
4.58 (0.31)
Benevolence
.65
2
4.81 (0.70)
1
4.59 (0.25)
Hedonism
.68
3
4.80 (0.84)
7
3.82 (0.65)
Value types
∗
Universalism
.73
4
4.67 (0.71)
3
4.25 (0.29)
Stimulation
.67
5
4.65 (0.91)
8
3.43 (0.34)
Security
.69
6
4.56 (0.80)
5
3.99 (0.36)
Conformity
.69
7.5
4.54 (0.78)
6
3.98 (0.48)
Achievement
.72
7.5
4.54 (0.83)
4
4.02 (0.30)
Tradition
.50
9
4.22 (0.75)
9
2.73 (0.48)
Power
.65
10
3.70 (0.96)
10
2.39 (0.43)
∗
The mean ratings for the Filipino adolescent sample were derived from scores using a 6-point scale,
whereas the mean ratings for the pan-cultural student norms were derived from scores using an 7-point
scale; thus, the two sets of means may not be directly compared to each other.
using the pan-cultural norms uncovered by Schwartz and
Bardi (2001), an approach which is not usually employed in
studies profiling Filipino adolescents’ values.
Method
Participants
Participants were recruited from two secondary schools and
one university in Metro Manila. A total of 752 students participated in the study. However, data of seven participants
were excluded because they failed to answer a substantial
number of items. Thus, data from 745 participants were included in the final analyses. The participants’ mean age was
16.7 years (SD = 1.84). Majority of the participants were
males (N = 477).
Materials
The 40-item Portrait Values Questionnaire (PVQ) was used
in this study. The PVQ includes short verbal portraits which
describe a person’s goal, aspirations or wishes that point implicitly to the importance of a value (Schwartz, 2006). For
instance: ‘It’s very important to him/her to help the people
around him/her. S(he) wants to care for their well-being’
describes a person for whom benevolence values are important. For each verbal portrait, participants were asked to
think about how much the person being described is like
him/her. In this study, the PVQ was administered to different batches of students on separate occasions. Participants
responded with options ranging from 6 (very much like me)
to 1 (not like me at all).
To test the structural validity of the scale, a confirmatory
factor analysis was conducted to verify whether the data
fitted the proposed ten-factor model. Instead of using all
of the scale items as indicators for each of the ten factors,
we used two parcels for each factor (Little, Cunningham,
Shahar, & Widaman, 2002). Parcels were used because it
is likely that having numerous indicators in a large sam-
4
ple would lead to unsatisfactory fit in the measurement
model (Bagozzi & Heatherton, 1994); the use of parcels also
reduces the likelihood that parameters will be affected by
item-specific variance (Lance, Woehr, & Fisicaro, 1991). The
use of parcels in confirmatory factor analysis of the PVQ has
been done in previous studies (Leung, Au, Huang, Kurman,
Niit, & Niit, 2007). The results of the confirmatory factor
analysis indicated adequate fit between the data and the tenfactor model: ␹2 (125) = 473.203, p <0.001; ␹2 /df = 3.786;
CFI = 0.924, GFI = 0 .940, AGFI = 0.900; TLI = 0.884;
RMR = 0.045, and RMSEA = 0.061. The regression weights
for all ten factors were significant, with standardised regression weights ranging from 0.53 to 0.95. Similar to Liem et al.
(2011; see also Schwartz, 2005), the ten subscales that measured each of the ten value types had acceptable reliability
coefficients except for the four-item Tradition subscale (see
Table 1).
Results
Table 1 presents the mean ratings and rankings of the ten
value types for both the Filipino adolescents and the pancultural student norms. An Analysis of Variance for repeated
measures was conducted to test whether there were differences among the ten value types. However, the Mauchly’s
test indicated that sphericity cannot be assumed (Mauchly’s
W = 0.165; ␹2 (44) = 1332.601, p < 0.0001), so we used
the Wilks’ Lamba multivariate test. The results showed a
significant difference across value types, Wilks’ Lambda =
0.317, F(9,736) = 176.394, partial ␩2 = .683, p < 0.0001.
