GHENT UNIVERSITY Faculty of Medicine Department of Psychiatry and Medical Psychology Traits and Eating Disorders: associations with cognitive and behavioural characteristics Thesis submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor in Medical Sciences Myriam Vervaet Ghent, 2005-07-08 Promotor: Prof. Dr. K. Audenaert CONTENT Introduction Part I : ‘Dieting’ and ‘weight concerns’ as risk factors in the development of eating disorders ...................................................................................................................................................... 8 1. Introduction ………………………………………………………………………………………………………....8 2. Binge eating in obese patients five years after treatment .................................................................................................. 10 3. Weight concerns and eating patterns in schoolboys and schoolgirls 4. Eating-style and weight-concerns in young females ................................................................................ 20 .......................................................................................................... 34 Part II : The cognitive psychology of eating disorders: personality, ................................................................................................................................ 45 cognitions and behaviour Chapter 1: Theoretical background 1. Definition and epidemiology ............................................................................................................................45 .............................................................................................................................................. 45 2. The complexity of the ED diagnosis 2.1. Categorical classification .................................................................................................................................. 48 ..................................................................................................................................... 48 2.2. Dimensional classification and co-morbidity .................................................................................................... 50 3. Aetiology as a dynamic process: from trait- (vulnerability) to state- (eating disorder category) dependent characteristics ..................................................................................................................................................................................................... 53 3.1. Introduction ......................................................................................................................................................... 53 3.2. The relationship of low self-esteem and body-dissatisfaction 3.3. Perfectionism as a core trait ....................................................................... 55 .............................................................................................................................. 59 3.4. Perception of life events and the coping with life-events by individuals, characterized by low self-esteem and clinical perfectionism .............................................................................................................. 63 4. Information processing in eating-disordered patients ............................................................................................................. 66 4.1. Neuropsychological processes ................................................................................................................................ 66 4.2. Cognitive processes…………………………………………………………………………………… 69 4.3. Information processing and emotional arousal………………………………………………………… 72 4.4. From schemata to behaviour…………………………………………………………………………… 75 5. A hypothetical model for the prediction of eating-disordered behaviour ............................................................................ 80 Chapter 2: Personality characteristics as trait-dependent vulnerability: study results ........................................................................................................................................................................101 1. Binge eating disorder and non-purging bulimia: more similar than different? ............................................................101 2. Is ‘drive for thinness’ in anorectic patients associated with personality characteristics? .............................................122 3. Cognitive and behavioural characteristics are associated with personality dimensions in patients with eating disorders ...........................................................................................................................................138 4. Personality-related characteristics in restricting versus binging and purging eating disordered patients ..............................................................................................................................................................166 5. Attempted suicide and personality in eating disorders. .........................................................................................................174 Chapter 3: Integration of personality characteristics in a psychobiological model of eating disorders ......................................................................................................................................................... 195 Part III : Further study of the psychobiological model of eating ..........................................................................................................................................................................228 disorders 1. Introduction………………………………………………………………………………………………………228 2. Decreased 5-HT2a receptor binding in patients with anorexia nervosa .......................................................................229 3. Binding potential of cortical 5-HT2A receptors is not different in bulimia nervosa patients and healthy volunteers .............................................................................................................................................................................236 General Conclusions ...........................................................................................................................................239 Nederlandse samenvatting ..........................................................................................247 BIJLAGE: GEBRUIKTE AFKORTINGEN Ingesloten publicaties: 1. Vervaet M & van Heeringen C (1995). Binge Eating in Obese Patients Five Years after Treatment. Eating Disorders: The Journal of Treatment and Prevention. Vol. 3 : 3, pp. 229-236. 2. Vervaet M, van Heeringen C, & Jannes C (1998). Weight Concerns and Eating Patterns in Schoolboys and -Girls. Eating Disorders: The Journal of Treatment and Prevention, 6, 41-51. 3. Vervaet M & van Heeringen C (2000). Eating-style and weight concerns in young females. Eating Disorders: The Journal of Treatment and Prevention. Vol. 8 : 233-240. 4. Vervaet M, van Heeringen C, & Audenaert K (2004). Psychological differences in Binge Eating Disorder and Bulimia Non-Purging patients. European Eating Disorders Review, 12 (1), 27-33. 5. Vervaet M, van Heeringen C, & Audenaert K (2004). Is Drive for Thinness in Anorectic Patients Associated with Personality Characteristics. European Eating Disorders Review, 12, 6, 375-379. 6. Vervaet M, Audenaert K, & van Heeringen C (2003). Cognitive and behavioural characteristics are associated with personality dimensions in patients with eating disorders. European Eating Disorders Review, 11 (5), 363-379. 7. Vervaet M, van Heeringen C, & Audenaert K (2004). Personality-related characteristics in restricting versus binging and purging eating disordered patients. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 45 (1), 37-43. 8. Vervaet M, van Heeringen C, & Audenaert K (Submitted). Attempted suicide and personality in eating disorders. Journal of Affective Disorders. 9. Audenaert K, Van Laere K, Dumont F, Vervaet M, Goethals I, Slegers G, Mertens J, van Heeringen C, Dierckx R (2003). Decreased 5-HT2a receptor binding in patients with anorexia nervosa. European Journal of Nuclear Medicine, 44, 163-169. 10. Goethals I, Vervaet M, Van De Wiele C, Audenaert K, Slegers G, van Heeringen C, Dierckx R (2004). Binding potential of cortical 5-HT2A receptors is not different in bulimia nervosa patients and healthy volunteers. American Journal of Psychiatry. 161:10, 1916-1918. Voorwoord Dit wetenschappelijk werk kwam tot stand onder leiding van Prof. van Heeringen wiens professionele onuitputtelijkheid bijzonder stimulerend werkt, niet alleen bij de realisatie van dit werk. Aansluitend wens ik Prof. Audenaert te bedanken voor zijn bereidwilligheid om het promotorschap op zich te nemen. Beiden zijn voor mij meer dan waardevolle collega’s. Bovendien getuigen hun persoonlijkheden van de realiteit dat dwang en impulsiviteit ook heel gezonde kenmerken kunnen zijn en vruchtbare interacties kunnen opleveren. Tevens wil ik alle leden van mijn begeleidings- en examencommissie bedanken voor hun waardevolle vragen en opmerkingen. Speciale dank gaat uit naar Prof. Jansen, die als hoogleraar eetstoornissen mij niet alleen reeds vele jaren boeide maar ook telkens weer verraste met haar originele onderzoeksvragen. Dat uitzonderlijke psychologen ook snel carrière kunnen maken wordt ontegensprekelijk door Prof. De Soete bewezen. Hij was nog student toen hij mij al leerde hoe ik een doctoraat moest schrijven. Blijkbaar met succes, gezien het voor herhaling vatbaar was. Een gezamenlijke schrijfopdracht vijftien jaar geleden en gezamenlijke pogingen om te stoppen met roken eindigden steevast in bijzonder gezellige etentjes met Prof. Kaufman. Met Prof. Thiery ontwikkelde ik in al die jaren een zeer dierbare band, als hoogleraar Medische Psychologie, als excellente deskundige in de Neuropsychologie, als toegewijd en competent behandelaar en tenslotte (hopelijk) ook als vriend. Dat Prof. Colardyn bovenop zijn uitgebreide professionele opdracht deze psychologische brok onderzoekswerk te verwerken kreeg, was het gevolg van zijn, door ons zeer gewaardeerd, voormalig diensthoofdschap. Bovendien was hij als hoofdarts steeds bereid tot luisteren en helpen als wij met onze patiënten weer diep in de moeilijkheden zaten, met de snelheid en de flexibiliteit die hem zo kenmerkt. Ook Prof. Van Cauwenberge heeft vaderlijk de verantwoordelijkheid als onze vakgroepvoorzitter opgenomen in tijden van hoge nood (en die was er meermaals). Van bij mijn start in deze faculteit tot nu heeft hij meermaals zijn psychologische kwaliteiten getoond bij de verzuchtingen van een vreemde eend tussen al die artsen-specialisten. Dat Prof. Kips momenteel mee beleidslijnen uittekent ligt in het verlengde van zijn capaciteiten om zowel structureel als inhoudelijk de puntjes op de i te zetten. Ook hem wil ik bedanken voor de vele gesprekken, de doorverwijzingen van patiënten en zijn toestemming om in de examencommissie te zetelen. En tenslotte mijn dank aan Prof. Debacquer die meermaals mijn obese databank bekeek en probeerde via SPSS het juiste dieet te vinden. In de voorgeschiedenis van dit proefschrift waren Prof. Evrard en Prof. Van Oost de sleutelfiguren bij mijn eerste doctoraat over eetstoornissen. Daarnaast werd ik positief geconditioneerd door Prof. Vandereycken, dé pionier in het onderzoek en behandeling van eetstoornissen in Vlaanderen. Ook hen wil ik nu bedanken. Hun degelijke wetenschappelijke basis gaven mij het vertrouwen om in de faculteit Geneeskunde een plaats te zoeken en te krijgen dank zij Prof. Jannes. Als geboeid wetenschapper kon ik echter niet zonder de kliniek. Het behandelen van eetstoornissen kan enkel door en met een multidisciplinair team dat via een duurzaam en intensief engagement de wondere wereld van deze patiëntengroep probeert te begrijpen. De bouwstenen voor een gespecialiseerde afdeling werden op de dienst Endocrinologie gelegd. Bij deze wens ik Dr. Deslypere en het verpleegkundig team onder leiding van Bea van harte te bedanken voor de vele uren die zij extra spendeerden aan onze probleemmeisjes. Het was de bakermat voor onze huidige 21-bedden tellende eetkliniek. In 1998 verhuisden we naar de afdeling Psychiatrie. Van in het begin was daar een bijzondere grote inzet en zonder deze kon dit werk nooit gemaakt worden. Siska, Tina, Bart, Katrien, Nele, Rita en alle verpleegkundigen ben ik bijzonder dankbaar en ik geef met gerust gemoed het roer door aan Dr. Katrien Bernagie die de hospitalisatie-eenheid in de toekomst zal leiden. Bijzondere dank gaat uit naar Lieve Rousseau, die haar logistieke, esthetische en psychologische kwaliteiten weet te combineren in een relativerende stijl die mij altijd opfleurt. Ook Betty die ons allen buffert tegen de vaak overspoelende telefoons en afspraken. Samen met Brigitte vormden zij met zijn drieën bovendien de trouwe achterhoede in moeilijke jaren. Tot slot gaat mijn dank naar mijn familie. Ik ben de laatste jaren te veel en zeer belangrijke mensen verloren, waardoor mijn werk vaak mijn troost werd. Mijn moeder wil ik bedanken omdat ze ondanks haar gevleugelde uitspraak “dat ze niet begrijpt waarom ze haar kinderen zo lang heeft laten studeren om daarna zo hard te moeten werken” toch accepteert dat zij ons daardoor minder ziet. Mijn zus No die mij al 40 jaar probeert ‘in balans’ te krijgen: “meer sport, meer natuur, meer buiten”, en Isidoor die steeds geboeid informeert naar de fenomenologie van onze patiënten en ondertussen zelf ‘mindful’ probeert te worden. Ook mijn ongelooflijke kranige en mature nichten Ruth en Karlien, die het wetenschappelijke niveau in de toekomst zullen ondersteunen, wil ik hierbij extra bedanken. En tenslotte Elise: bij de finalisering van mijn eerste doctoraat zat zij op mijn schoot als baby en nu zat zij achter haar computer Spaans te leren of Italiaans te sms-en. Haar doorzettingsvermogen in combinatie met haar grote tolerantie zorgden ervoor dat ik mij als moeder nooit schuldig hoefde te voelen. Gent, 20 juni 2005 Introduction Recent models in psychiatry explain psychopathology as the consequence of an interaction between stressors (eliciting factors) and a predisposition or vulnerability (predisposing factors), which is expressed through (problem) behaviour. Due to its reinforcing properties, such behaviour may act as a maintaining factor and may become a symptom. A disorder may thus be constituted of maintaining factors. The vulnerability can be caused by genetic or developmental influences, and modified by environmental and psychosocial variables. Vulnarability is caused by neurobiological mechanisms, in particular by neuroanatomical brain circuits with their neurophysiological and neuropsychological characteristics. This vulnerability, however, becomes not necessarily manifest during all periods of life. Eating disorders occur predominantly during adolescence or early adulthood, probably because this transition period is characterised by biological, psychological and social changes. The experience and necessity of change can be very stressful for an individual, especially for an anxious individual. In addition, ‘differentiating’, in terms of individuality/personality, becomes a very important process during adolescence. Indeed, conformity through following the rules of caregivers, i.e. parents, is predominantly the adaptive behavioural pattern during childhood. However, during adulthood, individuals are expected to formulate their own goals and to direct their own behaviour in relation to chosen values and norms. This demand can elicit dysfunctional coping behaviour, such as ‘creating a perfect body’ with divergent meanings and motives. Thus, an extreme need for control can sustain the self-starvation in anorectic patients, or the fear of rejection can motivate a bulimic girl to strive for ideal body shapes. Over the past few decades, eating disorders have emerged as clinical problems of growing relevance, due to their increasing prevalence, the substantial somatic and psychiatric co-morbidity, increased mortality, frequent relapse, and common failure of treatment. With regard to treatment, and from a cognitive-behavioural point of view, maintaining factors were the first target. The over-evaluation of body shape and weight has been described as the ‘core psychopathology’ of clinical eating disorders (Fairburn, Cooper, & Shafran, 2003). Dietary restraint is the most obvious behavioural expression of this over-evaluation. In the nineties, the cultural standard of beauty ideals was assumed to be associated with thinness in Western-societies, thus triggering weight concerns and behaviours such as dieting, particularly among young women and girls. The terms ‘dietary restraint’ and ‘restrained eating’ refer to a tendency to consciously restrict food intake in an effort to maintain weight or produce weight-loss (Herman and Polivy, 1975). Dieters display the tendency to overeat when anxious or stressed, whereas normal, unrestrained individuals tend to eat less when anxious (Rutledge and Linden, 1998). Among dieters, overeating can be induced by a dysphoric mood, alcohol consumption, and conditions, in which restrained eaters believe that they have broken their diet-rules by consuming a preload of food. This phenomenon is called ‘disinhibition’ or ‘counterregulation’. Explanations for this effect range from physiological mechanisms, i.e. via a dissociation from conditioned satiety signals, to distraction from distress and learned helplessness (Polivy and Herman, 1999). Additional psychological features associated with restrained eating include exaggerated responsiveness to nutritional or food-related cues (Tuschl, 1990), increased distractibility and emotionality (Herman and Polivy, 1975; Polivy et al., 1978), and depression and anxiety (French and Jeffery, 1994). Physiological consequences of dieting have also been noted, such as elevated triglyceride levels (Laessle et al., 1989) and weight cycling, which in itself has been associated with a higher risk of morbidity (Lissner et al., 1991). Unsuccessful dieters show poorer impulse regulation and more weight fluctuation, and have higher levels of perfectionism and drive for thinness when compared to successful dieters (Van Strien, 1997). They also are more prone to use dietary practices such as counting calories, eating low-calorie foods, and avoiding certain foods. In contrast, dieters who exert more enduring restraint tend to take smaller portions, and eat more slowly (Westenhoefer, 1991). The relationship between body dissatisfaction and self-esteem on the one hand and unsuccessful dieting on the other hand is less clear. Appearance-motivated dieters are at particular risk of eating pathology. These dieters mainly are younger women, who are characterized by a low self-esteem, body dissatisfaction, and a strong desire to change their bodies despite their normal weight. However, it remains to be demonstrated whether it is the perception that one is “dieting” (the cognitive aspect), or the actual decrease in caloric consumption and resulting physiological changes that disrupts normal eating (Putterman & Linden, 2004). The first three papers in Part 1 of this thesis describe aspects of the association between the risk factors ‘weight concerns’ and ‘dieting’ and the development of eating disorders. Without denying the importance of these eliciting and maintaining factors, the main goal of this thesis was to explore the vulnerability for eating disorders, i.e. to study predisposing factors. Therefore, a major research question for the studies as described in this thesis addressed, first the extent to which a predisposition to eating disorders can be described in terms of personality dimensions, and secondly, the expression of such a predisposition through cognitive and behavioural characteristics and their interactions, which are of particular interest for eating disorders (see figure 1). PREDISPOSING FACTORS Psychobiological dimensions PERSONALITY ELICITING ENVIRONMENT FACTORS MAINTAINING FACTORS COGNITIVE PROBLEM SCHEMATA BEHAVIOUR - feelings o f uselessness - feelings of loneliness and emptiness STATE - BMI - eating behaviour ~ CLINICAL-PHENOMENOLOGICAL type of eating disorder (DSM-IV) Figure 1: Model of the development of disturbed eating behaviour Personality is commonly described as a stable organisation of psychobiological structures reflecting temperamental and character dimensions. Rutter (1987) has defined temperament as “simple, non-motivational, non-cognitive stylistic approaches that are heritable and likely rooted in underlying neurobiological processes”. Although biologically driven temperamental constructs motivate and restrain behaviour, the expression of the same temperamental constructs is modified by experiences (Kagan, Arcus, & Snidman, 1993). Temperament refers to automatic emotional responses to experience that are moderately heritable and stable throughout life. In contrast, character refers to self-concepts and individual differences in goals and values, which influence voluntary choices, intentions, and the meaning of what is experienced in life. Dimensions of character are influenced by sociocultural learning, and mature in progressive steps throughout life. Behavioural genetic research has indicated that nonshared environmental effects, i.e. experiences that are not shared by siblings in the same family, have the greatest non-genetic effect on the development of psychopathology (Kendler, Neale, Kessler, Heath, & Eaves, 1992). These dimensions of personality interact with each other in order to motivate adaptation to life experiences and to influence susceptibility to emotional and behavioural disorders. Information processing characteristics are determined by these temperamental and character structures, and result in schemata. A schema is defined as a mental structure that consists of a stored domain of knowledge, which interacts with the processing of new information (Williams, Watts, MacLeod, & Mathews, 1997). It is a mental ‘filter’, which is shaped by previous experiences and which colours subsequent interpretations. A schema is rich in meaning, and represents much more than a single belief. Schemata comprise ‘meaning’, with its physical, emotional, verbal, visual, acoustic, kinetic, olfactory, tactile and kinaesthetic features. A schema contains information about the self, the others and the surrounding world, and directs our perception, interpretation and memory. Daily experiences activate schemata, which elicit corresponding feelings and behaviours. While a behaviour is observable, cognition is the quality of knowing which includes perceiving, judging, sensing, reasoning, and imaging” (Weisenberg, 1994). It should be mentioned that this “knowing” can take place inside or outside the realm of attention. Research findings regarding the application of this theoretical model to eating disorders are described in Part 2, followed by an elaboration of the research hypothesis. Data on 800 eating disordered patients are reported and compared with previous research findings. Furthermore, particular attention is given to the correlation between personality dimensions and three cognitive characteristics, which are defined as core characteristics in the main trans-diagnostic models of eating disorders (Fairburn, Cooper, & Shafran, 2003; Serpell & Troop, 2003). These cognitive factors, i.e. body dissatisfaction, perfectionism and interpersonal distrust, were measured using the Eating Disorder Inventory (EDI; Garner, Olmstead, & Polivy, 1983). The personality model as used in the studies, which constitute this thesis, is psychobiological in nature, and serves as a basis for the study of the neurobiological correlates of these psychological characteristics. Part 3 describes two such studies. Conclusions and suggestions for further research are provided in the final chapter. References Fairburn CG, Cooper Z, & Shafran R (2003). Cognitive behaviour therapy for eating disorders: A ‘transdiagnostic’ theory and treatment. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 41, 509-528. French SA, & Jeffery RW (1994). Consequences of dieting to lose weight: Effects on physical and mental health. Health Psychology, 13: 195-212. Garner DM, Olmstead MP, & Polivy J (1983). Development and validation of a multidimensional Eating Disorder Inventory for anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 2, 15-24. Herman CP, & Polivy J.(1975). Anxiety, restraint and eating behaviour. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 84, 666-672. Kagan J, Arcus D, & Snidman N (1993). The idea of temperament: Where do we go from here? In R.Plomin & G.E. McClearn (Eds.), Nature, nurture and psychology. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Kendler KS, Neale MC, Kessler RC, Heath AC, & Eaves LJ (1992). The genetic epidemiology of phobias in women: The interrelationship of agoraphobia, social phobia, situational phobia, and simple phobia. Archives of General Psychiatry, 49, 273-281. Laessle RG, Tushel RJ, Kotthaus BC, & Pirke KM (1989). Behavioral and biological correlated of dietary restraint in normal life. Appetite 12: 83-94. Lissner L, Odell PM, D’Agostino RB, Stokes J, Kreger BE. Belanger AJ & Brownell KD (1991). Variability of body weight and health outcomes in the Framingham population. N. Eng. J. Med. 324-: 1839-1844. Polivy J, & Herman CP (1999). Distress and eating: why do dieters overeat? International Journal of Eating Disorders, 26, 153-164. Polivy J, Herman CP, & Warsh S (1978). Internal and external components of emotionality in restrained and unrestrained eaters. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 87: 497-504. Putterman E, & Linden W (2004). Appearance versus Health: Does the reason for dieting affect dieting behavior? Journal of Behavioral Medicine, Vol. 27, 2, 185-204. Rutledge T, & Linden W (1998). To eat or not to eat: Affective and physiological mechanisms in the stress-eating relationship. J. Behav. Med. 21: 221-240. Rutter M (1987). Temperament, personality, and personality disorder. British Journal of Psychiatry, 150, 443-458. Serpell L, & Troop N (2003). Psychological factors. In : J. Treasure, U. Schmidt & E. van Furth (Eds), Handbook of Eating Disorders. Second Edition, Wiley & Sons, Chichester. Tuschl RJ, Laessle RG, Platte P, & Pirke KM (1990). Differences in food-choice frequencies between restrained and unrestrained eaters. Appetite 14: 9-13. Van Strien T (1997). The concurrent validity of a classification of dieters with low versus high susceptibility toward failure of restraint. Addictive Behaviors, 22, 587-597. Weisenberg M (1994). Cognitive aspects of pain. In P.D. Wall, & R. Melzack (Eds.), Textbook of pain (pp. 275-289). New York: Churchill Livingstone. Williams JMG, Watts FN, Mac Leod C, & Mathews A (1997). Cognitive psychology and emotional disorders (2nd ed.). Chichester: Wiley. PART I ‘Dieting’ and ‘weight concerns’ as risk factors in the development of eating disorders 1. Introduction The core psychopathological feature of eating disordered patients is their over-evaluation of shape and weight. In contrast with the probably complex and badly understood cause of the disease, the symptoms are clear forward, measurable and uniting all patients. Moreover, the physical abnormalities in these patients appear to be largely secondary to their disturbed eating habits, and the majority of the somatic symptoms are reversed by restoration of healthy eating habits. Consequently, the somatic treatment seems to be easy, and merely a question of willpower and clear indications. At the same time young adolescents grow up, while living in a society with television soaps, commercials and computergames in which thin female bodies are idealized. The consequent pressure of ‘looking good’ often leads to a ‘normative discontent’ (Rodin, Silberstein, & Striegel-Moore, 1985), due to which many girls worry about their body shape and try to induce a negative energy balance. A dramatic consequence is the prevailing view, even among medical professionals, that eating disorders are self-inflicted and that they are an expression of temporary immaturity. Disturbed eating patterns can indeed be placed on a continuum from harmless to pathological, but this unscientific opinion ignores the fact that typical eating disorders are severe and often chronic disorders associated with high levels of physical and psychological co-morbidity and poor quality of life. For example, the mortality rate in anorectic inpatients is indeed twice as high as the rate in other psychiatric inpatients. Equally important is the question whether restricting behaviour can induce a psychiatric syndrome. Bearing in mind the study of Keys (1946), it was hypothesized that too strict dieting may have iatrogenic effects, such as inducing binge eating and other eating-related pathology. Following this line of reasoning, the first part of this thesis includes three studies describing the presence of disturbed eating patterns among risk groups such as obese patients (after treatment with strict diets), fashion models and young adolescents. These articles were published between 1995 and 2000 and formed the start for the search to distinguish between dieting as symptom of psychopathology and dieting as a means of reaching a healthy weight. 2. Binge eating in obese patients five years after treatment.1 Abstract Five years after treatment for obesity, combining cognitive-behavioral treatment with a very low calorie diet (VLCD) or a low calorie diet (LCD), the eating behavior of 112 female patients was studied. At the time of reassessment most of the women showed a restrained eating style combined with bingeing. Significantly more patients in the VLCD-group than in the LCD-group reported bingeing. More than half of the successfully treated women (BMI ≤ 30) reported binge eating. No association between treatment success and a restrained eating style was found. Habitual dieters show marked overcompensation in eating behavior in a way that is similar to the bingeing in eating disorders (Polivy & Herman, 1985; Wardle, 1987). But binge eating does not occur in all dieters (Tuschl, 1990). More-over, in some people binge eating is a transitory phenomenon, while for others it progresses to a chronic condition. Nevertheless there is a close connection between dietary restraint and binge eating. Hence it is no surprise that binge eating is a common problem among the obese (Hudson et al., 1988; Kolotkin et al., 1987; Marcus & Wing, 1987). Binge eating may also be a serious complication of treatment. Marcus and Wing (1987) found that between 20% and 46% of obese individuals participating in a behavioral weight control program reported binge eating. The quantity of food consumed and the duration of episodes were similar to those in patients with bulimia nervosa. Garner and Wooley (1991) suggest that excessive dieting associated with overweight may induce binge eating, which in turn leads to increased caloric intake and greater adiposity. Since treatment of the obese according to the addiction model includes intensifying restraint rather than just normalizing food intake (Bemis, 1985; Vandereycken, 1990; Wardle, 1987), it may have iatrogenic effects. Of further interest is the finding that both obese and lean dieters eat more when depressed, a pattern which is reversed in non-dieters (Baucom & Aiken, 1981; Polivy & Herman, 1985). Therefore, dieting may cause depression which in turn makes the individual more vulnerable to bouts of overeating. 1 Vervaet, M. & van Heeringen, C. (1995). Binge eating in obese patients five years after treatment. Eating Disorders The Journal of Treatment and Prevention, 3, pp.229-236. Telch, Agras, and Rossiter (1988) found that binge eating becomes significantly more common when the degree of overweight increases. A significant positive relationship between the degree of overweight and binge severity has also been demonstrated by other researchers (Kolotkin et al. 1987; Marcus et al., 1985). These findings suggests that individuals who binge eat may be at greater risk for overweight. Other studies have indicated that obese binge eaters may have a poorer treatment response in standard behavioral weight loss programs than obese nonbinge eaters (Keefe et al., 1984) because of a higher rate of dropout and a more rapid regain of weight (Marcus, Wing, & Hopkins, 1988). The current study aimed at the assessment of differences in the prevalence of binge eating between obese patients who followed a cognitive-behavioral treatment combined with a low calorie diet and those with a very low calorie diet. The association between binge eating and treatment outcome was also investigated. Methods We studied 260 obese women (definition: Body Mass Index of 30 or more) who followed a multidisciplinary treatment according to two different programs. In the first group, treatment included both a very low calorie diet (VLCD, 400-600 Kcal) and cognitive behavior group therapy for weight loss during one year. Treatment began with a six-week VLCD fasting program, followed by gradual refeeding. The second group started with a low calorie diet (LCD, 800-1,200 Kcal) during several months before refeeding, on an individually basis. During the initial three months, sessions occurred weekly, then biweekly for the next six months, and monthly for the final three months of treatment. Group sessions lasted for 60 minutes and individual sessions for 30 minutes. Both kinds of treatment were supplied by a female behavior therapist. The treatment included three basic elements. First, information about medical topics (e.g. the risks of obesity, influence of different diets) was presented by an endocrinologist who also followed the medical evolution of the patients. Fenfluramine was given in case of great carbohydrate cravings. Second, patients were engaged in an exercise program included swimming, jogging, and fitness training on a regular basis (minimally three times a week for one hour). As to the specific diet, patients learned how to purchase, compose, and prepare meals that were different from fat- and carbohydrate-rich ones. Third, we applied a form of cognitive-behavior therapy based on self-monitoring, stimulus control, stress management, cognitive restructuring, relapse prevention training, enlargement of social support and self-esteem, and amelioration of body image. The final aim of the program was to reach a sense of self-control, especially in eating situations. The patient group consisted of 78 females treated in an outpatient clinic in Antwerp and 182 female patients who were consecutively admitted to the Unit for Eating Disorders of the Department of Endocrinology, University Hospital, Gent. Five years after treatment, a questionnaire was sent to all 110 obese women who had participated in the VLCD treatment and to the 150 in the LCD group. The following characteristics were monitored: age, situation of living, activity, weight before treatment and after 1 year and 5 years, body mass index (BMI), highest weight ever, desired weight, use of laxatives and/or anorectic drugs, vomiting, and eating-behavior. Subjects were also asked to fill out the Dutch eating questionnaire (Van Strien et al., 1986). For this questionnaire the results were compared to the norm tables of obese women (Van Strien et al., 1986) and divided in three groups: low (L), mean (M) and high (H) correspondence group, using the cut-off-scores shown in Table 1. Table 1: Cut-off Scores from the Dutch Eating Questionnaire Restrained eating External eating Eating with clear emotions Eating with diffuse emotions Totally emotional eating Low Mean High <2.7 <2.4 <1.1 <1.7 <1.1 2.7-3.4 2.4-3.1 1.1-2.9 1.7-3.3 1.1-2.9 >3.4 >3.1 >2.9 >3.7 >2.9 Results Of the total of 260 women, data on 112 patients (43 %) were available for analysis, including 56 patients of the VLCD group (group 1) and 56 females of the LCD group (group 2). To investigate the relationship between frequency of binge eating and treatment outcome, the latter was dichotomized as successful (BMI ≤ 30) or not successful (BMI > 30). The following picture of the eating behavior emerged from questionnaires: 74 % ate between the meals, including 49 % in the evening; 14 % skipped breakfast, 5 % lunch, and another 5 % dinner; 11 % ate a lot of sweets, 28 % regularly and 56 % sometimes; most of the women (71 %) preferred sweet to salt; 37 % ate alone regularly, 47 % thought a lot about food during the day, and 26 % counted calories. Loss of control while eating was reported by 74 women (66%), including 12 women (11 %) who binged daily. In order to get information about the difference of 'objective' and 'subjective' binges, patients were asked to quantify their binges: 21 women (19%) reported binges involving the intake of more than 2,000 Kcal, while 31 (28 %) reported the intake of about 1,000 Kcal; 17 (15%) reported binges of less than 500 Kcal, which can therefore be called ‘subjective’ binges. Table 2: Bingeing in VLCD and LCD Bingeing Occurrence VLCD n % 45 80.4 (n = 56) n 30 % 53.6 Frequency LCD (n = 56) p* .0026 .0200 *never 11 19.6 26 46.4 *daily 7 12.5 5 8.9 *regularly 24 42.9 15 26.8 *sometimes 14 25 10 17.9 Quantity .1619 *0 Kcal 16 28.6 27 48.2 *< 500 Kcal 11 19.6 6 10.7 *> 1000 Kcal 18 32.1 13 23.2 *> 2000 Kcal 11 19.6 10 17.9 *Chi-square tests A significant difference in the frequency of bingeing between the VLCD and the LCD group was found. Of the VLCD women, 45 women (80.4%) reported bingeing versus 30 (53.6 %) LCD-women, as shown in Table 2. No significant difference between the treatment groups could be found for the quantity of binges. No indications for an association between the occurrence or frequency of bingeing and treatment outcome could be found (see Table 3). Table 3: Bingeing in Successful and Unsuccessful Treatment Outcome. Bingeing Occurrence BMI≤30 (n = 44) BMI>30 (n = 68) n % n % p* 25 56.8 50 73.5 .0663 frequency never .2729 19 43.2 18 26.5 daily 3 6.8 9 13.2 regularly 13 29.5 26 38.2 sometimes 9 20.4 15 22.1 Quantity .0242 0 Kcal 19 43.2 18 26.5 < 500 Kcal 7 15.9 10 14.7 > 1000 Kcal 8 18.2 23 33.8 > 2000 Kcal 5 11.4 16 23.5 * Chi-square tests However, a significant difference between successfully and unsuccessfully treated patients emerged for the kind of binges : objective binges (involving the intake of at least 1,000 Kcal) were reported more often by women in the non-successful group. No significant differences between the successfully and unsuccessfully treated groups could be found for the use of other methods to influence the body weight: 34 patients (30 %) of the total group used appetite suppressing drugs, 9 patients (8 %) reported vomiting, and 35 patients (31 %) used laxatives. Table 4: High scores on the Dutch Eating Questionnaire in VLCD and LCD. H-score VLCD (n = 56) LCD (n = 56) n % n % p* Restrained eating 28 52.8 27 50.0 .950 External eating 18 32.1 10 17.9 .120 Clear emotional eating 29 51.8 15 26.8 .004 Diffuse emotional eating 23 41.1 17 30.9 .037 Total emotional eating 32 57.1 17 30.4 .005 * Chi-square tests Of particular interest is the finding of a significant difference between the VLCD and the LCD groups on the indices for Emotional Eating of the Dutch Eating Questionnaire (df = 2; X2 = 10.277; p = 0.005). As shown in Table 4, 32 patients in the VLCD-group (57.1 %) had a high score versus 17 in the LCD-group (30.4 %). Only two VLCD-patients (3.6 %) had a low score on this item, versus nine LCDpatients (16.1 %). No significant differences between the treatment groups were found for the proportion of high scores on Restrained Eating, and External Eating. However, high scores on Eating by Clear Emotions and Eating by Diffuse Emotions were found more often in the VLCD group than in the LCD group. With respect to the association between high scores on the questionnaire and treatment outcome, it appeared that high scores on External Eating and Eating by Clear Emotions were more common among unsuccessfully treated patients (see Table 5). Table 5: High scores on the Dutch Eating Questionnaire in Successful and Unsuccessful Treatment Outcome. H-score BMI≤30 BMI>30 n % n % p* Restrained eating 27 65.8 28 42.4 .060 External eating 8 18.9 20 29.4 .001 Clear Emotional eating 11 25.0 33 48.5 .035 Diffuse emotional eating 11 25.6 29 42.6 .170 Total emotional eating 15 34.1 34 50.0 .252 *Chi-square tests Discussion Preceding a discussion of the results of this study, some methodological shortcomings should be addressed. First, the results are likely to be biased because follow-up data are known for only 43% of the patients. Second, because of the retrospective nature of the study no base line data on the occurrence and characteristics of bingeing before treatment were available. Thus, it cannot be concluded that bingeing has been induced by the treatment. Nevertheless, the differences between VLCD and LCD outcome seem to support the hypothesis that the degree of dieting determines the degree of loss of control over eating: loss of control was present in 80 % of the VLCD-patients and in 53 % of the LCD-group. Our results also suggest a link between emotional eating and bingeing. VLCD-patients more often reported bingeing, and also more commonly had high scores on items related to eating, because of clear or diffuse emotions. With respect to the association between treatment outcome and binge eating, we found that more than half of the successfully treated subjects reported bingeing, but objective binges occurred significantly less often. Although restrained eating was as common as in unsuccessfully treated women, the results suggest that successfully treated women lose less control in situations where they are externally or emotionally stimulated to eat. Anyway, the results from this study indicate that bingeing is a serious problem even several years after treatment for obesity. Many women in this study used laxatives and anorectic drugs, vomited, and dieted, without losing weight. One explanation for this failure can be found in their disturbed eating pattern: they skip meals, prefer sweets, eat alone regularly, and think a lot about food, but most of all they regularly lose control over their eating in emotional situations. In view of the finding that better treatment outcome is associated with less external and emotional eating, and with a smaller quantity of food intake in case of bingeing, the results of the current study indicate that the focus of treatment for obesity should be shifted from stimulating dieting to improving self-control. Finally, our follow-up data make the use of very low calorie diets - still very popular in Europe - indicate a rather questionable practice. References Baucom, D. H., & Aiken, P.A.(1981). Effect of depressed mood on eating among obese and nonobese dieting and nondieting persons. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 41, 577-585. Bemis, K.(1985). "Abstinence" and "nonabstinence" models for the treatment of bulimia. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 4, 389-406. Garner, D.W., & Wooley S.C.(1991). Confronting the failure of behavioral and dietary treatments for obesity. Clinical Psychology Review, Vol. 11, PP. 729-780. Hudson, J.I., Pope, H.G., Wurtman, J., Yurgelun-Todd, D., Mark, S., & Rosenthal, N.E.(1988). Bulimia in obese individuals: Relationship to normal-weight bulimia. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 176, 144-152. Keefe, P.H., Wyshogrod, D., Weinberger, E., & Agras, W.S. (1984). Binge eating and outcome of behavioral treatment of obesity: A preliminary report, Behaviour Research and Therapy, 22, 319-321. Kolotkin, R.L., Revis, E.S., Kirkley, B.G., & Janick, L. (1987). Binge eating in obesity: Associated MMPI characteristics. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 55, 872-876. Marcus, M.D., & Wing, R.R.(1987). Binge eating among the obese. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 9, 23-27. Marcus, M.D., Wing, R.R., & Hopkins, J.(1988). Obese binge eaters: Affect, cognitions, and response to behavioral weight control. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 56, 433-439. Marcus, M.D., Wing, R.R., & Lamparski, D.M.(1985). Binge eating and dietary restraint in obese patients. Addictive Behaviors, 10, 163-168. Polivy, J., & Herman, C.P.(1985). Dieting and bingeing: A causal analysis. American Psychologist, 40, 193-201. Telch, C.F., Agras, W.S., & Rossiter, E.M.(1988). Binge eating increases with increasing adiposity. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 7, 115-119. Telch, C.F., & Agras, W.S.(1993). The effects of a Very Low Calorie Diet on binge eating. Behavior Therapy, 34, 177-193. Tuschl, R.J.(1990). From dietary restraint to binge eating: Some theoretical considerations. Appetite, 14, 105-109. Van Strien, T., Frijters, J., Roosen, R., Knuiman-Mijl, D., & Defares, P.(1986). Eating behavior, personality traits and body mass in women. Agricultural University, Wageningen, The Netherlands. Vandereycken, W.(1990). The addiction model in eating disorders: Some critical remarks and a selected bibliography. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 9, 95-101. Wardle, J.(1987). Compulsive eating and dietary restraint. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 26, 47-55. 