The means were then compared using a pairwise comparison with Bonferroni adjustment, and the results indicate
a clustering of means across the ranks. In particular, the
means for the top three ranked value types (self-direction,
benevolence and hedonism) were not significantly different
from each other, but were all significantly higher than all
JOURNAL OF TROPICAL PSYCHOLOGY
Downloaded from https:/www.cambridge.org/core. IP address: 88.99.165.207, on 17 Jun 2017 at 07:27:25, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at
https:/www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/jtp.2014.2
Values of Filipino adolescents
other value types, thus forming the highest cluster of value
types. The mean for universalism and stimulation were also
not significantly different from each other, but universalism
was significantly higher than all other value types, whereas
stimulation was not different from security, conformity and
achievement. We consider these five value types as forming
the middle cluster of value types in the ranking. The two
lowest ranked value types (tradition and power) were significantly lower compared to all other value types, and were
also significantly different from each other; thus, forming
the lowest cluster of value types in the ranking.
How do the value hierarchies of the Filipino sample compare to the pan-cultural hierarchy? The correlation between
the ranks was computed, and indicated a moderate correlation between the two (rs [8] = 0.74, p = 0.015). The
correlation suggests a substantial degree of agreement regarding the relative importance of the value types. However,
the relationship is not as strong when compared to correlation coefficients that Schwartz and Bardi (2001) found in
other countries (i.e., median rs = 0.82). Inspection of the
ranks would indicate some notable differences: hedonism
and stimulation ranked higher (3rd and 5th, respectively) in
the Filipino adolescent sample compared to the pan-cultural
norms, and achievement ranked relatively lower (7.5th) in
the Philippine sample when compared to the norms. So
although there are clear similarities in the way the value
hierarchies are ordered, there also seem to be important
differences.
Discussion
The primary goal of the present study was to describe the
values of Filipino adolescents by measuring how they prioritise ten value types. As a complement to the more emic descriptions in the extant literature on adolescence and value
systems in the Philippines, we took an etic approach by using Schwartz’s theory of basic human values to illustrate
what Filipino adolescents value and compared their value
priorities with pan-cultural norms. The results of comparing the Filipino adolescents’ value types and hierarchies to
the pan-cultural norms suggest that Filipino adolescents (a)
prioritise stimulation (pursuit of an exciting and novel life)
and hedonism (quest for sensuous gratification for oneself) more than the average adolescent does, and (b) place
less importance on achievement (i.e., attaining success with
reference to society’s standards).
But before we discuss these comparative results, we
should note that the general ranking of the values of the
Filipino adolescent sample was not very different from the
ranking of the pan-cultural norms for the student sample,
with the same pair of values (self-direction and benevolence) ranked highest and another pair of values (tradition and power) ranked the lowest in both samples. That
Filipino adolescents share such a pattern with other adolescents across cultures could be reflective of the common
experiences that all adolescents undergo in this develop-
mental stage. Adolescence is referred to as a state of tension
between connection and individualism (Brown & Larson,
2002) where adolescents begin to negotiate between dependence on their close groups and independence to do tasks
on their own. In their negotiation of such tensions, they may
be guided by openness to change values (e.g., self-direction)
and less by conservative values (i.e., tradition), while still
recognising the importance of taking care of their in-group
(in this case, their family, aligned with prioritising benevolence). Noting these similarities to the pan-cultural norms
leads to different characterisations of the values of Filipino
adolescents compared to if the values are described in isolation. For example, if we just consider the Filipino adolescent
means in isolation, it would seem that the young Filipinos
prioritise the values of self-direction and benevolence, but
do not give importance to tradition and power given their
low ranks in the value hierarchy. But a comparison with the
pan-cultural norm indicates that that pattern is not unique
to Filipino adolescents and may actually be common to most
adolescents.