3. Weight Concerns and Eating Patterns in Schoolboys and -Girls2 In this study, weight concerns and eating patterns were assessed in a large group of school-aged adolescents by means of questionnaires. Girls far more commonly reported problems related to eating and body weight, restrained eating with counting of calories, and abuse of laxatives. Loss of control over eating was reported by more than half of the girls. The occurrence of eating problems and associated behavioral characteristics among girls clearly increased with age, even though young girls reported problems related to body weight and eating patterns to such an extent that the introduction of prevention programs should be considered in the first years of secondary school. Outcome studies indicate a substantially increased risk of medical and psychiatric disorders (Fombonne, 1995), and of premature death (Sullivan, 1995), among eating disordered patients. These findings underline the need for more effective preventive measures that should be based on the knowledge of factors that facilitate or predict the development of eating disorders. Longitudinal studies have shown that dissatisfaction with body shape is a major factor contributing to the development of eating disorders (Attie & Brooks-Gunn, 1989; Striegel-Moore, Silberstein, & Rodin, 1989). Therefore, it is alarming that recent surveys consistently demonstrate high levels of body dissatisfaction among adolescent girls despite normal actual weights (Attie & BrooksGunn, 1989; Huenemann, Shapiro, Hampton, & Mitchell, 1966; Paxton et al., 1991; Richards, Boxer, Petersen, & Albrecht, 1990). Consequently, it is not surprising that adolescent girls commonly engage in weight reducing behavior, most often by means of dieting. Weight reduction behavior was already reported to be common by the 1960s (Dwyer, Feldman, Seltzer, & Mayer, 1969; Huenemann et al., 1966; Nylander, 1971). However, the average weight of young women has tended to increase over the last 30 years (Gulliford, Rona, & Chinn, 1992; Shah, Hannan, & Jeffery, 1991), while the ideal body weight has decreased 15 kg over the same period (Kinoy, 1994). The relationship of dieting and development of eating disorders is not clear. Many studies have shown only a limited association between dieting and being overweight. For example, only half of the 2 Vervaet, M., van Heeringen, C., & Jannes, C. (1998). Eating Disorders. The Journal of Treatment and Prevention, 6,41-51. girls studied by Dwyer et al. (1969) were actually overweight. Individuals such as ballet dancers, student fashion models, and young athletes had higher scores on self-report measures of eating disturbances than individuals in the general population, and they developed clinically defined eating disorders at much higher frequencies (Attie & Brooks-Gunn, 1989; Garner, Garfinkel, Schwartz, & Thompson, 1980; Garner, Garfinkel, Rockert, & Olmstead, 1987; Hamilton, Brooks-Gunn, & Warren, 1985; Szmukler, Eisloer, Gillies, & Hayward, 1985). However, none of the authors mentioned in the studies cited above have said that dieting per se is a risk factor for eating disorders in these populations. Longitudinal studies of general population samples of school-aged girls also have demonstrated that dieters at the time of the first interview were seven times as likely as nondieters to develop an eating disorder by the time of the second interview 1 year later (Johnson-Sabine, Wood, Patton, Mann, & Wakeling, 1988; Patton, Johnson-Sabine, Wood, Mann, & Wakeling, 1990) . Recent studies indicate a difference in eating styles between men and women (Van Strien, 1994). "Restrained eating" and "emotional eating" are more commonly reported by women. However, in Western societies more men are overweight, and the typical male abdominal fat distribution is associated with a higher risk of coronary diseases than the typical female fat distribution (Van Gaal, Rillaerts, Creten, & De Leeuw, 1988). Therefore, the reported differences in eating styles and weight concerns according to gender appear not to be due to medical reasons. Next to an effect of gender, a limited number of studies have indicated a substantial effect of age on body dissatisfaction and weight concern. At the onset of puberty, girls experience a weight spurt as their body weight increases by 10 kg because of a substantial increase in body fat (Fombonne, 1995). This increase is accompanied by negative psychological states, particularly among early maturers (Dornbusch et al., 1984). On the contrary, during puberty boys experience an increase in weight and muscularity that may enhance their self-esteem. Boys perceive thinness rather negatively, as they generally prefer to be tall and muscular (Paxton et al., 1991). Thus, the currently available data indicate that weight concerns and disturbed eating patterns are common among adolescents and may provide clues to the prevention of eating disorders. However, when the results of distinct studies are combined it appears that gender and age also have to be taken into account to delineate specific subgroups with particularly increased risks of developing eating disorders. Therefore, this study aimed at the assessment of (a) weight concerns and eating patterns and (b) the effects of age and gender on these characteristics in a large sample of school-aged adolescents. Subjects and methods The study population consisted of students between 13 and 18 years of age who were attending four secondary schools in Gent, Belgium. Data were collected by means of two anonymous self-report questionnaires. Questionnaires were filled in by the students collectively at the same moment in each class, under the supervision of the first author. Items on the first questionnaire inquired about age, weight, gender, desired weight, use of anorectic drugs and laxatives, weight and eating problems (defined as self-criticism on weight and eating patterns: weight is perceived to be too high or too low and a feeling of lack of control over eating), vomiting, eating patterns, thinking about eating (degree of preoccupation), eating alone (eating in secret), counting calories, preference for sweet or salt, loss of control over eating, and the frequency and severity of this loss. We calculated body mass indexes (BMI = G/LXL) by using reported weights and heights. The ideal BMI was defined as a current BMI between 20 and 25, and we calculated the desired BMI (DBMI) by means of reported desired weights and heights. Subjects were also asked to fill out the Dutch Eating Questionnaire (Van Strien, Frijters, Roosen, Knuiman-i\tlijl, & Defares, 1986), which is based on the Eating Pattern Questionnaire (Wollersheim, 1970), the Questionnaire for Latent Obesity (Pudel, Metzdorff, & Oeting, 1975), and the Eating Behavior Inventory (O'Neil et al., 1979). The following subscales can be identified: Emotional Eating, Restrained Eating, and External Eating. We compared the scores on the questionnaire and on the subscales with those of nonobese controls after dividing the subjects into three subgroups-low (L), mean (M), and high (H)-using the cutoff scores (Van Strien et al., 1986) . Table 1 Occurrence of assessed characteristics Variable (total n = 745) BMI < 20 20 <BMI <= 25 BMI > 25 DBMI < 20 20 < DBMI <= 25 DBMI > 25 Weight problems Eating problems Vomiting Laxative abuse Skipping breakfast Skipping lunch Skipping dinner Eating between meals Eating alone Thinking about food during the day Counting calories Preference for sweet Perception of loss of control Restrained Eating scores > 2.38 Emotional Eating scores > 2.1 External Eating scores > 3.04 N 454 293 11 423 167 1 130 103 32 68 95 17 12 692 173 250 55 595 390 335 475 285 % 61 39 1 57 22 0.1 17 14 4 9 13 2 2 93 23 33 7 80 52 45 64 38 Note: BMI = body mass index; DBMI = desired body mass index We examined the effect of age by comparing two subject age groups: 13-15 years versus 16-18 years. We performed statistical analyses using Statistica (Statsoft, 1994). Differences between groups were tested by means of the Mann-Whitney U test for continuous variables and the chi-square test for the comparison of proportions. Results The questionnaires were filled in by 745 adolescents attending four secondary schools. Two schools were exclusively attended by girls (n = 370), one school was attended exclusively by boys (n = 161), and the fourth school was attended by students of both sexes (n = 214). The frequency distribution of the measured variables is shown in Table 1. Although 16% of the participants indicated weight problems and 13% eating problems, 39% had a current BMI between 20 and 25. In 60% of the participants the BMI was even lower than 20. As indicated by the reported desired BMI, 71 % wanted a BMI under the ideal BMI. Noteworthy was the common occurrence of "eating between meals" (87%), perception of loss of control (49%), restrained eating (42%), and emotional eating (60%). Loss of control was reported as occurring "sometimes" in 61 % of the subjects, "regularly" in 30%, and "frequently" in 8%. Table 2 shows the means of the current and desired BMIs for girls and boys and the scores for restrained, emotional, and external eating as measured by the Dutch Eating Questionnaire. Table 2 COMPARISON BETWEEN GIRLS AND BOYS Characteristic BMI DBMI Restrained Eating score External Eating score Emotional Eating score Girls (n=496) Boys (n=249) 19,5 18,7 2,5 2,8 2,6 19,2 19,7 1,6 2,9 1,7 Girls Eating problems Weight problems Loss of cntrol Use of laxatives Thinking a lot about food Counting calories Eating alone Vomiting p .105 .000 .000 .177 .000 Boys n % n % 87 107 275 59 168 50 97 25 17 21 54 8 22 6 19 5 16 20 109 9 78 5 73 7 6 7 41 1 10 1 27 3 .000 .000 .000 .000 .260 .000 .015 .085 Although no significant difference between girls and boys was found for the current BMI, the desired BMI among girls was significantly lower than among boys and was clearly lower than the ideal BMI. Furthermore, significant differences between boys and girls were found for restrained and emotional eating. Girls reported significantly more eating problems, weight problems, and loss of control over eating. They abused laxatives and counted calories significantly more than boys did; however, boys reported eating alone more than girls. Finally, no significant difference regarding thinking about eating between both groups could be demonstrated. Table 3 shows the mean scores and frequency distributions on the same characteristics for the two age groups among the girls. Older girls restrained significant more commonly and ate more commonly in emotional situations. No difference was found in regard to scores on the External Eating subscale. The mean score for older girls was high when compared to scores among the controls in the Van Strien et al. (1986) study. A significantly higher number of older girls reported eating problems, loss of control, Table 3. COMPARISON BETWEEN YOUNGER (13-15 YEARS) AND OLDER (16-18 YEARS) GIRLS Characteristic <15 years (n=139) BMI DBMI Restrained Eating score External Eating score Emotional Eating score >15 years (n=370) 18,3 17,5 2,1 2,8 2 20 19,1 2,7 2,8 2,9 <15 years Eating problems Weight problems Loss of control Use of laxatives Thinking a lot about food Counting calories Eating alone Vomiting p 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.822 0.000 >15 years n % n % 17 28 45 4 27 8 10 9 12 20 33 3 19 6 7 7 70 79 230 55 141 42 87 16 18 21 62 15 38 11 24 4 .040 .915 .000 .000 .000 .026 .000 .367 abuse of laxatives, thinking about food, counting calories, and eating alone when compared with the younger group. In both groups, 1 in 5 girls indicated a weight problem in spite of a normal mean BMI. There was a nonsignificant trend for a higher frequency of vomiting among younger girls than among older girls. We made a similar comparison for boys and, as shown in Table 4, a totally different picture emerged. First, the desired BMI was higher than the current BMI in both age groups. Younger boys showed a more pronounced restrained eating style than older boys, but these scores were normal for both groups when compared with controls (Van Strien et al., 1986). For emotional eating, scores were higher among older than among younger boys but were equally comparable to scores among controls. A significantly higher number of older boys than younger boys reported that they ate when they were externally or emotionally stimulated. With respect to eating patterns, significant differences between younger and older boys were found for thinking about food, eating alone, and vomiting. Table 5 shows a comparison of the numbers of students with high scores for restrained, external, and emotional eating for the total group and for the different age- and gender-specific groups. High scores for restrained and emotional eating were found more commonly among girls-more specifically, among older girls. Among boys, an effect of age could be Table 4. COMPARISON BETWEEN YOUNGER (13-15 YEARS) AND OLDER (16-18 YEARS) BOYS Characteristic Boys BMI DBMI Restrained Eating score External Eating score Emotional Eating score <15 years (n=146) >15 years (n=127) 18 18,4 1,7 2,8 1,6 20,7 21,2 1,5 3 1,8 <15 years Eating problems Weight problems Loss of control Use of laxatives Thinking a lot about food Counting calories Eating alone Vomiting p .000 .000 .005 .008 .040 >15 years n % n % 11 14 51 6 32 3 28 7 7 10 36 4 22 2 19 5 5 6 58 3 46 2 45 0 4 5 46 2 37 2 36 0 .203 .089 .071 .521 .001 .917 .000 .002 Table 5. COMPARISON BY AGE Girls subscale Boys n 133 171 288 % 26 35 57 Restrained Eating External Eating Emotional Eating n 21 49 25 Young Girls % 15 35 18 n 112 122 263 Restrained Eating External Eating Emotional Eating n 31 50 30 Young boys % 21 34 21 n 13 60 40 Restrained Eating External Eating Emotional Eating n 44 110 70 p % 16 40 26 .000 .099 .000 % 30 35 71 .000 .999 .000 % 10 47 31 .002 .010 .016 Older Girls Older boys observed on the occurrence of high scores on the Restrained Eating (more common among younger boys) and Emotional Eating (more common among older boys) subscales. Discussion The results of this large-scale survey of weight concerns and eating patterns among adolescents clearly indicate first that current and desired body weights (as measured by means of body mass index) very commonly are lower than ideal from a medical point of view. Therefore, it is not surprising that restrained eating is very commonly reported, and that, to a lesser extent, adolescents additionally report vomiting, laxative abuse, counting of calories, and skipping meals in order to lose weight. However, the results of this study indicate that a restrained eating style commonly occurs in association with emotional eating, because eating between meals, loss of control over eating, and eating under emotional circumstances were commonly reported. Second, the results show a marked effect of gender on these characteristics. Although girls and boys reported current and desired BMIs that are lower than ideal from a medical point of view, the desired BMI was lower than the current BMI among girls. On the contrary, boys indicated a higher desired than current BMI. When compared with boys, girls far more commonly reported problems related to eating and body weight, the occurrence of restrained eating with counting of calories, and abuse of laxatives. Loss of control over eating was reported by more than half of the girls. Third, an effect of age can be recognized. Among girls, the incidence of eating problems and associated behavioral characteristics as described above clearly increased with age. .Among boys the effect of age was less marked. It is noteworthy, however, that in spite of a higher desired than current BMI, restrained eating and vomiting were reported by boys but that the rate of these characteristics decreased with age. The occurrence of externally or emotionally stimulated eating increased with age. Before we discuss these results we should note that the study group is not representative of the general population, because only adolescents at higher educational levels were included. This selection bias may result in an overestimation of the occurrence of eating problems, as previous research has shown a positive correlation between rates of eating disorders and educational level (Fombonne, 1995). The overall picture emerging from this survey is that girls want to lose weight, whereas boys indicate a desire to gain weight. Current BMI does not differ between boys and girls, but girls show. more concern about their body weight and are at a clearly increased risk of developing behavioral disorders associated with a restrained eating style and loss of control over eating under emotional circumstances. This risk increases with age. The combination of restrained eating styles with eating in emotional circumstances is likely to reflect a vicious circle resulting in persisting high levels of body dissatisfaction. The demonstrated association between female gender and weight related concerns and behaviors indeed indicates the common existence of a dissatisfaction with the body shape among female adolescents. This finding is in keeping with the results from previous studies (Attie & Brooks-Gunn, 1989; Huenemann et al., 1966; Paxton et al., 1991; Richards et al., 1990). As described earlier, this dissatisfaction with body shape does not appear to exist for medical reasons. It is likely to be the result of a perceived pressure exerted by society. The present study adds to our current knowledge a similar difference between current and desired BMIs among boys, albeit in an opposite way. In agreement with a previously demonstrated negative appreciation of thinness among boys (Paxton et al., 1991), the present study shows that adolescent boys actually desire to gain body weight. This may be due to the enhancement of self-esteem associated with an increase in body weight and muscularity among boys. The difference between current and desired BW among boys, however, does not appear to be associated with disturbed eating pat- terns. Therefore, it appears that the desire to increase body weight does not induce disturbed eating patterns. In conclusion, a large proportion of female adolescents shows disturbed eating patterns. For some of these girls these symptoms may be risk factors for the development of eating disorders or symptoms of a current eating disorder. For the other girls, the significant impairment that these symptoms may bring cannot be underestimated. Therefore, the introduction of curriculum-based programs to prevent eating disorders needs to be considered. The results of this study indicate that the risk of developing eating disorders increases with age. However, young girls already shows disturbances in eating patterns to such an extent that prevention programs should start in the first years of secondary school. References Attie, I., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (1989). Development of eating problems in adolescent girls: A longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology. 25. 70-79. Dornbusch. S. M., Carlsmith, J. M., Duncan, P. D., Gross, R. T., Martin, J. A., Ritter, P. L., Siegel-Gorelick, B. (1984). Sexual maturation, social class, and the desire to be thin among adolescent females. Developmental and Behavior Pediatrics, 5, 308314. Dwyer, J. T., Feldman, J, J., Seltzer, C. C., & Mayer, J. (1969). Body image in adolescents: Attitudes toward weight and perception of appearance. American Journal of Clinical nutrition. 20, 1045-1056. Fombonne, E. (1995). Eating disorders: Time trends and possible explanatory mechanisms. In M Rutter & D. Smith (Eds.), Psychosocial disorders in young people: Time trends and their causes Gamer, D. M., Garfinkel, P. E., Rockert, W., & Olmsted, M. P. (1987). A prospective study of eating ilisturbances in the ballet. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 48, 170-175. Gamer, D. M., Garfinkel, P. E., Schwartz, D., & Thompson, M. (1980). Cultural expectations of thinness in women. Psychological Reports, 47, 483-491. Gulliford, M. C., Rona, R. J., & Chinn, S. (1992). Trends in body mass index in young adults in England and Scotland from 1973 to 1988. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health, 46, 187-190. Hamilton, L. H., Brooks-Gunn, J., & Warren, M. P. (1985). Sociocultural influences on eating disorders in female professional dancers. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 4, 465-477. Huenemann, R. 1., Shapiro, L. R., Hampton, M. C., & Mitchell, B. W. (1966). A longitudinal study of gross body composition and body conformation and their association with food and activity in a teenage population. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 18, 325-338. Johnson-Sabine, E., Wood, K., Patton, G., Mann, A., & Wakeling, A. (1988). Abnormal eating attitudes in London schoolgirls-A prospective epidemiological study: Factors associated with abnormal response on screening questionnaires. Psychological Medicine, 18, 615-622. Kinoy, B. (1994). Eating Disorders: New Directions in Treatment and Recovery (p. 16). New York: Columbia University Press. Nylander, I. (1971). The feeling of being fat and dieting in a school population: Epidemiologic interview investigation. Acta Sociomedica Scandinavica, 3, 17-26. O'Neil, P. H., Currey, H. S., Hirsch, A. A., Malcolm, R. J., Sexauer, J. D., Riddle, F. E., & Taylor, C. I. (1979). Development and validation of the Eating Behavior Inventory. Journal of Behavioral Assessment, 1, 123-132. Patton, G. C., Johnson-Sabine, E., Wood, K., Mann, A. H., & Wakeling, A. (1990). Abnormal eating attitudes in London schoolgirls-A prospective epidemiological study: Outcome at twelve month follow-up. Psychological medicine, 20, 383-394. Paxton, S. J., Wertheim, E. H., Gibbons, K., Szmukler, G. I., Hillier, 1., & Petrovich, J. (1991). Body image satisfaction, dieting beliefs, and weight loss behaviors in adolescent girls and boys. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 20, 361-379. Pudel, V., Metzdorff, M., & Oeting, M. (1975). The personality of the obese in psychological texts taking into account the latent obesity. Journal of Psychosomatic Medicine and Psychoanalysis, 21, 345-361. Richards, M. H., Boxer, A. M., Petersen, A. C., & Albrecht, R. (1990). Relation of weights to body image in pubertal girls and boys from two communities. Developmental Psychology, 26, 313-321. Shah, M., Hannan, P. J., & Jeffery, R. W. (1991). Secular trend in body mass index in the adult population of three communities from the upper mid-western part of the USA. The Minnesota Heart Health Program. International Journal of Obesity, 15, 499-503. StatSoft. (1994). Statistica for the Macintosh [Computer software] Tulsa: StatSoft, Inc. Striegel-Moore, R. H., Silberstein, L. R., & Rodin, J. (1989). A prospective study of disordered eating among college students. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 8, 499-509. Sullivan, P. F. (1995). Mortality in anorexia nervosa. American Journal of Psychiatry, 152, 1073-1074. Szmukler, G. I., Eisloer, I., Gillies, C., & Hayward, M. E. (1985). The implications of anorexia nervosa in a ballet school. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 19, 177-181. Van Gaal, 1., Rillaerts, E., Creten, W., & De Leeuw, I. (1988). Relationship of body fat distribution pattern to atherogenic risk factors in NIDDM. Preliminary results. Diabetes Care, 11(2) 103-106. Van Strien, T. (1994). The increase in occurence of anorexia nervosa. Journal of ,Wental Health, 6, 647-652. Van Strien, T., Frijters, J., Roosen, R., Knuiman-Mijl, D., & Defares, P. (1986). Eating behavior, personality traits and body mass in women. Agricultural University, Wageningen, The Netherlands. Wollersheim, J. P. (1970). Effectiveness of group therapy based upon learning principles in the treatment of overweight women. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 76, 462-474. 4. Eating-style and weight-concerns in young females3 In this study, attitudes towards eating among adolescent school-girls were compared to those among fashion models and eating disorder patients. The results clearly demonstrate a preoccupation with body weight, even in school-girls. A majority of the girls indicated a wish to lose weight, even when the current Body Mass Index was between 20 and 25. Eating disorder patients more commonly reported premorbid overweight. Furthermore, fashion models tended to have an eating-style that was comparable to that of patients with eating disorders. The finding that the mean current body weight of all girls was lower than before suggests that many girls developed a restrained eating style. Vomiting, the use of anorectic drugs and laxatives, eating alone, and counting calories were reported by a substantial proportion of the adolescent girls. Moreover, a substantial proportion of the girls experienced a loss of control over eating. The combination of this eating-style and being overweight, can be considered as risk factor for the development of eating disorders. The culture of slimming in Western societies creates an important and increasing discrepancy between the real shape and the ideal shape (Garner, Garfinkel, Schwartz, & Thompson, 1980). Preoccupation with body weight and excessive concerns about body shape may induce eating patterns that are similar to those of patients with eating disorders. It remains to be demonstrated under which conditions such eating patterns develop into eating disorders. Dissatisfaction with body weight and shape are very common among preadolescent and adolescent girls. It is particularly concerning that girls who show relatively unhealthy baseline eating attitudes are more likely to fit in the category of the partial syndrome of eating disorders (PSED) at follow-up (Button & Whitehouse, 1981). While PSED is not inevitably associated with the development of anorexia or bulimia nervosa, it can, at the least, be regarded as similar to the first stage of Slade's (1987) developmental model of eating disorders. The danger exists that any girl who has reached this stage is prone to see low body weight and control as rewarding, and that she will then go on to develop a more robust disorder as a consequence. Thus, cultural factors may be a necessary condition in the development of eating disorders, or at least act as a facilitating factor, especially as Western cultures also suggest that success and the approval of others are solutions for problems (Wooley & Wooley, 1985). To be slim is a symbol of beauty, sexual 3 Vervaet, M., & van Heeringen, C. (2000). Eating Style and Weight Concerns in Young Females. Eating Disorders: The Journal of Treatment and Prevention, 8: 233-240. attractiveness, and success. Weight control is considered synonymous with discipline, personal strength and willpower (Noordenbos, 1990). Contemporary values such as perfectionism and control and pressure on females to enhance their appearance induce social stresses. The question remains: To what extent does such an internalisation process remain adaptive, or proceed beyond a certain limit and develop into psychopathology? Knowledge of psychological factors contributing to the development of pathological eating behaviors is indispensable for the identification of diagnostic criteria of the eating disorders. This study compared eating styles and weight concerns among schoolgirls, fashion models, and patients diagnosed as suffering from eating disorders. The groups were chosen in order to assess the association between eating behaviour, weight concerns, and eating disorders. Method Girls attending the fifth year of two large secondary schools participated in this study (n=333). Questionnaires were filled in collectively. The questionnaire was also sent to 20 fashion models of a national agency and to 40 patients with eating disorders (ED) admitted to the Department of Eating Disorders (University Hospital Gent) in 1993 (31.0% anorectics in treatment, n = 10; 62.5% bulimics of normal weight, n = 20; 6.3% overweight (BMI > 25) bulimics, n = 2). Items in the questionnaire covered age, weight, desired weight, educational level, presence of a boyfriend, use of anorectic drugs and laxatives, weight and eating problems (defined as self-criticism on weight and eating patterns, perceived excessive bodyweight, or lack of control over eating), vomiting, eating-patterns, thinking about eating (i.e. extent of preoccupation), eating alone (eating in secret), counting calories, preference for sweet or salty foods, loss of control, and the frequency (exceptional, sometimes, daily) and severity (<500Kcal, 1000-2000 Kcal, >2000 Kcal) of this loss of control. Moreover, the participants were asked to fill in the Dutch Eating Behaviour Questionnaire (DEBQ; Van Strien, 1986) which consists of the following subscales: emotional eating (eating elicited by emotional stimuli), restrained eating (the tendency to eat to lose weight), and external eating (eating elicited by external stimuli). Statistical analysis was performed using SPSS 9.0, and included a comparison of the characteristics between the schoolgirls, models and ED patients, by means of chi-square analysis for nominal variables and non-parametric tests for continuous variables. Results Questionnaires were returned by 333 schoolgirls (100 %), 11 fashion models (55 %), and 32 ED-patients (80%). Table 1 shows mean values for age, weight, and body mass index (BMI= weight/length x length) in the three groups. Table 1: A Comparison of the Mean Values and Standard Deviation for Age, Weight and BMI Among Schoolgirls, Fashion Models, and ED-patients. Schoolgirls Models ED n = 333 n = 11 n = 32 Mean (SD) Mean (SD) Mean (SD) _____________________________________________________________ Age (years)1 16.8 (0.8)ab 19 (0.9)ac Weight (kg)2 56 (6.4) 57 (3.5) BMI3 20 (1.9) 18 (1.O) 24 (5.2)bc 54 (12.6) 19 (4.1) 1 One-way ANOVA, F = 262.8; (df = 2); p < .0001 post-hoc comparison (Bonferroni) a p < .000; b p < .000; c p < .000 2 One-way ANOVA, F = .765; (df = 2); p = .466 3 One-way ANOVA, F = 1.5444; (df = 2); p = .215 There was a significant difference between the three groups in mean age, but not in mean weight and BMI. Mean reported former weight was higher than mean current weight in all three groups (Table 2). Table 2: A comparison of the Mean and Standard Deviation in Highest Weight, Desired Weight, and Desired BMI among Schoolgirls, Models and ED-patients. ------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------Schoolgirls Models ED n = 333 n = 11 n = 32 _____________________________________________________________ Highest weight1 58 (6.9)ab 61 (5.6)ac 66 (16.5)bc Desired weight2 54 (5.5) 55 (1.9) 52 (7.7) Desired BMI3 19 (1.5) 18 (0.7) 19 (2.2) 1 One-way ANOVA, F = 12.9; (df = 2); p < .000 post-hoc comparison (Bonferroni) a p = .445; b p = .000 c p = .528 2 One-way ANOVA, F = .1.2; (df = 2); p = .297 3 One-way ANOVA, F = 2.5; (df = 2); p = .083 On the contrary, girls in the three groups indicated a desired weight that was lower than the ideal BMI (63,5% of the schoolgirls want to lose weight, n = 212 versus 8,1% who want to gain weight, n = 27; 81,8% of the fashion models want to lose weight, n = 9 versus 9,1% who want to gain weight, n = 1; 62,5% of the eating disorder group want to lose weight, n = 20 versus 21,9% who want to gain weight, n = 7). Mean reported highest weight was significantly higher in ED patients than for the other groups. In the eating disorder group 40.6% (n = 13) reported a premorbid overweight, in contrast to 1,2% (n = 4) among the schoolgirls and 0% among the fashion models. Significant differences between the three groups were found for educational level and for reported weight and eating-problems (Table 3). Table 3: A comparison of the frequency (%) of the Highest School Level, Relationship, Weight Problems, and Eating Problems Among Schoolgirls, Models and ED Patients. Models ED X2 p-value Highest school level 74.4 27.3 62.5 20.120 0.000 Relationship 27.9 54.5 21.9 4.460 0.108 Weight problems 21.6 27.3 78.1 47.619 0.000 Eating problems 18.3 45.5 90.6 83.060 0.000 Schoolgirls Fashion models had a lower educational level when compared to the other two groups. Although the mean weight of attending schoolgirls was ideal, nearly one in four indicated problems with their body weight. One in two fashion models and one in five schoolgirls experienced eating problems. No significant association was found between having a boyfriend and belonging to one of the study groups. It is noteworthy that approximately 10% of the ED patients denied having eating problems. Table 4 shows the results regarding the use of more drastic methods to reduce body weight, i.e. anorectic drugs, vomiting, or laxatives. It was found that 16 % of the schoolgirls used laxatives, compared to 36 % of the models. Five percent of the schoolgirls used anorectic drugs, while 4 % reported vomiting. Table 4: A comparison of the Use of Anorectic Drugs, Vomiting and Laxatives Among Schoolgirls, Models, and ED patients. X2 p-value Schoolgirls Models ED % % % ___________________________________________________________ Drugs 4.8 0 15.6 6.500 0.100 Vomiting 3.9 0 37.5 52.34 0.000 Laxatives 15.9 36.4 46.9 20.83 0.000 Eating patterns also were compared between the three groups. As shown in Table 5, no significant differences in 'skipping breakfast' and 'experienced loss of control over eating' were found. Table 5: Comparison of Eating Pattern Among Schoolgirls, Models and, ED Patients Skipping breakfast Skipping lunch Skipping dinner Never eating between meals Eating alone Thinking about food Counting calories Loss of control once a day (frequency) >2000Kcal (amount) school-girls % 10.2 Models % 18.2 ED % 21.9 X2-test p-value 4.419 0.100 2.1 9.1 9.4 6.936 0.030 1.2 9.1 9.4 14.67 0.001 13.2 27.3 34.4 23 90.9 62.5 44.03 0.000 38.9 72.7 90.3 34.13 0.000 12 27.3 50 33.93 0.000 63 72.7 71.9 1.403 0.500 6.3 12.5 30.4 9.885 0.007 21.1 42.9 47.8 6.636 0.030 0.000 However, the severity and especially the frequency of loss of control differed significantly between the groups. ED patients showed more frequent loss of control over eating and reported the ingestion of larger amounts of food (in terms of Kcal). The significant character of the differences regarding the other items was mainly due to a lower frequency of disturbed eating patterns among the schoolgirls. However, even 12% of the schoolgirls counted calories, 40% thought about food during the day, and 23 % reported frequent eating while being alone. Mean scores on the items of the Dutch Eating Questionnaire are shown in Table 6. Table 6: Comparison of the Mean Scores and Their Standard Deviation on the Dutch Eating Questionnaire Among Schoolgirls, Models, and eating ED Patients. School-girls Models ED _____________________________________________________________ Restrained eating1 2.6 (0.9)ab 3.4 (1.0) ac 3.6 (0.9) bc Diffuse emotions2 2.8 (0.8) ab 3.4 (0.7) ac 3.3 (1.2) bc Clear emotions3 2.1 (0.7) ab 2.1 (0.7) ac 2.7 (1.2) bc Total emotions4 2.3 (0.7) ab 2.5 (0.4) ac 2.8 (1.1) bc External eating5 2.9 (0.5) ab 2.9 (0.5) ac 2.5 (0.7) bc 1 One-way ANOVA, F = 16,1; (df = 2); p = .000 post-hoc comparison (Bonferroni) a p = .028; b p =.000; c p =1.000 2 a 3 a 4 a F = .7,8; (df = 2); p = .000 p = .058; b p =.003 c p =1.000 F = 8,7; (df = 2); p = .000 p = 1.000; b p =.000; c p =.101 F = 16,1; (df = 2); p = .001 p = 1.000; b p =.001; c p =.595 5; a F = 16,1; (df = 2); p = .001 p = 1.000; b p =.001; c p =.265 Significant differences (p = 0.001) in the occurrence of ‘restrained eating’, ‘emotional eating’ and ‘external eating’ were found between schoolgirls and ED patients, but no significant differences were found between models and ED patients. The mean score for ‘restrained eating’ was significant lower for schoolgirls than that of the fashion models. Discussion and conclusion This study in a large group of young women suggests a substantial preoccupation with body weight, not only in patients diagnosed with eating disorders, but also in fashion models and in schoolgirls. The means of desired weight are lower in all three groups so we may hypothesize that a majority of girls in this study wanted to lose weight, even when their current BMI could be regarded as ideal. The eating pattern of fashion models appears to resemble that of eating disordered patients. Schoolgirls express severe concerns about their body weight, and one in four indicates problems related to their body weight, although this apparently is not the case in view of girls’ normal BMIs. The perceived problems induce counting calories and a restrained eating style, albeit to a lesser extent than in fashion models and eating disordered patients. The results also show that fashion models do not report to suffer from an eating disorder in spite of the fact that their eating patterns closely resemble those of eating disordered patients. However, even one in ten ED patients does not report to suffer from a disorder although she is currently being treated. In order to lose weight models commonly report the use of laxatives while patients additionally report vomiting. Finally, the results indicate that the highest ever mean body weight was reported by the patients suffering from an eating disorder. The finding of a premorbid overweight in eating disordered patients is in keeping with the results from previous studies. Two methodological aspects of this study should be addressed. First, the schools were not randomly selected. Therefore, participating subjects cannot be considered to be representative of the general population, and may actually have an increased risk of developing an eating disorder as the schools were only attended by girls who belonged to higher social classes, who were obliged to wear uniforms, and who were confronted with a very competitive spirit. Second, some questionnaires were not returned by the fashion models and the patients, so some results may be due to a selection bias. The low response rate in the model group may be due to distrust or lack of interest. The higher weight in models in comparison with the schoolgirls may be due to the significant difference in mean age between schoolgirls and fashion models. Finally, a number of schoolgirls and models may have actually been suffering from an eating disorder. Keeping these methodological limitations in mind, the findings from this study indicate that the mean body weight in schoolgirls can be regarded as ideal, as far as the body mass index is a correct index of body weight in this age group. In any case, the results demonstrate that there are no severe problems regarding body weight in this group of young females. However, restrained eating, vomiting, and the use of anorectic drugs or laxatives appear to be known as weight reducing strategies even among schoolgirls and nearly one in five schoolgirls reports subjective problems with their body weight and eating. Moreover, eating alone, thinking about food during the day, counting calories and experiencing of a loss of control over eating are commonly experienced. In view of a demonstrated association between binge eating and psychopathology (Telch and Agras, 1994), the finding that almost two-thirds of the schoolgirls report a loss of control over eating is important. Six percent of the schoolgirls even report loss of control to occur once a day, involving the ingestion of more than 2000 Kcal in more than 20%. Equally important are the demonstrated similarities between patients and fashion models, suggesting that the stronger the perceived pressure on body weight, the higher the risk of developing an eating disorder might be. The fact that the models do not consider themselves as suffering from a disorder may reflect the appreciation associated with their appearance, but might also be due to a denial of the negative consequences of their job. In view of the strong impact of pictures of models in fashion magazines (Field et al., 1999) our current findings indicate the necessity of providing information about potential negative consequences of a restrained eating style to become underweight in terms of developing psychopathology. As a conclusion, the results of this study suggest that being overweight during adolescence is a risk factor for developing identity related problems in a Western society with a slimming culture. During the months following this study, 10 of the participating schoolgirls attended the outpatient Department of Eating Disorders. Their symptoms fitted the criteria for an eating disorder according to DSM-IV. Thus, this study not only resulted in the detection of potential risk factors for eating disorders among schoolgirls, but also may have lowered the threshold for seeking treatment. References American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: APA. Button, E.J., Withouse, A.(1981). Subclinical anorexia nervosa. Psychological Medicine, 11, 509-516. Field, A.E., Cheung, L., Wolf, A.M., Herzog, D.B., Gortmaker, S.L., Colditz, G.A.(1999). Exposure to the Mass Media and Weight Concerns Among Girls. Pediatrics, Vol. 103, (3), 36-45. Garner, D.M., Garfinkel, P.E., & Olmsted, M.P.(1983). An overview of sociocultural factors in the development of anorexia nervosa. In: Darby, P.L., Garfinkel, P.E., Garner, D.M., & Coscina, D.V.(Eds.). Anorexia Nervosa. Recent Developments in Research (pp. 65-82). New York: Alan R. Liss. Garner, D.M., Garfinkel, P.E., Schwartz, D., Thompson, M. (1980). Cultural expectations of thinness in women. Psychological Reports, 47, 483-491. Noordenbos, G. (1990). Looking for the hidden identity behind a secret disorder. The recovery process of anorexia and bulimia nervosa. In: Petherson, G., Essed, Ph., & Richardson, D. (Eds.), Between Selfhelp and Professionalism ( pp.197-212). Amsterdam: The Moon Foundation The International Congress on Menthal Health Care for Women. Slade, P.D.(1987). Early recognition and prevention: Is it possible to screen people at risk of developing an eating disorder? In: D.Hardoff & E.Chigier (Eds). Eating Disorders in Adolescents and Young Adults. London: Freund. Telch, C.F., Agras, W.S.(1994). Obesity, Binge Eating and Psychopathology: Are They Related? International Journal of Eating Disorders, 15(1), 53-61. Van Strien, T., Frijters, J., Roosen, R., Knuiman-Mijl D., Defares, P.(1986). Eating behavior, personality traits and body mass in women. Wageningen, The Netherlands: Agricultural University . Wooley, S.C., & Wooley, O.W.(1985). Intensive outpatient and residential treatment for bulimia. In: Garner D.M. & Garfinkel P.E.(Eds.). Handbook of psychotherapy for anorexia nervosa and bulimia (p. 391). New York/London: Guilford Press. PART II The cognitive psychology of eating disorders: personality, cognitions and behaviour. Chapter 1: Theoretical background 1. Definition and epidemiology An eating disorder (ED) can be defined as a syndrome, in which disturbed eating behaviour is the central and meaningful characteristic (Vandereycken & Noordenbos, 2002). An ED thus can be differentiated from disturbed eating behaviour as a symptom of other psychiatric disorders, such as a mood disorder, schizophrenia, or obsessive compulsive disorder, or as a symptom of somatic disturbances, including infectious, neoplastic and endocrine disorders or malabsorption. The central characteristic of disturbed eating behaviour in syndromal eating disorders is associated with an extreme worry about or preoccupation with body size and body weight. Disturbed eating behaviour may include fasting, dieting and/or binge eating, and/or inappropriate compensating behaviour, including selfinduced vomiting or misuse of laxatives, diuretics, or enemas, and excessive exercise. Binge eating is defined as eating, in a limited period of time (e.g., within a two-hour period), an amount of food that is definitely larger than most people would eat during a similar period of time and under similar circumstances, combined with a sense of lack of control over this eating behaviour. More than half of the patients suffering from eating disorders have concomitant and secondary problems of anxiety and disturbed mood. Syndromal eating disorders can be ‘typical’, i.e. anorexia nervosa (AN) and bulimia nervosa (BN), or ‘atypical’, i.e. eating disorders not otherwise specified (EDNOS). The latter do not meet all criteria for a typical ED. For example, body weight may not have diminished to less than 85% of the expected body weight in spite of restricting behaviour, so that a diagnosis of AN can not be made, even if patients may not menstruate regularly. Bulimic women who binge and vomit weekly, but less than two times per week, are also diagnosed with EDNOS. Within the group of EDNOS patients, there is a subgroup of binge eaters who do not show compensating behaviour, and who are diagnosed with binge eating disorder (BED). BED will probably be described as a separate diagnostic category in the following edition of the DSM classification system of psychiatric disorders. Estimated prevalence rates among young females aged 18-30 years are 0.3% and up to 3 % for AN and BN, respectively, and ED are approximately 10 times more common in females than in males (APA, 2000). EDNOS appears to be much more prevalent, and it is estimated that up to 4% of the general population suffers from BED, but accurate epidemiological data are lacking. Assuming that even studies with the most complete case finding methods yield an underestimate of the true prevalence, the overall annual incidence of AN and BN can be estimated to be, at the least, 8 and 12 per 100 000 inhabitants, respectively. In 2000, primary care incidence rates were 4.7 and 6.6 per 100 000 population for anorexia and bulimia nervosa (Currin, Schmidt, Treasure, & Jick, 2005). The incidence rate of AN has a small global increase throughout the 20th century (Keel & Klump, 2003), particularly in females aged 10-24 years, with a stable European incidence since the 1970s (Hoek & van Hoeken, 2003; Currin et al., 2005). Overall there was an increase in the incidence of bulimia, at the least during the first five years after BN was added to the DSM classification system, but rates declined after a peak in 1996 (Currin et al., 2005). For EDNOS, including BED, information regarding incidence rates is insufficient (van Hoeken, Seidell, & Hoek, 2003). Although the occurrence of ED is not associated with socio-economic status, individuals with particular professions, including dancers, fashion models, actors or athletes, carry an increased risk of ED. Possibly the choice of profession is related to a predisposition to ED. It can be questioned whether anorexia nervosa is a culture-bound disorder, which is elicited by a pervasive pressure to diet and which is rare or absent in non-Western cultures. First, there are historical descriptions of cases of self-starvation without weight concern in cultures in which there was no emphasis on slimness (Bemporad, 1996). Secondly, cross-cultural comparison has suggested that body shape indeed is not the primary motivation in AN (Lee, 1996). Recent studies in South Africa (La Grange, Telch & Tibbs, 1998; Wassenaar le Grange Winship, et al, 2000), Nigeria (Oyewumi & Kazarian, 1992a, 1992b), Gana (Bennett, Sharpe, Freeman, & Carson, 2004), and Asia (Bhadrinath, 1990; Khandelwal & Saxena, 1990; Lee, Ho & Hsu, 1993), suggest that AN may take different forms in different cultures. The morbid self-starvation may have many motives, such as to atone for sins, to achieve better results, or for religious reasons. 