On the other hand, the comparison with pan-cultural
norms also suggests that Filipino adolescents might actually
give more importance to stimulation and hedonism relative
to other young people. The pan-cultural comparisons show
that the Filipino adolescents ranked these two value types as
more important relative to their counterparts in other parts
of the world. Recall that stimulation refers to the pursuit of
excitement and novelty and hedonism is the quest for sensuous gratification for oneself, and the asserted shift in the
values of Filipino adolescents might be more in these particular areas of their development. These results regarding
hedonism and stimulation seem consistent with previous
observations regarding the shifting values of Filipino adolescents (Gastardo-Conaco et al., 2003; Ogena, 1999), but
can also be seen from the lens of certain aspects of the Filipino culture. Both these value types emphasise the freedom
to pursue pleasure and an exciting life. The higher ranking of
these values might be linked to the importance that Filipinos
ascribe to their relationships (Enriquez, 2008). The pursuit
of pleasurable activities might be within the context of the
adolescents’ ever-expanding social networks. In addition to
the shared activities with family members, Filipino adolescents are also engaging in more (leisure) activities outside
the home, with their peers and romantic partners (Santa
Maria, 2002). Because shared pleasurable activities are a
means to establish and maintain relationships, it might be
that Filipino adolescents find such pursuits to be extremely
important.
The comparison with pan-cultural norms also suggests
that Filipino adolescents may value achievement less than
other youths. Recall that achievement refers to the goal
of demonstrating competence with reference to standards
set by society. We can try to make sense of this result by
considering Filipino adolescents’ values and aspirations in
their educational life, particularly as school is one of the
main preoccupations of the Filipino adolescents in the
JOURNAL OF TROPICAL PSYCHOLOGY
Downloaded from https:/www.cambridge.org/core. IP address: 88.99.165.207, on 17 Jun 2017 at 07:27:25, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at
https:/www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/jtp.2014.2
5
Allan B. I. Bernardo, Jose Antonio R. Clemente, Gregory Arief D. Liem
sample. Indeed, although the goal of demonstrating
normative competence based on external standards (or
performance-approach goals) seems to be important to Filipino adolescent students (Dela Rosa & Bernardo, 2013;
Ganotice, Bernardo & King, 2012), some comparisons suggest that they give such goals less importance than other
Asian students and that they actually perceive students who
aspire to display such normative competence less favourably
(c.f., Bernardo & Ismail, 2010). Other research indicate
that Filipino adolescents perceive achieving in school as
not being important to succeeding in Philippine society
(Bernardo, 2003), and reject parental norms regarding
the level of achievement they should aspire for in school
(Bernardo, 2010, 2012). In other words, if we consider adolescents’ aspirations related to schooling, there seems to be
convergent data showing that Filipino adolescent students
ascribe less importance to achievement in the academic
domain.
In this study we wanted to emphasise that values of adolescents are best interpreted when compared to the pancultural norms. Indeed we noted the degree of similarity
between the value hierarchies of the Filipino adolescents
and the pan-cultural sample, but there were also some value
types that were either more emphasised or less emphasised
in the Filipino adolescent sample. We proposed some speculative interpretations of the trends we observed, but what
we wish to underscore is how simple and straightforward
descriptions of the Filipino adolescents’ values may not be
warranted. We acknowledge that our results may also not
be generalisable given the nature of our sample, which was
drawn from an urban and educated sample, and may also
be drawn from a relatively higher socioeconomic group. It
would be important that future studies employ the same etic
approach we employed in this study using a more representative sample; and indeed, the benefit of the etic approach is
that the data-gathering procedures are such that it is actually
easier to obtain the desired data from a more representative
sample. The current study also does not inquire into the
factors that shape these value hierarchies, and future studies
may now look into other contextual factors that shape endorsement of values. For example, what socialisation agents
and social developmental experiences encourage the pursuit of stimulation while potentially stifling achievement?