2. The complexity of the ED diagnosis 2.1. Categorical classification As described above, eating disorders are classified in three categories, i.e. AN, BN and EDNOS (DSM-IV; APA, 1994). Subgroups within the AN and BN have been defined on the basis of variations in eating and compensatory behaviours. Thus, the AN category is subdivided in a restricting (RAN) and binge-purging (BPAN) type. Individuals with BN are classified as a purging type (BP) when they show binge eating and abuse laxatives, self-induce vomiting and/or use enemas, or as a non-purging type (BNP) when they combine binge eating with fasting or extreme exercise. Although, by definition, EDNOS means a failure to meet all criteria for a formal ED category, a diagnosis of EDNOS does not necessarily reflect a lack of clinical significance (Anderson, Bowers, & Watson, 2001). This categorical classification, predominantly based on descriptive behavioural and weight-related characteristics, has a number of limitations. First, ED categories are heterogeneously composed, and some but not all patients may shift from one category or type (e.g. restricting) to another (e.g. purging and binging) in the course of their illness (Löwe et al., 2001; Wentz, Gillberg, Gillberg & Rastam, 2001). Secondly, personality traits (Bulik, Sullivan, Weltzin, & Kay, 1995b; Bulik, Sullivan, Joyce, Carter, & McIntosh, 1998; Podar, Hannus, & Allik, 1999; Rosenvinge, Martinussen, & Ostensen, 2000) or personality disorders (Bulik, Sullivan, Joyce, & Carter, 1995a; Matsunaga et al., 2000; Muller et al., 2000) may play a particular role in the emergence (Kay et al., 2000a) and maintenance of eating disorder symptomatology (van Hanswijck de Jonge P, van Furth, EF, Lacey JH, & Waller G, 2003). ICD-10 (WHO, 1992) defines a personality disorder (Axis II) as a severe, long-term disorder in an individual’s characterological constitution and behavioural tendencies, which usually extends to various aspects of the personality and is nearly always associated with or results in serious personal and social disturbance. As a consequence, the boundaries between Axis I and Axis II disorders are blurred in ED patients (Webber, 1994). Data suggest a greater incidence of Cluster B personality disorders, e.g. in particular borderline personality disorder (Carroll et al., 1996; Wonderlich & Mitchell, 1997; Matsunaga et al., 2000) in bulimic patients (compared to other ED) associated with behaviours such as self-harm, shoplifting and other impulsive behaviours (Welch & Fairburn, 1996; Wiederman & Pryor, 1996). In that case, some authors have suggested that a diagnosis of ‘multi-impulsive bulimia’ may be appropriate (Lacy, 1993; Lacy & Read, 1993). Cluster C personality disorders, e.g. the avoidant personality disorders (Gillberg et al., 1995; Roosenvinge e.a., 2000) were found more in anorectic individuals. There is some evidence that those with BPAN show higher rates of personality disorders than either RAN or BN patients (Piran et al., 1988; Herzog et al., 1992). However, recent studies (Grilo, Sanislow, Shea, Skodo, Stout et al., 2003a; Grilo, Sanislow, Skodel, Gunderson, Stout et al., 2003b) showed that patients with PD do not have significantly more ED than patients with major depressive disorder but without PD. In addition, patients with specific forms of PD do not differ in their frequencies of ED (Ilkjaer, Kortegaard, Hoerder, Joergensen, Kyvik, & Gillberg, 2004). Nevertheless, rates of co-occurrence of ED and PD are sufficiently high, especially for cluster C, to warrant careful consideration during routine assessment and treatment planning stage, since diagnosable personality disorders occur, and they appear to be associated with greater chronicity and poorer functioning (Skodol et al., 1993; Inceoglu et al., 2000; Johnson et al., 1990). Moreover, it could be taken to mean that the PD, which is considered a more stable “trait” than ED, which is usually regarded as a “state”, is heritable and precedes the onset of ED (Ilkjaer et al., 2004). 2.2. Dimensional classification and comorbidity While the presence or absence of a personality disorder diagnosis reflects a categorical approach, a dimensional model focuses on the extent to which personality traits are present, assessing the number of personality disorder criteria that are present to an accentuated and/or pathological degree, regardless of whether the patients has a full diagnosis. Nevertheless, it should be noted, that also the distinction between EDs based on personality traits is by no means perfect, as studies have shown particular traits to be present in AN and BN (Carroll et al., 1996; Inceoglu et al., 2000). Three personality clusters have been identified in ED patients: a high functioning, self-critical, perfectionist group, which was mainly associated with BN; a constricted, over-controlled group restricting pleasure, needs, emotions, relationships and self-knowledge, which was associated with RAN; and an impulsive, under-controlled and emotionally dysregulated group also associated with BN and with BED (Fahy & Eisler, 1993; Goldner et al., 1999). A useful and interesting dimensional approach differentiates the compulsive (or restrictive type, RAN) and the impulsive type (or the bulimic, binging and purging patients, BPAN & BP) of ED patients (Claes, Vandereycken & Vertommen, 2002; Polivy & Herman, 2002; Steiger & Seguin, 1999). With regard to impulsivity, many components have been described (Whiteside and Lynam, 2001), but two have been particularly studied in ED, i.e. urgency and lack of planning (Fischer, Smith, Anderson, 2003). Individuals high in urgency are likely to act rashly in order to cope with distress. Several studies found that the bulimic symptoms ‘binge eating’ and ‘purging’ are related to urgency, since these symptoms can be seen as reflecting mechanisms to cope with negative affect, which is relatively more common in BN patients (Pryor & Wiederman, 1996; Stice, Killen, Hayward, & Taylor, 1998; Telch & Stice, 1998; Tylka & Subich, 1999). Emotional states such as anxiety and depression have indeed been shown to predict binge episodes (Arnow, Kenardy, & Agras, 1995). In contrast, several researchers have found lack of planning to be within the normal range in bulimics (Bushnell, Wells, & Oakley Brown, 1996; Fahy & Eisler, 1993; Newton, Freeman, & Munro, 1993; Fischer et al., 2003). Bulimic behaviour is thus associated with impulsivity, but obsessional features such as rigidity, neatness, conscientiousness and preoccupation with rules and ethics characterize the restrictive anorexic type. This pattern of responding was shown even at a basic perceptual level, with a rigid pattern of responding in AN and an unstable or fluctuating style in BN patients (Tchanturia, Serpell, Troop, & Treasure, 2002). If the latter characteristics are present to such an extent that they result in marked impairment of social or occupational functioning over a considerable period of time, a diagnosis of obsessive-compulsive personality disorder (OCPD) may be warranted (Serpell et al., 2002). A wealth of clinical literature describes the cluster of rigidity, perfectionism and inflexible thinking, that is characteristic of OCPD, in AN (Casper et al., 1992; Karwautz, Troop, Rabe-Hesketh, Collier, & Treasure, 2003; Rosenvinge, Martinussen, & Ostensen, 2000; Strober, 1980; Vitousek & Hollon, 1990). Estimates of the comorbidity of OCPD and ED vary from 3% (Piran et al., 1988) to 60% (Wonderlich et al., 1990). Childhood OCPD personality traits also showed a high predictive value for the development of ED (Brecelj et al., in press). Preliminary data from family studies indicate an increased risk of OCPD in relatives of AN probands compared to controls (Lilenfeld et al., 1998). These findings suggest the existence of a phenotype with core features of rigid perfectionism and propensity for extreme behavioural constraint. Sohlberg and Strober (1994) have suggested that obsessional symptoms are related to the state of starvation, while obsessional traits are stable personality features, which are maintained after weight gain. Although phobic thoughts of food and weight repeatedly enter the mind of AN patients, they are not regarded as senseless or unwanted (Mazure, Halmi, Sunday, Romano, and Einhorn, 1994), which is in contrast to the typical obsessions and compulsions unrelated to eating that also may occur in AN patients (Bastiani et al., 1996). Obsessions involving symmetry, such as ordering and arranging, were found to be the most common obsessions in AN patients (Bastiani et al., 1996; Matsunaga et al., 1999). The results of the most recent study of this type (Halmi, Sunday, Klump, Strober, Leckman et al., 2003) with larger patient groups at all stages of illness, were remarkably similar to those of the Bastiani study, with no significant difference between the RAN and BPAN women. The neurobiological basis of the relationship between obsessive-compulsive disorder OCD and ED remains unclear. Data from twin studies suggest that genetic vulnerability factors contribute to both AN (Bulik, Sullivan, Wade, & Kendler, 2000; Klump, Miller, Keel, McGue, & Jacono, 2001; Kortegaard, Hoerder, Joergensen, Gillberg, & Kybik, 2001; Wade et al., 2000) and OCD (Leckman, Zhang, Asobrook, & Paus, in press). The few family aggregation studies suggest that these disorders may be independently transmitted in families (Lilenfeld et al., 1998). Neuroimaging studies suggest that both disorders are associated with alterations in the frontal cortex and in the subcortical and limbic regions (Kaye et al., 2001; Saxena, Brody, Schwartz, & Baxter, 1998). This frontal-subcortical circuitry contains complex direct and indirect pathways that are modulated by serotonin and dopamine. Pharmacologic (DeVeaugh-Geiss, 1991; Halmi, 1999) and physiological (Lilenfeld et al., 1998) studies also indicate that alterations in functional serotonin activity are present in both OCD and AN subjects. The common nature of these phenotypical characteristics of AN and OCD patients may indicate that these disorders share common brain behavioural pathways. However, the incomplete nature of the overlap between these disorders suggests that they have different loci of pathology within these pathways (Halmi et al., 2003). Describing the restricting versus the bulimic group provides a good example of the overlap between Axis-I ED diagnostic categories and between Axis-I ED diagnoses and Axis-II PD categories. A dimensional approach may thus add information about traits and clinical phenomena to a categorical diagnosis, offering the possibility of a more accurate and effective patient assessment and treatment planning (Van Hanswijck, De Jonge et al., 2003). 3. Aetiology as a dynamic process: from trait- (vulnerability) to state- (eating disorder category) dependent characteristics 3.1. Introduction Although the aetiology of AN and BN is incompletely understood, a comprehensive aetiological model is likely to include a combination of genetic and familial (Bulik et al., 2000; Treasure & Holland, 1995), personality and psychological (Vitousek & Manke, 1994), environmental and neurobiological elements (Nasser, Katzman, & Gordon, 2001). A role of these factors in the development of ED can be described in terms of their predisposing, precipitating (or eliciting: such as death of a loved one, transition into puberty, dieting) and sustaining effect. Genetic factors most probably contribute to the predisposition to ED. Twin and family studies indeed suggest a substantial heritability for AN (Collier & Treasure, 2004; Lilenfeld LR, Kaye WH, Greeno CG, Merikangas KR, Plotnicov K, Pollice C, et al., 1998). Also in BN, family and twin studies point at a role of genetic factors in the predisposition (Collier & Treasure, 2004; Hsu, 1990; Walters, Neale, Eaves, Lindon, & Heath, 1992), in addition to - biological factors such as reduced levels of beta-endorphin (Brewerton, Lydiard, Laraia, Shook, & Ballenger, 1992), norepinephrine (Goldbloom, Garfinkel, & Shaw, 1991), and/or serotonin (Jimmerson, Lesen, Kate, & Brewerton, 1992); - family factors such as poor communication (Hsu, 1990) and high expectations (Pike & Rodin, 1991); - individual factors such as perfectionism, maturity fears (Garner, Olmsted, Polivy, Garfinkel, 1984) and low self-esteem (Katzman & Wolchik, 1984); - sociocultural factors such as preoccupation with thinness and dieting at a societal level (Brownell, 1991; Garner & Garfinkel, 1980). More recently, a first study reported heritability of the core BED syndrome (Reichborn-Kjennerud, Budlike, Tambs, & Harris, 2004), although the estimates of heritability are somewhat lower than those reported for AN and BN (Budlike et al., 2000). The focus of the studies as described in this thesis was on psychological factors as the link between neurobiological characteristics and the overt behaviour of eating-disordered patients. Core psychological traits and personality characteristics of ED patients were described as, among others, temperamental and character dimensions which develop as the result of an interaction between genetic and learning processes. In addition to these dimensions, three psychological characteristics were studied as they were identified as core characteristics of ED patients in two recent transdiagnostic models of eating disorders, i.e. negative self-esteem (inextricably related to body dissatisfaction) perfectionism/rigidity and interpersonal difficulties (Fairburn, Cooper & Shafran, 2003; Serpell & Troop, 2003). These three characteristics possibly reflect a premorbid vulnerability, and appear to be necessary (though possibly not sufficient) conditions for the development of ED. 3.2. Low self-esteem and body dissatisfaction Since the start of the systematic study of ED, the failure to develop a separate and integrated sense of self, as the capacity for self-regulation, was found to be an important psychological factor in the development of eating disorders (Bruch, 1973). Since then it has become clear that deficits in the self also may become manifest as • disturbances in the ability to identify and modulate bodily cues and emotional experiences (de Groot & Rodin, 1998; Bydlowski, Corcos, Jeammet, et al., 2005), • low self-esteem and pervasive feelings of ineffectiveness (Rosen, 1990), and • increased sensitivity to external evaluation, or fears of negative evaluation (Gilbert & Meyer, 2005). ED patients thus lack positive self-schemata by means of which they organize information about the self. In cognitive psychology, self-esteem refers to a cognitive evaluation of one’s competencies. Longitudinal studies have suggested a relationship between low self-esteem and the subsequent development of ED symptoms (e.g. Button et al., 1996; Wood et al., 1994). Retrospective reporting suggests that negative self-evaluation during childhood may be more common among women with AN or BN than among women in a non-psychiatric comparison group or among women with other psychiatric disorders (Fairburn et al., 1997, 1999). However, follow-up studies suggest that self-esteem improves with recovery of BN, but that it remains low in patients whose ED symptoms persist (e.g. Troop et al., 2000). Clinical observations appear to confirm theoretical considerations of an association between eating disorders and disturbances in the mother-child relationship. These disturbances may include maternal empathic failure and unresponsiveness to child-initiated cues that are thought to interfere with the emerging sense of self. More specifically, studies have identified lower perceived maternal care (Calam, Waller, Slade, & Newton, 1990; Haudek, Rorty, & Henker, 1999), greater maternal criticism (Dare, LeGrange Eisler, & Rutherford, 1994), hostile maternal over-involvement (Humphrey, 1989; Rorty, Yager, Rossotto, & Buckwalter, 2000), and a diminished sense of psychological separateness (Ogden & Steward, 2000) as characteristics of the relationship between AN patients and their mothers. In addition, dieting and body dissatisfaction have been found in girls whose mothers perceive their own autonomy as low (Ogden & Steward, 2000). Self-esteem may thus be associated with a negative self-evaluation in which environmental effects may be influential. It is not clear, however, to what extent this association can be explained by genetic factors. Indeed, a significant proportion of the variance in self-esteem in the population is due to genetic factors, which, in turn, may become manifest through temperamental characteristics (Kendler, Gardner, & Prescott, 1998). Since personality has an effect on how the environment is experienced, interpreted, and reacted to, children and adolescents with particular temperaments, such as negative emotionality and low sociability, possibly interpret and experience life events in a more negative way (see e.g. the social-information processing model of Crick & Dodge, 1994). There is some evidence that particularly the strength of the association between self-esteem and self-evaluation of weight and shape differentiates between individuals with ED and non-ED young women (and those with other psychiatric disorders) (Serpell et al., submitted; Vitousek and Hollon, 1990). There is thus no doubt that the association between self-esteem and self-evaluation of the weight and shape of the body is typical for eating disorders. It is not clear, however, when this association between self-esteem and self-evaluation of weight and shape emerges in relation to the onset of ED. Girls with a vulnerable sense of self may be at greater risk of linking their self-concept to the weight and shape of their body if they live in familial environments that emphasize the importance of appearance, and associate thinness with femininity, beauty and competence (Levine & Smolak, 1992). Self-concepts and self-esteem emerge in a relational context in which the sense of self may be defined by relationships with others and through evaluation by others, and this contextual effect appears to be more influential among girls than among boys (Gilligan, 1993). This focus on external evaluation may place girls at greater risk of adopting weight and shape as a gauge of their self-worth, especially in such families. Negative self-evaluation may thus develop during the onset or after the onset of ED, and become a symptomatic manifestation of low self-esteem. On the other hand, body weight- and shape-related self-evaluation was the concept used to refer to the process whereby an individual determines her self-worth based on an evaluation of her body weight and shape (McFarlane, McCabe, Jarry, Olmsted, & Polivy, 2001). The social comparison theory (Festinger, 1954) suggested that individuals’ drive for self-evaluation can be met by comparison with similar others. An upward social comparison can have negative effects on mood and self-esteem (Major, Testa, & Bylsma, 1991). Exposure to images of thinness, which are idealized by mass media, can thus have a consistent negative effect on body satisfaction, particularly among certain individuals (Groesz, Levine, & Murnen, 2002), possibly more important among girls. A recent 16-month follow-up study among adolescent boys and girls (McCabe & Ricciardelli, 2005) indeed showed an increase of perceived messages to lose weight over time among the girls, whereas the boys perceived the messages as related to increasing muscles. Among the girls, the strongest influences were mothers and best female friends, while among the boys fathers play the most important role. Trait-dependent psychological features (i.e. low self-esteem and perfectionism) and dysfunctional information processes, which will be described later, may also lead to a disturbed body-image. This disturbance can be viewed as a predisposing factor and as a maintaining factor of the disorder (Cooley & Toray, 2001; Rosen, 1990; Tuschen-Caffier, Vögele, Bracht, & Hilbert, 2003; Stice, 2002; Wilson, Fairburn, & Agras, 1997). Patients with a body-image disturbance are preoccupied with the appearance of their body and show compulsive behaviours such as mirror checking and body measuring. Cognitive models of eating disorders propose that this selective attention to appearance cues is due to underlying knowledge structures (schemas) that filter information and direct attention. These schemata guide the attention to stimuli, the memory for stimuli, and the interpretation of stimuli in ways that serve to maintain the disorder (Ainsworth, Waller, & Kennedy, 2002; Hargreaves and Tiggemann, 2002; Viken, Treat, Nosofsky, McFall, & Palmeri, 2002). It should be noted that many studies have used self-reports to assess attentional processing, which may well reduce the reliability of findings. However, more direct measures of attentional processing show that eating-disordered subjects allocate their attention more towards a self-identified ugly body part, contrary to the focus of attention when looking at another body, which is a beautiful body part. Normal controls do exactly the opposite (Jansen, Nederkoorn, & Mulkens, 2005). These authors found an increase of attention in the eating-disordered patients when they looked at their own body with activating negative appearance-related schemata as a consequence, instead of the usually found avoidance behaviour. 