Future research could also inquire into specific subtypes of
the ten value types that were recently defined in a refined
version of Schwartz’ values theory (Schwartz et al., 2012).
We wanted to underscore the importance of using etic
approaches in assessing values of adolescents not as the only
approach to measuring values, but as a complementary approach to emic approaches to describing values of a population. We note that there is some convergence between
the values found in emic studies and the results of our current study. In particular, we see that benevolence, which was
ranked high in our results, was also mentioned in the survey
of Gastardo-Conaco and colleagues (2003), and is consistent with kagandahang-loob or kindheartedness and respect
6
for others observed by Clemente and colleagues (2008) in
their study of Filipino adolescents’ values. The high ranking
of benevolence can also be seen as somewhat consistent with
Enriquez’s (2008) shared identity model where in a sense of
kapwa (shared identity of self and others) is assumed to be
the core value of the Filipinos. Our data show that Filipino
adolescents also give relatively high priority to benevolence
and universalism, which are the two value types most closely
related to a sense of shared identity. But the results show that
these two value types are not the most important for the Filipino adolescents, which might lead us to question whether
these are actually ‘core’ values for the Filipino adolescents.
There are more obvious discrepancies between the results of emic studies and of the current study. For example,
the emic studies on Filipino values also identify some values
such as piety (Clemente et al., 2008), nationalism and pride
regarding one’s identity (Gastardo-Conaco et al., 2003) that
do not seem to be captured in our data. Likewise, the higher
priority for the value types of hedonism and stimulation
found in our sample is not a value that has been noted in
emic studies of Filipino values. We believe that these discrepancies underscore the importance of doing both emic
and epic studies on Filipino adolescents’ values. The emic
approaches can identify culture-specific or group-specific
values that may not be included in etic-based taxonomies
of value types. On the other hand, comparisons involving
pan-cultural data and norms could also pick up value priorities that are not readily drawn from emic approaches.
As we have demonstrated, etic approaches seem to capture
both similarities and differences that emic approaches may
not readily surface. Indeed, it is hard to imagine how an
emic study might reveal that Filipino adolescents do not
give too much importance to the achievement value type.
Thus, future studies must indeed take both the emic and
etic approaches into account. Looking at values from these
two perspectives should increase our appreciation of these
important principles that guide adolescents and all people
in their everyday lives.
References
Bagozzi, R. P., & Heatherton, T. F. (1994). A general approach
to representing multifaceted personality constructs: Application to state self-esteem. Structural Equation Modelling,
1, 25–67.
Bernardo, A. B. I. (2003). Do Filipino youth really value education? Exploring Filipino adolescents’ beliefs about the
abstract and pragmatic value of education and its relationship to achievement goals and learning strategies. Philippine
Journal of Psychology, 36(2), 49–67.
Bernardo, A. B. I. (2010). Exploring Filipino adolescents’ perceptions of the legitimacy of parental authority over
academic behaviors. Journal of Applied Developmental
Psychology. 31, 271–280.
Bernardo, A. B. I. (2012). Perceived legitimacy of parental
control over academic behaviors and students’ academic
JOURNAL OF TROPICAL PSYCHOLOGY
Downloaded from https:/www.cambridge.org/core. IP address: 88.99.165.207, on 17 Jun 2017 at 07:27:25, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at
https:/www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/jtp.2014.2
Values of Filipino adolescents
adjustment. European Journal of Psychology of Education.
27, 557–571.
Bernardo, A. B. I., & Ismail, R. (2010). Social perceptions
of achieving students and achievement goals of students
in Malaysia and the Philippines. Social Psychology of
Education, 13, 385–407.
Bilsky, W., Niemann, F., Schmitz, J., & Rose, I. (2005). Value
structure at an early age: Cross-cultural replications. In
W. Bilsky & D. Elizur (Eds.), Facet theory: Design, analysis
and applications. Proceedings of the 10th International Facet
Theory Conference in Rome, 10–13 July 2005 (pp. 241–248).
Prague: Agentura Action.
Brown, B. B., & Larson, R. W. (2002). The kaleidoscope of
adolescence: Experiences of the world’s youth at the beginning of the 21st century. In B. B. Brown, R. W. Larson, & T.
S. Saraswathi (Eds.), The World’s Youth: Adolescence in eight
regions of the globe (pp. 1–20). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge
University Press.
Bubeck, M., & Bilsky, W. (2004). Value structure at an early
age. Swiss Journal of Psychology, 63(1), 31–41.
Church, A. T., & Katigbak, M. S. (2000). Filipino Personality:
Indigenous and cross-cultural studies. Manila: De La Salle
University Press.
Clemente, J. A., Belleza, D., Yu, A., Catibog, E. V. D., Solis, G.,
& Laguerta, J. (2008). Revisiting the Kapwa theory: Applying alternative methodologies and gaining new insights.
Philippine Journal of Psychology, 41(2), 1–32.
Dela Rosa, E. D., & Bernardo, A. B. I. (2013). Are two achievement goals better than one? Filipino students’ achievement
goals, learning strategies and affect. Learning and Individual
Differences, 27, 97–101.
Duffy, R. D., & Sedlacek, W. E. (2007). What is most important to students’ long-term career choices: Analyzing
10-year trends and group differences. Journal of Career
Development, 34(2), 149–163.
Enriquez, V. G. E. (2008). From Colonial to Liberation
Psychology: The Philippine experience (Reprint ed.).
Quezon City, Philippines: University of the Philippines
Press.
Ganotice, F. A., Bernardo, A. B. I., & King, R. B. (2012). Testing the factorial invariance of the English and Filipino versions of the Inventory of School Motivation with bilingual
students in the Philippines. Journal of Psychoeducational
Assessment, 30, 298–303.
Gastardo-Conaco, M. C., Jimenez, M. C. C., & Billedo, C. J. F.
(2004). Filipino Adolescents in Changing Times. Quezon
City, Philippines: University of the Philippines Center for
Women’s Studies and Philippine Center for Population and
Development.
Hardy, S. A., & Carlo, G. (2005). Religiosity and prosocial behaviours in adolescence: The mediating role of
prosocial values. Journal of Moral Education, 34(2), 231–
249.
Hong, Y., & Phua, D. Y. (2013). In search of culture’s role in
influencing individual and social behavior. Asian Journal of
Social Psychology, 16, 26–29.
Huang, M. P., Liang, W. C., & Hsin, C. N. (2012). Confucian
dynamism work values and team performance: A multiple-
level analysis. Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 15, 178–
188.
Lance, C. E., Woehr, D. J., & Fisicaro, S. A. (1991). Cognitive
categorization processes in performance evaluation: Confirmatory tests of two models. Journal of Organizational
Behavior, 12, 1–20.
Little, T. D., Cunningham, W. A., Shahar, G., & Widaman, K. F.
(2002). To parcel or not to parcel: Exploring the question:
Weighing the merits. Structural Equation Modeling, 9, 151–
173.
Leung, K., Au, A., Huang, X., Kurman, J., Niit, T., & Niit,
K. K. (2007). Social axioms and values: a cross-cultural
examination. European Journal of Personality, 21(2), 91–
111.
Liem, G. A. D., Martin, A. J., Nair, E., Bernardo, A. B., &
Prasetya, P. H. (2011). Content and structure of values in
middle adolescence: Evidence from Singapore, the Philippines, Indonesia, and Australia. Journal of Cross-Cultural
Psychology, 42(1), 146–154.
Liem, G. A. D., Martin, A. J., Porter, A. L., & Colmar, S. (2012).
Sociocultural antecedents of academic motivation and
achievement: Role of values and achievement motives in
achievement goals and academic performance. Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 15, 1–13.