3.3. Perfectionism as core trait Perfectionists are anxious subjects with a strong tendency to overestimate the probabilities of negative events and to over-predict threatening events, dangers, and damages (MacLeod, 1999; Rachman, 1998). Anxious subjects spend much time worrying intensively about their fears (Borkovec, Ray, & Stöber, 1998). Among perfectionists, the over-prediction of threatening events is associated with an intensive fear of failure after important performances. In turn, this type of over-feared failure leads to further damage in terms of decreased self-esteem and impaired social, familial and interpersonal wellbeing. Perfectionists have an increased probability of experiencing failure in a stress situation, because even minor shortfalls are tantamount to significant failures. Stress situations and major life events negatively affect eating habits in human and animal models (Connan & Treasure, 1998). In turn, perfectionism may facilitate the development of ED by increasing the impact of distressing environmental events (Hewitt & Flett, 1993a; Hewitt et al., 1995). Therefore, perfectionists have a tendency to worry about and to feel dissatisfied with perceived mistakes (e.g. regarding eating) and imperfections (e.g. regarding body shape and weight) (Ruggiero et al., 2003). Perfectionism is a salient trait in women with AN and BN during acute illness (Cockell, Hewitt, Seal, Sherry, Goldner, Flett, & Remick, 2002; Fairburn, Cooper, Doll, & Welch, 1999; Goldner et al., 2002; Halmi, Sunday, Strober, Kaplan, Woodside et al., 2000; Lilenfeld, Stein, Bulik, Strober, Plotnicov et al., 2000; Stice & Shaw, 2002) and after recovery (Bastiani, Rao, Weltzin, Kaye, 1995; Bulik, Sullivan, Fear, & Pickering, 2000; Halmi et al., 2000; Pia & Toro, 1999; Srinivasagam, Kaye, Plotnicov, Greeno, Weltzin, Rao, 1995; Sutandar-Pinnock, Woodside, Carter, Olmsted, & Kaplan, 2003). Clinical (or ‘neurotic’) perfectionism, in which there is an inability to derive pleasure from one’s successes because the performance is never good enough, (e.g. Kiemle et al., 1987; Ruggiero, Levi, Ciuna, Sassaroli, 2003; Slade et al., 1990, 1991) is in particular accompanied by a tendency to evaluate one’s own behaviour in an overly critical way (Frost, Marten, Lahart, & Rosenblate, 1990). “Clinical perfectionism” was defined as “the overdependence of self-evaluation on the determined pursuit of personally demanding, selfimposed, standards in at least one highly salient domain, despite adverse consequences” (p. 778, Shafran, Cooper, & Fairburn, 2002). In addition, the term perfectionism refers to - selective attention to and over-generalization of failure - stringent self-evaluations - a tendency to engage in ‘all or none’ thinking, whereby total success or total failure exist as outcomes (Hewitt & Flett, 1991) - cognitive rumination over mistakes and imperfections - frequent automatic thoughts about attaining perfection (Hewett, Flett, Besser, Sherry, & McGee, 2002). It should be noted, however, that studies are characterized by poor operational definitions and assessments of perfectionism. As in the current studies, perfectionism is commonly measured using a subscale of the Eating Disorder Inventory (EDI; Garner, Olmstead & Polivy, 1983), a 64-item, selfreport questionnaire that measures cognitive and behavioural characteristics of ED. The Perfectionism subscale (EDI-P) consists of six items that emphasize personal standard setting and parental expectations. Although performance on this subscale is expressed by means of one score, this subscale measures both self-directed (3 items) and socially based (3 items) dimensions of perfectionism (Frost, Marten, Lahart, Rosenblate, 1990; Hewitt & Flett, 1991; Wyatt & Gilbert; 1998; Sherry, Hewitt, Besser, McGee, & Flett, 2004). Recent research (Sherry et al., 2004) emphasized that both the intrapersonal (self-oriented) and interpersonal (other-oriented and socially prescribed) aspects of perfectionism are independently implicated in the emergence and continuance of eating disorder symptoms. This conclusion is consistent with theoretical considerations (Bruch, 1981) and findings from some (Hewitt, Flett & Edinger, 1995) but not all studies. Self-oriented perfectionism with a tendency to interpret mistakes as failures is significantly associated with the presence of AN and BN (Bulik, Tozzi, Anderson, Mazzeo, Aggen, & Sullivan, 2003; Cooper, Cooper, & Fairburn, 1985). Socially prescribed perfectionism is positively associated with higher levels of hopelessness and suicidal ideation and negatively associated with positive future thinking (Hewitt, Flett & Turnbull-Donovan, 1992; Hewitt, Newton, Flett, & Callander, 1997, Hunter & O’Connor, 2003). This is not surprising, as socially prescribed perfectionism has been characterized as being driven by the fear of failure or by the avoidance of punishment. ED patients share high scores on the subscale for doubts about actions (describing the reduced ability to accomplish tasks and obsessional aspects of perfectionism, as assessed by means of the ‘Frost Multidimensional Perfectionism Scale’; Purdon, Antony & Swinson, 1999) with patients suffering from anxiety disorders, possibly due to shared genetic factors. Although truly prospective studies have not been conducted, retrospective clinical reports have frequently described premorbid perfectionism as a risk factor for AN (Bruch, 1978; Halmi et al., 1977; Fairburn, Cooper, Doll, Welch, 1999; Rastam, 1992; Slade, 1982; Srinivasagam et al., 1995). These studies have also provided evidence for a trait-dependent nature of this characteristic, which is thus not simply due to the effects of the illness (e.g. starvation) but which may reflect an underlying, whether or not genetically determined, vulnerability factor. Few studies have assessed dimensions of temperament or cognitive/psychological domains, which are believed to be important in the development of ED, among parents of ED patients. However, elevated rates of perfectionism have been found in the mothers (and to a lesser extent fathers) of individuals with AN (Woodside, Bulik, Halmi, Fichter, Kaplan et al., 2002), and in the first-degree relatives of BN patients (Lilenfeld et al., 2000). Possibly, the trait perfectionism will be transmitted through families and represent a vulnerability factor for the development of AN (Woodside et al., 2002). Indeed, twin studies have provided preliminary evidence for the heritability of AN (Holland, Hall, Murray, Russell, & Crisp, 1984; Holland, Sicotte, Treasure, 1988; Klump, Miller, Keel, McGue, & Iacono, 2000). These findings support Strober’s (1991) genotypic foundation hypothesis of AN, with harm avoidance, obsessiveness and self-doubting perfectionism as predisposing traits. Underweight and weight-restored AN patients did not differ from normal controls regarding scores on ‘Parental Expectations’ or ‘Other-Oriented Perfectionism’, while they showed only a trend in differing significantly from normal controls on the ‘Doubt about Action’ and ‘Socially Prescribed Perfectionism’ dimensions (Bastiani et al., 1995). These results suggest that perfectionism in AN is particularly of a selfimposed nature. Perfectionism could thus well be the behavioural expression of a biologically determined vulnerability. Kaye and colleagues (1991) have provided evidence for such vulnerability by describing an increased neuronal serotonin activity, which persists after long-term recovery of AN patients. The related trait rigidity (related to obsessiveness) is also of interest (Goldner et al., 1999), functioning in various neuropsychological domains, including perception, among AN patients (Tchanturia et al., 2002). This perceptual rigidity appears to be also a stable trait, which persists after recovery from AN (Tchanturia et al., in press). Rigidity may be specifically related to AN, since women with BN typically show a fluctuation rather than rigidity in their perceptual processes (Tchanturia et al., 2002). Reduced serotonergic activity has been associated with impulsive and aggressive behaviour (Coccaro, Kavoussi, & Lesser, 1992; Spoont, 1992), that is opposite in character to the behavioural pattern of AN patients. Indeed, in these patients control, or the absolute certainty of avoiding all the threatening events predicted by perfectionists (Sassaroli & Ruggiero, 2002), is an important behavioural feature. This sense of control often is obtained by monitoring continuously a particular parameter, such as body perception in panic, or intrusive thoughts in obsessionality. For subjects suffering from ED, the parameters may be related to eating and/or body weight and shape (Fairburn, Shafran, & Cooper, 1998). Therefore, ED’s could be described as disorders of the sense of self-esteem and self-worth, which are without remedy pervasively negative, if not lacking in such subjects. Achieving self-control is the ultimate goal of ED individuals, who fear that they are not sufficiently worthy (Bruch, 1973; Button, 1985; Katzman & Lee, 1997). This general self-schema, a core cognitive characteristic of ED, is called “long-standing negative self evaluation” (Vitousek & Hollon, 1990). Moreover, perfectionist subjects perceive their parents’ love as being connected to parental expectations and critical evaluations (Patch, 1984). Subsequently, the familial form of failure intensively feared by the perfectionist subject is the loss of parental love (Ruggiero et al., 2003). 3.4. Perception of life events and the coping with life-events by individuals, characterized by low self-esteem and clinical perfectionism Serpell and Troop (2003) have recently discussed the presence of stressors and suboptimal coping prior to the onset of eating disorders, while distinguishing between stressors that were temporally distant from onset (i.e. occurring in childhood) and those occurring immediately prior to onset. In general, ED patients appear to report trauma, including sexual abuse and parental antipathy, indifference and over-control, more frequently than women without eating disorders. Within the group of patients with ED, binging and purging patients (BPAN and BP) show the highest levels of childhood adversity (Schmidt et al., 1997a). Based on retrospective reporting and thus subject to recall biases, Schmidt’s (1993a, 1999) and Strober’s (1984) studies suggest that sufferers of the RAN subtype report relatively little childhood adversity, but they report severe events and difficulties prior to onset. Women with BPAN report high levels of childhood adversity, but low rates of events and difficulties prior to onset. Relationship problems with a meaning of loss most commonly provoke the onset of eating disorders (Schmidt et al., 1997b). Events related to pudicity, i.e. crises of a sexual nature that were perceived as shameful, embarrassing or disgusting, were in particular significantly more common in patients developing AN than in those developing BN and in non-psychiatric controls (24%, 3% and 8%, respectively). The coping response is important in determining the impact of life events and difficulties, i.e. whether the life event or difficulty will result in high levels of stress. Research shows, in general, that women with AN and BN display high levels of coping through avoidance when compared to non-ED woman. Moreover, women with BN, but not AN, seek less support and are less confident (Bloks, Spinhoven, Calewaert, Willems-Koning, & Turksma, 2001; Neckowitz & Morrison, 1990; Soukup et al., 1991; Troop et al., 1994, 1998, Yager et al., 1995). Using a retrospective semi-structured interview, Troop and Treasure (1997a) found that cognitive avoidance was associated with the onset of AN, while cognitive rumination was associated with the onset of BN. In addition, the onset of an ED was related to higher levels of helplessness in response to the provoking event/difficulty. These authors also found higher levels of helplessness in girls who subsequently developed an ED, suggesting that helplessness is related to the vulnerability for developing an ED. Overall, women with RAN reported lower levels of helplessness than the other ED groups BN and BPAN. However, this may be due in part to the lower levels of severe adversity in childhood in RAN women, since when only those women with two or more adverse childhood experiences were included (e.g. sexual abuse, parental antipathy, parental indifference, etc.) the levels of helplessness were rather similar for the ED subgroups, all of which were higher than the levels found in non-ED women. Interestingly, Tiller and colleagues (1997) found differences in structural and functional aspects of social support between ED subtypes. For example, women with AN reported fewer support figures than non-ED women, but they were equally satisfied with the support they received. BN women, however, reported a network of potential support figures of a similar size as that of non-ED women, but they were considerably more dissatisfied. However, AN and BN women reported fewer friends and more loneliness during their childhood than non-ED women (Fairburn et al., 1997, 1999; Karwautz et al., 2001; Troop & Bifulco, 2002). Two-thirds of AN patients reported social isolation, poor social interpersonal relationships and difficulties with relatives and therapists (Flament, Godart, Fermanian & Jeammet, 2001). Bulik and colleagues (2000) reported significantly lower maternal and paternal care scores in AN patients who were chronically ill. These associations can be interpreted in different ways. While poor social support may indeed reflect the use of particular coping strategies that may act as a vulnerability factor for ED, the long-standing presence of an ED may affect the family system in an adverse way and/or may bias an individual’s perception of her parents. However, it appears that the absence of adequate maternal and paternal care may contribute directly or indirectly to the chronicity of an ED. Based on these findings, it can be hypothesized that vulnerable individuals due to a low self-esteem, associated with body dissatisfaction and perfectionism, cope with perceived threatening life-events by avoidance or escape behaviour. The perception and interpretation of those events is based on neuropsychological and cognitive processes, which elicited those behaviours. 4. Information processing in eating disordered patients 4.1. Neuropsychological processes When compared to healthy controls, AN patients show deficits in various neuropsychological domains including verbal and visual memory, visuospatial ability, attentional skills (with a bias toward an analytic and controlled information-processing mode) and executive functioning (Bowers, 1994; FrantzFox, 1981; Green, Elliman, Wakeling, & Rogers, 1996; Kaye, Bastiani, & Moss, 1995; Lauer, Gorzewski, Gerlinghoff, Backmund, & Zihl, 1999; Mathias & Kent, 1998; Pendleton-Jones, Duncan, Brouwers, & Mirsky, 1991; Strauss & Ryan, 1988; Szmukler et al., 1992; Thompson, 1993). Some (Kingston et al., 1996; Lauer et al., 1999; Moser, Benjamin, Bayless et al., 2003; Szmukler et al., 1992), but not all (Green et al., 1996) studies indicate that these neuropsychological deficits recover with treatment. Among AN patients the attentional and perceptual deficits may be stimulus-specific with specific difficulties in inhibiting irrelevant information, while perceiving stimuli related to body images, food and weight (Cooper and Todd, 1997; Long, Hinton, & Gillespie, 1994; Perpina, Himsley, Treasure, & De Silva, 1993; Rieger, Schotte, Touyz et al., 1998; Smeets, Smit, Panhuysen, & Ingelby, 1998; Smeets, Ingelby, Hoek, & Panhuysen, 1999). The pathological preoccupation with body shape leads to an intensive focus on the body and the search for perfection, which is typical of rigid personalities (Fassino, Piero, Abbate Daga et al., 2002). Performance appears to be affected more when the situation or task is relevant to the individual’s specific concern (Williams et al., 1997). However, methodological flaws associated with the use of particular tasks (such as a modified Stroop colour-naming task) and validity problems of the used questionnaires, hamper the interpretation of the results of studies in ED patients (Mogg & Bradley, 1998). Using an electrophysiological technique Dodin and Nandrino (2003) showed, that these deficits extend to generic stimuli, irrespective of the task-complexity or to the task-specificity (e.g. requiring attentional, perceptual or motor resources). Logically, it was hypothesized that food deprivation and/or low BMI or the comorbid presence of depression were responsible, but this explanation was not sufficient (Hamsher et al., 1981; Lauer et al., 1999; Kingston et al., 1996; Mathias & Kent, 1998; Moser et al., 2003; Pendleton-Jones et al., 1991; Szmukler et al., 1992). Parallel with the findings of preoccupying cognitions in emotional disorders (Williams, Watts, MacLeod, & Mathews, 1997), the bias towards a controlled processing mode could be related to anxiety with a systematically allocating high level of attentional resources, impeding appropriate selective processing of relevant information (Green, Elliman, & Rogers, 1997; Green & Rogers, 1995, 1998; Green, Rogers, Elliman, & Gatenby; 1994; Jones & Rogers, 2003; Rogers & Green, 1993). Thus, Dodin and Nandrino (2003) explained this phenomenon by the hyperarousal state of AN patients, characterized by a constant need to reassess new incoming stimuli using their working memory. Working memory saturation would then be faster than normally expected, reducing in this way the ability to learn in anorexic subjects. This failure to inhibit frequent stimuli may correspond to an impaired learning process in association with a working memory deficit, as suggested by Green and co-workers (1996). Even in simple tasks that could be carried out in a more automatic fashion, patients may favour a controlled information-processing mode causing longer perceptual decision times. Dieters and restrained eaters indeed displayed slower reaction times than non-restrained eaters in the presence of a food-related cue reactivity manipulation (Green, Rogers, Elliman, & Gatenby, 1994) and this effect was stronger during the earlier parts of the cognitive processing measure (Green, Rogers, & Elliman, 2000). Non-clinical subjects scoring high on trait anxiety measures showed interference during the automatic but not the controlled processing of emotional material (MacLeod & Hagan, 1992). Non-ED restrained eaters did not show distortions in the processing of body shape and weights stimuli, nor an early automatic processing priority or a pattern of strategic processing selectivity in Stroop studies (Jansen, Huygens, & Tenney, 1998). This finding might point at a qualitative difference between normal restrained eaters and subjects with eating disorders of clinical severity. Slower information processing and deficiencies in the initiation of an adequate problem-solving strategy appear to be present also in BN patients ((Black, Wilson, Labouvie, & Hefferman, 1997; Carter, Bulik, McIntosh, & Joyce, 2000; Cooper & Fairburn, 1994; Hsu, Kaye, & Weltzin, 1993; Ferraro, Wonderlich, & Jocic, 1997). Moreover, unsuccessful dieters, when compared to successful dieters, show increased appetitive physiological responses to food stimuli and a greater vulnerability to disinhibition of restraint and thus to overeating (Jones & Rogers, 2003). They also have more inflexible attitudes towards food and eating (see review by Mela & Rogers, 1998). As a consequence, they may feel the adverse effects of dietary violation more intensely, leading to a marked increase in preoccupation and further impairment of task performance. 4.2 Cognitive processes Cognitive-behavioural research has addressed cognitions that are relatively available to conscious report (i.e., negative automatic thoughts and conditional assumptions, principally regarding food, shape, and weight). However, there may also be a particular role for schema-level cognitive representations, or core beliefs, which are not related to food, shape or weight, but which reflect unconditional negative beliefs about the self, others, or the world. Indeed, the schematic level of cognitive representation (unconditional core beliefs) may well be responsible for phenomena such as rapid mood swings (a result of the triggering of unconditional core beliefs) and mood suppression (caused by the activation of emotional inhibition beliefs) (see e.g., Young, 1994). Although schema-level representations have received attention particularly in terms of their capacity to explain personality disorder-related pathology, they may also play a specific role in disorders with an impulsive component, such as bulimia, or with a compulsive element, such as restriction (see e.g., Leung, Waller, & Thomas, 1999). The cognitive model suggests that schemata relating to threat, particularly threats to self-esteem are associated with bulimic attitudes and behaviours (Everill & Waller, 1995; Waller et al., 1996; Van Strien, 2000a; Vervaet, van Heeringen, & Audenaert, 2004; Gilbert & Meyer, 2005). These schemata are activated prior to (or even without) conscious awareness of the threat and associated with an attentional bias. Binge eating appears to be one way of reducing the activation or dominance of such threat-related schemata. This phenomenon has been studied using a modified Stroop task, but the results of these studies may have been biased as performance on this task may reflect a number of processes in addition to attention (Foa, Feske, Murdock, Kozak, & McCarthy, 1991), and may be under some degree of conscious control. Therefore, researchers such as Waller & Mijatovich (1998) have used subliminal visual presentation of a threatening stimulus (Patton, 1992). The results from studies using both methods confirm earlier conclusions of other models, i.e. that (1) binging serves the function of reducing awareness of negative affect and cognitions (Lacey, 1986; Root and Fallon, 1989) or that (2) attention is narrowed as a means of reducing awareness of threat, and that eating is disinhibited as a result of this narrowing of attention (Heatherthon and Baumeister, 1991). Clinical experience suggests that both mechanisms take place in the same individual at different times and to different degrees during the development of bulimic disorder (McManus & Waller, 1995). There is evidence that the processes of attentional bias and cognitive avoidance are not independent (de Ruiter & Brosschot, 1994). This hypothesized process is similar to a recent model of anxiety. Beck and Clarke (1997) suggest that information processing is initially automatic, but that it becomes a more strategic, purposeful responding over time. When information is aversive, the initial automatic response (due to an attentional bias to material that is strongly represented cortically) will be followed by a more purposeful form of processing (avoidance of the aversive material). Bulimic psychopathology thus appears to be a cognitive “escape” mechanism (Meyer, Waller, & Watson, 2000). Using a semi-structured interview, it was found that the self-beliefs of ED patients, unlike those of non-ED women, are negative, unconditional and concerned with themes such as abandonment and uselessness (Cooper, Todd, & Wells, 1998). They were also invariably associated with negative early experiences. Underlying assumptions were of two kinds, i.e. (1) weight and shape as a means to selfacceptance, and (2) weight and shape as a means to achieve acceptance from others. There were also assumptions about eating. The interview also indicated that underlying assumptions provide a link between negative self-beliefs and dieting behaviour. Assumptions appeared to be compensatory beliefs, providing a way for the individual to overcome negative self-beliefs (Young, 1994). However, the conclusions from this study were purely qualitative, and no information was provided regarding the validity and reliability of the interview. Results of the study using a revised version of the interview (Turner & Cooper, 2002) with a good inter-rater and test re-test reliability and promising concurrent validity, in AN patients, non-symptomatic dieters and female controls were however comparable. Patients experienced more eating-related negative automatic thoughts than dieters and controls, with similar differences for duration, degree of belief and associated distress. Patients also reported more eating-, weight- and shape-related thoughts than controls and had more underlying assumptions related to eating, and weight and shape as a means to acceptance by self and others. Cooper and colleagues (1998) have recently argued that both negative self-beliefs and underlying assumptions about weight, shape and eating need to be present for the development of an ED. Successful dieting may reduce the distress caused by negative self-beliefs and enhance self-esteem and the feeling of success. These beliefs may thus function as ‘schema compensation beliefs’ (Young, 1990). The enhanced self-esteem is however quickly negated by thoughts of inadequate dieting, which in turn reinforce negative self-beliefs. This process may be similar to the ‘schema maintenance processes’ as identified by Young (1990). The more common use of cognitive strategies involving punishment and worry among individuals, who show a strong tendency of disinhibition, in order to control their thoughts parallels the increased prevalence of these strategies in other clinical domains, such as the anxiety disorders (Wells & Davies, 1994). Furthermore, a rigid and punitive cognitive style is characteristic of dieters and may serve to control food intake as it was shown that the use of punishment correlates positively with ratings of anxiety, distress, perceived thought frequency, and actual number of thoughts about food (Garner & Bemis, 1982). It is unknown however, to what extent these strategies are adopted in response to upsetting intrusive thoughts (Oliver & Huon, 2001). Patients suffering from Binge Eating Disorder (BED) are characterized by substantially more pathological core beliefs than normal controls. A recent study (Waller, 2003) revealed differences between BED and BN for the nature of these core beliefs, but not for the extent of these beliefs, as in previous research (e.g., Ardovini et al., 1999; Wilfley et al., 2000). In this study, patients with BED showed particularly negative beliefs about their ability to experience or express emotions, their ability to function independently, and their need to sacrifice their own needs for those of others. When compared to the BN patients, BED patients reported however less pathological beliefs regarding the likelihood that they would be abandoned. The absence of purging may thus be related to the lower level of abandonment beliefs, and the presence rather than the extent of relatively negative abandonment beliefs drives purging behaviours in BN (Patton, 1992). 