Liem, A. D., & Nie, Y. (2008). Values, achievement goals, and
individual-oriented and social-oriented achievement motivations among Chinese and Indonesian secondary school
students. International Journal of Psychology, 43(5), 898–
903.
McCann Erikcson, Philippines (2001). The McCann Youth
Study 2000 Edition: A synopsis. Makati City, Philippines:
McCann Erickson Philippines.
Ogena, N. B. (1999). How are the Filipino youth changing?
The shifting lifestyles of our nation’s young, 1970s to 1990s.
Philippine Social Sciences Review, 56(1–4), 83–106.
Pe-Pua, R., & Protacio-Marcelino, E. (2000). Sikolohiyang
Pilipino (Filipino Psychology): A legacy of Virgilio G. Enriquez. Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 3, 49–71.
Ramirez, M. (1994). The Filipino worldview and values. In
T. B. Obusan & A. R. Enriquez (Eds.), The Filipino spiritual culture (Reprint Series No. 1, pp. 3–14). Quezon City,
Philippines: Mamamathala, Inc.
Santa Maria, M. (2002). Youth in Southeast Asia: Living within
the continuity of tradition and the turbulence of change.
In B. B. Brown, R. W. Larson, & T. S. Saraswathi (Eds.),
The World’s Youth: Adolescence in eight regions of the globe
(pp. 171–206). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University
Press.
Schwartz, S. H. (1992). Universals in the content and structure
of values: Theoretical advances and empirical tests in 20
countries. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in Experimental
Social Psychology (Vol. 25, pp. 1–65). New York: Academic
Press.
Schwartz, S. H. (2005). Robustness and fruitfulness of a
theory of universals in individual human values. In
A. Tamayo & J. B. Porto (Eds.), Valores e comportamento
nas organizações [Values and behavior in organizations]
(pp. 56–95). Petropolis, Brazil: Vozes.
JOURNAL OF TROPICAL PSYCHOLOGY
Downloaded from https:/www.cambridge.org/core. IP address: 88.99.165.207, on 17 Jun 2017 at 07:27:25, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at
https:/www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/jtp.2014.2
7
Allan B. I. Bernardo, Jose Antonio R. Clemente, Gregory Arief D. Liem
Schwartz, S. H. (2006). Basic human values: Theory, measurement, and applications. Revue française de sociologie, 47(4),
249–288.
Schwartz, S. H. & Bardi, A. (2001). Value hierarchies across
cultures: Taking a similarities perspective. Journal of CrossCultural Psychology, 32(3), 268–290.
Schwartz, S. H. & Bilsky, W. (1987). Toward a universal psychological structure of human values. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 53(3), 550–562.
Schwartz, S. H., Cieciuch, J., Vecchione, M., Davidov, E.,
Fischer, R., Beierlein, C., . . . & Konty, M. (2012). Refining
the theory of basic individual values. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 103, 663–688.
8
Schwartz, S. H., Melech, G., Lehman, A., Burgess, S., Harris,
M., & Owens, V. (2001). Extending the cross-cultural validity of the theory of basic human values with a different method of measurement. Journal of Cross-Cultural
Psychology, 32(5), 519–542.
Talisayon, S. D. (1994). Distinct elements of Filipino values: Cross-national comparisons. In T. B. Obusan &
A. R. Enriquez (Eds.), The Filipino Spiritual Culture
(Reprint Series No. 1, pp. 39–48). Quezon City, Philippines:
Mamamathala, Inc.
Tarry, H., & Emler, N. (2007). Attitudes, values and moral
reasoning as predictors of delinquency. British Journal of
Developmental Psychology, 25, 169–183.
JOURNAL OF TROPICAL PSYCHOLOGY
Downloaded from https:/www.cambridge.org/core. IP address: 88.99.165.207, on 17 Jun 2017 at 07:27:25, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at
https:/www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/jtp.2014.2