4.3. Information processing and emotional arousal In cognitive theories, emotion is a multifaceted phenomenon with cognitive processing of stimuli, motivation, physiological activation, motor behaviour and subjective feeling state (Williams, Watts, MacLeod, & Mathews, 1997). Emotional disorders involve acute reactions with a strong autonomic component but also long-term emotional disturbance and cognitive preoccupations. Some individuals may adopt perfectionist standards as compensation for an underlying inferior sense of self and shame-proneness (Miller, 1996), with early infantile narcissistic origins related to the fear of showing one’s physical, intellectual or emotional defects to others (Jacobson, 1964). Shame is a complex emotion in which the self is judged to have fallen short of an internalised set of standards. Even if experienced in private it is as if the self is being judged by some external other. Shame can be considered as the experience that the self is defective (see e.g. Tangney, 1993), while others conceptualise shame as a psychological consequence of being judged to be low in social rank (see e.g. Gilbert, 1997). Implicit in both these conceptualisations is the notion of social comparison (Lazarus, 1999). Shame, unfavourable social comparison and submissive behaviour are associated with socially prescribed perfectionism, suggesting that these individuals see themselves as rather lacking in status, and it may be this that is particularly pathogenic (Wyatt & Gilbert, 1998). In non-clinical samples shame-proneness is related to eating pathology (Sanftner et al., 1995). In a diary study, Sanftner and Crowther (1998) showed that shame, in general, was more markedly present in women who reported binge eating, but fluctuations in shame were not related to the occurrence of binge episodes. Interestingly, Waller and colleagues (2000) found that a core belief of Defectiveness/Shame was related to the frequency of vomiting. At a group level, Defectiveness/Shame was one of three core beliefs that differentiated between bulimic subgroups (BN, BPAN and BED) and a non-ED comparison group, the other two core beliefs being Insufficient Self-Control and Failure to Achieve. Andrews (1997) found that bodily shame was a better predictor of BN than body dissatisfaction and that bodily shame mediated the relationship between childhood physical or sexual abuse and subsequent BN. Similar to shame, jealousy is a complex emotion involving social comparison (Lazarus, 1999). Two studies have used a sibling-comparison design to assess non-shared childhood environmental effects on the development of AN (Murphy et al., 2000; Karwautz et al., 2001). Both studies found that affected sisters reported having been more jealous of their unaffected sisters in childhood than the unaffected sisters were of them. Women with BN, on the other hand, report their mothers to have been jealous and competitive during their childhoods (Rorty et al., 2000a, 2000b). Thus, while the perception of others being jealous of oneself may be a risk factor for AN and the jealousy of others may be a risk factor in BN, these results suggest that the development of both disorders probably involves competition in the context of social comparison. The role of anger and hostility in ED has received less attention than other emotions. However, these emotions occur more frequently among patients with ED than among non-ED comparison women (e.g. Tiller et al., 1995). Milligan and Waller (2000) found that state anger, but not trait anger, and anger suppression were related to bulimic pathology. Anger suppression was uniquely related to the presence of binge eating, whereas state anger was uniquely related to the presence of vomiting. The authors suggest that their findings support the functional role of bulimia in blocking unpleasant affective states and, in particular, that different symptoms of bulimia may serve different functions. When compared to normal controls, ED women had higher levels of state anger and anger suppression, especially when they displayed bulimic symptoms (Waller, Babbs, Milligan, Meyer, Ohanian, & Leung, 2003). Considering specific symptoms, bingeing and vomiting were associated with trait anger, while excessive exercise was associated with state anger, and laxative abuse was linked to anger suppression. It can thus be hypothesized that bulimic behaviours serve different emotional functions, with a particular contrast between the facets of anger that are influenced by “fast-acting” behaviours (bingeing, vomiting, exercise) and those that are influenced by “slow-acting” behaviours (laxative abuse). When considering the role of cognitions, unhealthy core beliefs were associated with higher levels of trait anger in both groups but were relevant to anger suppression only in the ED patients. Finally, but very interestingly, a specific impairment in the recognition of facial and vocal expressions of emotion was found in ED patients (Kucharska-Pietura, Nikolaou, Masiak, & Treasure, 2004), which was possibly associated with their deficit in social functioning. This impairment may be due to a disturbed emotional processing, which is common to people within the internalizing spectrum of psychopathology (Uher, Murphy, Phillips & Dalgleish, 2001). Another possibility is that people with emotional and interpersonal difficulties have an attention problem in social situations, comparable to the attention deficit in people with social phobia (Chen, Ehlers, Clark, & Mansell, 2002). 4.4. From schemata to behaviour Clinical investigators have described difficulties in identifying, verbally expressing, and regulating a variety of physical tensions among ED patients (Bruch, 1973; Esplen, Garfinkel, & Gallop, 2000; Troop, Schmidt, & Treasure, 1995). According to Bruch, the confusion and apprehension in recognizing and accurately responding to emotional states and the uncertainty in the identification of certain visceral sensations related to hunger and satiety, tapped by the Interoceptive Awareness subscale of the EDI, may result in a pattern of responding to negative mood by food intake, or ‘emotional eating’. Interoceptive awareness is strongly associated with alexithymia (Taylor, Parker, Badbey, & Bourke, 1996). Social self-doubt and unhappiness have been suggested as central elements in inducing the eating pattern of emotional eaters. The escape theory of eating, as formulated by Heatherton and Baumeister (1991), which incorporates elements of externality and psychosomatic theory, stated that a shift to low levels of thinking would result in a reliance on immediate stimuli and in a dampening of affect. Reduced affect and eating would then occur following escape from self-awareness. As described by Van Strien (2000b), Slochower (1983) has also suggested that emotions and environment may operate conjointly to produce overeating. The restraint theory is not valid for all dieters, but mainly for a subpopulation with a strong tendency towards overeating (van Strien, Cleven & Schippers, 2000). Thus, overeating following a preload occurred only in subjects with high scores on both restraint and disinhibition, as measured by the ‘emotional eating’ and ‘external eating’ scales of the Dutch Eating Behaviour Questionnaire (DEBQ; Van Strien, 1986) and the ‘bulimia’ subscale of the Eating Disorder Inventory (EDI; Garner et al., 1983). A possible explanation is that anything that depletes restrained eaters’ self-regulatory strength (as many everyday self-regulatory tasks) may disinhibit subsequent eating (by undermining the ability to maintain cognitive regulation of the intake in the period following other self-regulatory tasks) and disrupt attempts to diet (Kahan, Polivy, Herman, 2003). The false hope of dieting may deplete the dieter’s resources due to the vicious circle of ill-advised attempts followed by failure and self-recrimination (Polivy & Herman, 2000). As described above, perfectionism/rigidity (as a particular risk for the restrictive disorders with overevaluation of achieving) and low self-esteem reflect a specific way of information processing, which may make individuals vulnerable to develop an ED. Exposure to severe interpersonal life events (possibly at least in part due to particular traits such as low sensitivity for social cues or impaired recognition of facial and vocal expression of emotion) or to difficulties in combination with helplessness or inadequate coping (another possible trait), in the presence of this predisposition or vulnerability, may precipitate the development of specific core beliefs, such as negative self beliefs. These cognitions can be expressed by a dysfunctional schema of body shape and weight-based self-esteem resulting in strict dieting, excessive exercise and other weight-control behaviour such as purging (Fairburn et al., 2003). Binge eating can then be a consequence of these restricting behaviours, or, as described above, it can be an escape mechanism in the context of inevitable conflict situations. Consistent with Stice’s (1994, 2001) dual pathway model of eating disorders, dieting and negative affect independently predict the subsequent onset of binge eating (Shepherd & Ricciardelli, 1998; Stice et al., 2000), though only in females. In contrast, experimental studies suggest an interactive effect, the socalled disinhibition of dietary restraint (Ruderman, 1986). Recently, cluster-analytic studies have yielded two subtypes of eating disorders: pure dietary and mixed dietary-negative affect (Grilo, 2004). This last type was characterized by a greater likelihood of binge eating, greater eating-related psychopathology, greater body image dissatisfaction, greater personality disturbance and more commonly reported concerns in clinical areas, including suicidality and childhood abuse. Polivy and Herman’s (1999) study of the disinhibiting effects of emotion on dietary restraint provided no support for the “comfort hypothesis”, which suggests that eating makes people feel better. However, strong support was found for the “masking hypothesis” in which the dieter, by over-eating, misattributes her distress to the problem of over-eating rather than to her real problems. In addition, the findings partially supported the “learned helplessness hypothesis”, in which the experience of stress/distress generalises to all areas in life including the ability to maintain a diet, and the “distraction hypothesis” in which restrained eaters over-eat in order to distract themselves from feeling distressed. Within a cognitive model, it has been proposed that overeating can serve the function of reducing the awareness of threatening information and intolerable emotions (McManus & Waller, 1995). Such a reduction in awareness corresponds with a lowering of activation of relevant cognitive representations (schemata) through distraction or activation of other structures. At a preconscious level, and thus automatic in nature, associations between abandonment and eating-related cognitions were found (Meyer and Waller, 1999, 2000). At a conscious level, women with BN often report a significant event involving the loss of a significant relationship prior to the onset of their disorder. These observations may be causally associated through an outspoken need for approval and feelings of social isolation, which are characteristic of bulimic psychopathology (Tiller et al., 1997). Based on their findings, Meyer and Waller (2000) hypothesized that the core beliefs are more likely to focus on abandonment, whereas negative automatic thoughts and dysfunctional assumptions are more likely to center on issues reflecting food, shape, and weight. This leads to states experienced as a non-specific ‘extreme state of tension’, or to feelings of emptiness and aloneness (Cross, 1993) combined with a lower level of soothing receptivity (Glassman, 1988) and poorer capacity for evocative memory, and finally to a sense of loneliness and to difficulties in the regulation of affect. This may offer an explanation for the limited effects of some current therapies, i.e. those that only address the cognitive elements related to body distortion and dieting behaviours, in treating disturbed regulation of affect (Arntz, 1994). Taken together, theoretical considerations and empirical findings have suggested a variety of reinforcing consequences that may shape and maintain disordered eating behaviours. Specifically, individuals may be more inclined to (1) respond to aversive internal and external events through engagement in disordered eating behaviours that are, in turn, subsequently negatively reinforced as they function to reduce or terminate these aversive events, and (2) engage in disordered eating as to experience immediate gratification of needs or the attainment of immediate reinforcers, or to increase the likelihood of attaining anticipated distal reinforcers (e.g. social approval for a slim appearance). Possibly, reward sensitivity and responsiveness to reward and relief from aversion, and/or insensitivity to signals of threat or punishment are important individual variables associated with the same underlying trait, temperament dimension or brain system (Cloninger, 1987; Gray, 1987). Recently, an association between reward sensitivity and purging behaviours has indeed been reported (Farmer, Nash, & Field, 2002). Through their association with core psychopathological characteristics of ED patients, traitdependent measures may thus be useful for our understanding of the development and maintenance of ED-related symptoms and for the prediction of eating-disordered behaviour. The following section will describe a hypothetical dimensional trait model of ED-related characteristics based on Cloninger’s psychobiological model of personality (Cloninger et al., 1993). Cloninger proposes that specific dimensions of temperament and character interact with one another to influence susceptibility to emotional and behavioural disorders. Cloninger refers to temperament as emotional responses that are moderately heritable, stable throughout life, and mediated by neurotransmitter functioning in the central nervous system. Cloninger’s four proposed temperament dimensions include: novelty seeking (NS), which reflects behavioural activation to pursue rewards and is posited to be related to decreased dopaminergic activity; harm avoidance (HA), which is the tendency to inhibit behavior to avoid punishment and is purported to be related to increased serotonergic activity; reward dependence (RD), which reflects the maintenance of rewarded behaviour and is hypothesized to be mediated by decreased noradrenergic activity; and persistence (P), which is perseverance without intermittent reinforcement that is also purported to be related to decreased noradrenergic activity. Character, in contrast, refers to self-concepts and individual differences in goals and values that develop through experience. The character dimensions are: self-directedness (SD), which is the degree to which the self is viewed as autonomous and integrated; cooperativeness (C), which reflects the degree to which the self is viewed as a part of society; and self-transcendence (ST), which reflects the degree to which the self is viewed as an integral part of the universe. 4. A hypothetical model for the prediction of eating-disordered behaviour Although significant advances regarding the identification of individual risk factors have been made, few prospective studies have tested a multivariate etiologic model that details how these individual risk factors work in concert to promote the development of anorectic and bulimic symptoms. Two prospective studies have provided support for a three-factor interactive model for bulimic symptoms in which the confluence of perfectionism, body dissatisfaction and low self-esteem promotes the development of bulimic symptoms (Vohs, Bardone, Joiner, Abramson, & Heatherton, 1999; Vohs, Voelz, Pettit, Bardone, Katz, Abramson, Heatherthon, & Joiner, 2001). Although a replication study did not support this model, the results confirmed those from previous prospective studies of the importance and significant effect of body dissatisfaction (Shaw, Stice, & Springer, 2004). According to the cognitive model of the maintenance of eating disorders (Fairburn, 1997), the core psychopathology, even among anorectic patients (Carter, Blackmore, Sutandar-Pinnock, & Woodside, 2004) is the over concern with body shape and weight. Due to this over concern, self-worth is judged largely or even exclusively in terms of satisfaction with weight and shape. The function of this overconcern is probably problem-avoidance. Taking into account the need for absolute self-control in AN patients (Fairburn et al., 1999), food restriction in combination with excessive physical activity was found to be highly reinforcing. This reinforcing effect is most probably related to the fact that these behaviours increase circulating levels of endogenous opiates that activate dopamine in the brain’s mesolimbic reward centres (Carter et al., 2004; Davis, Katzman, Kaptein, Kirsh, Brewer, Kalmbach, Olmsted, Woodside, & Kaplan, 1997; Strober, Freeman, Morrell, 1997). Many studies provide evidence for a strong relationship between impulsivity and eating-disordered behaviour. A higher score on measures of impulsive behaviour has been described in eating disordered individuals, especially in adolescents (Wonderlich, Connolly, & Stice, 2004). There is, however, a difference between trait-oriented impulsivity and behavioural measures of impulsivity. It can be hypothesized that the more outspoken behavioural impulsivity in behaviour among ED patients, particularly in those showing binging-purging behaviours, is the expression of the temperament dimension ‘Novelty Seeking’, whether or not in combination with a poorer character development as measured by ‘Self-directedness’. Based on a similar line of reasoning, it can be hypothesized that ED patients are characterised by a high score on ‘Harm Avoidance’, expressed by their ‘Perfectionism’ and ‘restricting behaviour’ as a need for control, and a low score on ‘Reward Dependence’, reflecting their rigidity or inability to cope with (social) situations. A higher ‘Persistence’ among ED patients, when compared to normal controls, could explain the persistent nature of avoidance behaviours, in spite of their adverse effects in the long run after the short-term reinforcing effects. With regard to ‘Novelty Seeking’ especially differences between the restricting type (RAN) and the binge-purging type (BPAN and BP) can be expected, expressed by means of a more outspoken ‘body dissatisfaction’ and more frequent ‘emotional and external eating’, combined with more ‘purging’ in the latter type. In view of the common high levels of helplessness, ED patients can be expected to have lower scores on the character dimensions, in particular the ‘Selfdirectedness’ dimension, possibly reflecting low self-esteem. Scores on the self-directedness dimension may even be lower among BN patients, as these individuals do not succeed in restricting and avoiding and are conditioned with regard to their escape behaviours by binging or/and purging. The following part of this thesis is constituted by studies of these hypothetical associations in the context of the proposed dimensional trait model of factors predisposing to ED. References Ainsworth C, Waller G, & Kennedy F (2002). Threat processing in women with bulimia. Clinical Psychology Review, 22, 1155-1178. Andersen AE, Bowers WA, & Watson T (2001). A slimming program for eating disorders not otherwise specified. Reconceptualizing a confusing, residual diagnostic category. Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 24, 271-280. Andreassi JL (2000). Associates. Psychophysiology. Human behaviour and physiological respons. London: Lawrence Erlbaum Andrews B (1997) Bodily shame in relation to abuse in childhood and bulimia: A preliminary investigation. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 36, 41-49. Ardovini C, Caputo G, Todisco P, & Grave RD (1999). Binge eating and restraint model: Psychometric analysis in binge eating disorder and normal weight bulimia. European Eating Disorders Review, 7, 293-299. Arnow B, Kenardy J, & Agras WS (1995). The emotional eating scale: The development of a measure to assess coping with negative affect by eating. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 18, 79-90. Arntz A (1994). Treatment of borderline personality disorder: A challenge for cognitive-behavioural therapy. Behavioural Research and Therapy, 32, 419-430. Bastiani AM, Altemus M, Pigott T, Rubenstein C, Weltzin TE, & Kaye WH (1996). Comparison of obsession and compulsion in patients with anorexia nervosa and obsessive-compulsive disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 39, 966-969. Bastiani, AM, Rao R, Weltzin T & Kaye W (1995) Perfectionism in anorexia nervosa. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 17(2), 147-52. Beck AT, & Clarke DA (1997). An information processing model of anxiety: Attentional bias or cognitive avoidance? Behaviour Research and Therapy, 32, 315-319. Bemporad J (1996). Self-starvation through the ages: reflexions on the pre-history of anorexia nervosa. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 19, 217-237. Bennet D, Sharpe M, Freeman C & Carson A (2004). Anorexia nervosa among female secondary school students in Ghana. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 185, 312-317. Bhadrinath ER (1990). Anorexia nervosa in adolescents of Asian extraction. British Journal of Psychiatry, 156, 565-568. Bloks H, Spinhoven P, Calewaert I, Willemse-Koning C, & Turksma A (2001). Changes in coping styles and recovery after inpatients treatment for severe eating disorders. European Eating Disorders Review, 9, 397-415. Borkovec TD, Ray WJ, & Stöber (1998). Worry: A cognitive phenomenon intimately linked to affective, physiological, and interpersonal behavioral processes. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 22, 561-567. Brecelj M, Tchanturia K, Rabe-Hesketh S & Treasure JL (in press). Rigidity traits in the child- hoods of eating disorder patients. American Journal of Psychiatry. Brewerton TD, Lydiard BR, Laraia MT, Shook JE, & Ballenger JC (1992). CSF Betaendorphin and dynorphin in bulimia nervosa. American Journal of Psychiatry, 149, 1086-1090. Brownell KD (1991). Dieting and the search for the perfect body: Where physiology and culture collide. Behavior Therapy, 22, 1-12. Bruch H (1973). Eating disorders: Obesity, anorexia nervosa, and the person within. New York: Basic Books. Bruch H (1978). The golden cage: the enigma of anorexia nervosa. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Bruch H (1981). Developmental considerations of anorexia nervosa and obesity. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 26, 212217. Bulik CM, Sullivan PF, Fear JL, Pickering A. (2000). Outcome of anorexia nervosa: eating attitudes, personality, and parental bonding. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 28(2), 139-47. Bulik CM, Sullivan PF, Joyce PR, & Carter, F.A. (1995a). Temperament, character, and personality disorder in bulimia nervosa. Journal of Nervous & Mental Disease, 183, 593-598 Bulik, C.M., Sullivan, P.F., Joyce, P.R., Carter, F.A., & McIntosh, V.V. (1998). Predictors of 1-year treatment outcome in bulimia nervosa. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 39, 206-214. Bulik, C.M., Sullivan, P.F., Wade, T.D. & Kendler, K.S. (2000) Twin studies of eating disorders: A review. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 27, 2-20. Bulik, C.M., Sullivan, P.F., Weltzin, T.E., & Kay, W.H. (1995). Temperament in eating disorders. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 17, 251-261. Bulik CM, Tozzi F, Anderson C, Mazzeo SE, Aggen S, Sullivan PF. (2003). The relation between eating disorders and components of perfectionism. American Journal of Psychiatry, 160(2), 366-8. Bushnell, JA, Wells, JE, & Oakley-Browne, MA (1996). Impulsivity in disordered eating, affective disorder, and substance use disorder. Britisch Journal of Psychiatry, 169, 329-333. Button E (1985). Eating disorders: A quest for control? In E.Button (Ed.), Persoanl construct theory and mental health (pp. 153-168). London: Croom Helm. Button, E.J., Sonuga-Barke, E.J., Davies, J., & Thompson, M. (1996). A prospective study of self-esteem in the prediction of eating problems in adolescent schoolgirls: Questionnaire findings. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 35 (2), 193-203. Bydlowski S, Corcos M, Jeammet P, Paterniti S, Berthoz S, Laurier C, Chambry J, & Consoli SM (2005). Emotion-processing deficits in eating disorders. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 37, 4, 321-329. Calam R, Waller G, Slade PD, & Newton T (1990). Eating disorders and perceived relationships with parents. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 9, 479-485. Carroll JM, Touyz SW & Beumont PJV (1996) Specjfic comorbidity between bulimia nervosa and personality disorders. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 19 (2), 159-170. Carter FA, Bulik CM, McIntosh VV, & Joyce PR (2000). Changes on the Stroop test following treatment: relation to word type, treatment condition, and treatment outcome among women with bulimia nervosa. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 28, 349-355. Carter JC, Blackmore K, Sutandar-Pinnock, & Woodside (2004). Psychological Medicine, 34, 671-679. Relapes in anorexia nervosa: a survival analysis. Casper RC, Hedeker D & McClough JF (1992) Personality dimensions in eating disorders and their subtyping. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 31 (5), 830-840. Chen YP, Ehlers A, Clark DM, & Mansell W (2002). Patients with generalized social phobia direct their attention awoy from faces. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 40, 677-687. Claes L, Vandereycken W, & Vertommen H (2002). Impulsive and compulsive traits in eating disordered patients compared with controls. Personality and Individual Differences, 32, 707-714. Cloninger CR, Svrakic DM & Przybeck TR (1993). A psychobiological model of temperament and character. Archives of General Psychiatry, 50, 975-990. Cloninger CR (1987a). Neurogenetic adaptive mechanisms in alcoholism. Science, 236, 410-416. Cloninger CR (1987b). A systematic method for clinical description and classification of personality variants. Archives of General Psychiatry , 44, 573-588. Coccaro EF, Kavoussi RJ, & Lesser JC (1992). Self- and other-directed human aggression : The role of the central serotonergic system. International Clinical Psychopharmacology, 6, 70-83. Cockell SJ, Hewitt PL, Seal B, Sherry SB, Goldner EM, Flett GL, & Remick RA (2002). Trait and self-presentational dimensions of perfectionism among women with anorexia nervosa. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 26, 745-758 Collier DA, & Treasure JL (2004). The aetiology of eating disorders. British Journal of Psychiatry, 185, 363-365. Connan F, & Treasure J (1998). Stress, eating and neurobiology. In H.W. Hoeck, JL Treasure, & M Katzman (Eds.), Neurobiology in the treatment of eating disorders (pp.211-236). Chichester, UK: Wiley. Cooley E, & Toray T (2001). Disordered eating in college Freshman women: A prospective study. Journal of American College Health, 49, 5, pp 229-236. Cooper M, & Todd G (1997). Selective processing of three types f stimuli in eating disorders. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 36, 279-281. Cooper, MJ, & Fairburn CG (1993). Confusion over the core psychopathology of bulimia nervosa. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 13, 385-389. Cooper MJ, Todd G, & Wells A (1998). Content, origin and consequences of dysfunctional beliefs in anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa. Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy, 12, 213-230. Cooper Z, Cooper PJ, & Fairburn CG (1989). The validity of the Eating Disorder Examination and its subscales. British Journal of Psychiatry, 154, 807-812. Crick NR, & Dodge KA (1994). A review and reformulation of social nformation prosessing mechanisms in children’s social adjustment. Psychological Bulletin, 113, 487-496. Cross LW (1993). Body and self in feminine development: Implications for eating disorders and delicate self-mutilation. Bulletin of the Menninger Clinic, 57, 41-68. Currin L, Schmidt U, Treasure J, & Jick H (2005). Time trends in eating disorder incidence. British Journal of Psychiatry, 186, 132-135. Dare C, LeGrange D, Eisler I, & Rutherford J (1994). Redefining the psychosomatic family: family process of 26 eating disorder families. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 16, 211-226. Davis C, Katzman DK, Kaptein S, Kirsh C, Brewer H, Kalmbach K, Olmsted MP, Woodside DB & Kaplan AS (1997). The prevalence of high-level exercise in the eating disorders: etiological implications. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 38, 321-326. DeGroot, J & Rodin G (1998). Coming alive: the psychotherapeutic treatment of patients with eating disorders. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 43, 359-366. De Ruiter C, & Brosschot JF (1994). The emotional Stroop interference effect in anxiety: attentional bias or cognitive avoidance? Behaviour Research and Therapy, 32 (3), 315-319. DeVeaugh-Geiss J (1991). Pharmacological treatment of obsessive-compulsive disorder. In J.Zohan, T.Insel, & S. Rasmussen (Eds.), The psychobiology of obsessive-compulsive disorder (pp. 187-207). New York: Springer. Dodin V, & Nandrino JL (2003). Cognitive processing of anorexic patients in recognition tasks: An event-related potentials study. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 33 (3), 299-307. Esplen MJ, Garfinkel P, & Gallop R (2000). Relationship between self-soothing, aloneness, and evocative memory in bulimia nervosa. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 27, 96-100. Everill J, & Waller G (1995). Dissociation and bulimia: Research and theory. European Eating Disorders Review, 3, 129147. Fahy T, & Eisler I (1993) Impulsivity and eating disorders. British Journal of Psychiatry, 162, 193-197. Fairburn CG (1997). Eating disorders. In D.M. Clark & C.G. Fairburn (Eds.), Cognitive behaviour therapy, Oxford University press, New York. Fairburn CG, Cooper Z, Doll HA & Welch SL (1999) Risk factors for anorexia nervosa: Three integrated case-control comparison. Archives of General Psychiatry, 56, 468-476. Fairburn CG, Cooper Z & Shafran R (2003). Cognitive behaviour therapy for eating disorders: A ‘transdiagnostic’ theory and treatment. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 41, 509-528. Fairburn CG, Cowen PJ, Harrison PJ. (1999). Twin studies and the etiology of eating disorders. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 26(4), 349-58. Fairburn CG, & Harrison PJ. (2003). Eating disorders. Lancet, 361 (9355), 407-16. Fairburn CG, Shafran R & Cooper Z (1998). A cognitive behavioural theory of anorexia nervosa. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 37, 1-13. Fairburn CG, Welch SL, Doll HA, Davies BA & O'Connor ME (1997). Risk factors for bulimia nervosa. A community-based case-control study. Archives of General Psychiatry, 54, 509-517. Farmer RF, Nash HM, & Field CE (2002). Disordered eating behaviours and reward sensitivity. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 32, 211-219. Fassino S, Piero A, Abbate Daga G, Leombruni P, Mortara P, & Rovera GG (2002). Attentional biases and frontal functioning in Anorexia Nervosa. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 31, 274-283. Ferraro FR, Wonderlich S, & Jocic Z (1997). Performance Variability as a New Theoretical Mechanism regarding eating disorders an cognitive processing. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 53 (2), 117-121. Festinger L (1954). A theory of social comparison processes. Human Relations, 7, 117-140. Fisher S, Smith GT, Anderson KG (2003). Clarifying the orle of impulsivity in bulimia nervosa. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 33, 406-411. Flament MF, Godart NT, Fermanian J, & Jeammet P (2002). Predictive factors of social disablility in patients with eating disorders. Eating and Weight Disorders, 6, 99-107. Foa EB, Feske U, Murdock TB, Kozak MJ, & McCarthy PR (1991). Processing of threat-related information in rape victims. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100, 156-162. Frantz-Fox C (1981). Neuropsychological correlates of anorexia nervosa. International Journal of Psychiatry in Medicine, 11, 285-290. Freeman R, Touyz S, Sara G, Rennie Ch, Gordon E, & Beumont P (1991). In the eye of the beholder: Processing body shape information in anorexic and bulimic patients. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 6, 709-714. Frost RO, Marten P, Lahart C, & Rosenblate R (1990). The dimensions of perfectionism. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 14, 449-468. Garner DM, & Bemis KM (1982). A cognitve-behavioural approach to anorexia nervosa. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 6, 123-150. Garner DM, & Garfinkel PE (1980). Socio-cultural factors in the development of anorexia nervosa. Psychological Medicine, 10, 647-656. Garner DM, Olmstead MP, & Polivy J (1983). Development and validation of a multidimensional Eating Disorder Inventory for anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 2, 15-24. Garner DM, Olmstead MP, Polivy J, & Garfinkel PE (1984). Comparison between weight-preoccupied woman and anorexia nervosa. Psychosomatic Medicine, 46, 255-266. Gilbert P (1997). The evolution of social attractiveness and its role in shame, humiliation, guilt and therapy. British Journal of Medical Psychology, 70, 113-147. Gilbert N, & Meyer C (2005). Fear of negative evaluation and the development of eating psychopathology: a longitudinal study among nonclinical women. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 37, 4, 307-312. Gillberg I, Rastam M & Gillberg C. (1995) Anorexia nervosa 6 years after onset: Part I. Personality disorders. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 36 (I), 61-69. Gilligan C (1993). In a Different Voice: Psychological Theory and Women’s Development. 2nd edn. Havard University Press: Cambridge, MA. Glassman EJ (1988). Development of a self-report measure of soothing receptivity. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. Toronto: York University (GENERIC). Goldbloom DS, Garfinkel PE, & Shaw BF (1991). Biochemical aspects of bulimia nervosa. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 35, 11-22. Goldner EM, Cockell SJ & Srikameswaran S. (2002) Perfectionism and eating disorders. In P.L. Hewitt & G.L. Flett (Eds), Perfectionism: Theory, Research and Interventions. APA Books: Washington DC. Goldner EM, Srikameswara S, Schroeder ML, Livesley WJ, & Birmingham CL (1999). personality pathology in patients with eating disorders. Psychiatric Research, 85, 151-159. Dimensional assessment of Gray JA (1987). The psychology of fear and stress (2nd ed). New York: Cambridge University Press. Green MW, Elliman NA, & Rogers PJ (1997). Impaired cognitive performance in dieters: Failure of attentional focus or resource capacity limitation? British Journal of Health Psychology, 2, 259-267. Green MW, Elliman NA, Wakeling A, & Rogers PJ (1996). Cognitive functioning, weight change and therapy in anorexia nervosa. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 30, 401-410. Green MW, & Rogers PJ (1995). Impaired cognitive functioning during spontaneous dieting. Psychological Medicine, 25, 1003-1010. Green MW, & Rogers PJ (1998). Impairments in working memory associated with spontaneous dieting behaviour. Psychological Medicine, 28, 1063-1070. Green MW, Rogers PJ, Elliman NA, & Gatenby SJ (1994). Impairment of cognitive performance associated with dieting and high levels of dietary restraint. Physiology and Behavior, 55, 447-452. Gregory RL (1998). OI_MRKOI_MRKEye and brain. The psychology of seeing. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Grilo CM (2004). Subtyping female adolescent psychiatric inpatients with features of eating disorders along dietary restraint and negative affect dimensions. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 42, 67-78. Grilo CM, Sanislow CA, Shea MT, Skodol AE, Stout RL, Pagano ME, Yen S, & McGlashan TH (2003a). The natural course of bulimia nervosa and eating disorder not otherwise specified is not influenced by personality disorders. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 34, 319-330. Grilo CM, Sanislow CA, Skodol AE, Gunderson JG, Stout RL, Shea MT, Zanarini MC, Bender DS, Morey LC, Dyck IR, & McGlashan TH (2003b). Do eating disorders co-occur with personality disorders? Comparison Groups Matter, International Journal of Eating Disorders, 33, 155-164. Groesz LM, Levine MP, & Murnen SK (2002). The effect of experimental presentation f thin media images on body satisfaction: A meta-analytic review. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 31, 1-15. Halmi KA (1999). Eating disorders: Anorexia nervosa, bulimia nervosa and obesity. In K.E. Hales, S.C. Yodofsky, & J. Talbot (Eds.), American psychiatric press textbook of psychiatry (3rd ed., pp. 983-1002). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association. Halmi K, Goldberg S, Eckert E, Casper R & Davis J. (1977) Pretreatment evaluation in anorexia nervosa. In R.A. Vigersky (Ed.), Anorexia Nervosa. New York: Raven Press. Halmi KA, Sunday SR, Klump KL, Strober M, Leckman JF, Fichter M, Kaplan A, Woodsid B, Treasure J, Berrettini WH, Shabboat MA, Bulik C, & Kaye WH (2003). Obsessions and compulsions in Anorexia Nervosa Subtypes. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 308-319. Halmi KA, Sunday SR, Strober M, Kaplan A, Woodside DB, Fichter M, Treasure J, Berrettini WH, & Kay WH (2000). Perfectionism in anorexia nervosa: variation by clinical subtype, obsessionality, and pathologic eating behaviour. American Journal of Psychiatry , 157(11), 1799-1805. Hamsher K, Halmi KA, & Benton AL (1981). Prediction of outcome in anorexia nervosa from neuro- psychological status. Psychiatry Research, 4, 79-88. Hargreaves D & Tiggemann M (2002). The role of appearance schematicity in the development of adolescent body dissatisfaction. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 26 (6), 691-700. Haudek C, Rorty M, & Henker B (1999). The role of ethnicity and parental bonding in the eating and weight concerns of Asian-American and Caucasian college women. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 25, 425-433. Heatherton TF, & Baumeister RF (1991). Binge eating as escape from self-awareness. Psychological Bulletin, 110, 86-108. Hewitt PL, & Flett GL (1991). Perfectionism in the self and social contexts: Conceptualization, assessment, and association with psychopathology. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 456-470. Hewitt PL, & Flett GL (1993). Dimensions of perfectionism, daily stress, and depression: A tst of the specific vulnerability hypothesis. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 102, 58-65. Hewitt PL, Flett GL, Besser A, Sherry SB, & McGee B (2002). Perfectionism is multidimensional: a reply to Shafran, Cooper and Fairburn. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 41, 1221-1236. Hewitt PL, & Flett GL, & Edinger E (1995). Perfectionism traits and perfectionistic self-presentation in eating disorder attitudes, characteristics, and symptoms. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 18, 317-326. Hewitt PL, Flett GL, & Turnbull-Donovan W (1992). Perfectionism and suicide potential. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 31, 181-190. Hilbert A, Tuschen-Caffier B, & Vögele C (2002). Effects of prolonged and repeated body image exposure in binge-eating disorder. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 52, 137-144. Hoek HW, & van Hoeken D (2003). Review of the prevalence and incidence of eating disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry, 152, 1272-1278. Holland AJ, Hall A, Murray R, Russell GF & Crisp AH (1984). Anorexia nervosa: a study of 34 twin pairs and one set of triplets. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 145, 414-419. Holland AJ, Sicotte N & Treasure J (1988). Anorexia nervosa: evidence for a genetic basis. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 32 (6), 561-571. Horne RL, Ferguson JM, Pope HJ, Hudson JI, Lineberry CG, Ascher J, Cato A (1988). Treatment of bulimia with bupropion: a multicenter controlled trial. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 49, 262-266. Hornyak LM, Baker EK (1989). Experiential Therapies for Eating Disorders. New York, Guilford Press. Howard CE, & Porzelius LK (1999). The role of dieting in Binge Eating Disorder: Etiology and treatment implications. Clinical Psycholgy Review, 19(1), 25-44. Hsu LKG (1990). Eating Disorders. New York, Guilford Press. Hsu LK, Kaye W & Weltzin T (1993). Are the eating disorders related to obsessive compulsive disorder? International Journal of Eating Disorders, 14, 305-318. Humphrey LL (1989). Observed family interactions among subtypes of eating disorders using struvtural analysis of social behaviour. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 57, 206-214. Hunter EC, & O’Connor RC (2003). Hopelessness and future thinking in parasuicide: The role of perfectionism. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 42, 355-365. Ilkjaer K, Kortegaard L, Hoerder K, Joergensen J, Kyvik K, & Gillberg C (2004). Personality Disorders in a Total Population Twin Cohort with Eating Disorders. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 45, 261-267. Inceoglu I, Franzen U, Backmund H & Gerlinghoff M. (2000) Personality disorders in patients in a day-treatment programme for eating disorders. European Eating Disorders Review, 8, 67-72. Jacobson E (1964). The self and the object world. New York: International Universities Press. Jansen A, Nederkoorn C & Mulkens S (2005). Selective visual attention for ugly and beautiful body parts in eating disorders. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 43, 2, 183-196. Jansen A, Huygens K, & Tenney N (1998). No evidence for a selective processing of subliminally presented body words in restrained eaters. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 24, 435-438. Jimmerson DC, Lesem MD, Kate WH, & Brewerton TD (1992). Low serotonin and dopamine metabolite concentrations in cerebrospinal fluid from bulimic patients with frequent binge episodes. Archives of General Psychiatry, 49, 132-138. Johnson C, Tobin DL, Dennis A (1990). Differences in treatment outcome between borderline and non-borderline bulimics at one-year follow-up. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 9, 616-627. Jones N, & Rogers PJ (2003). Preoccupation, Food, and Failure: An Investigation of cognitive performance deficits in Dieters. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 33, 185-192. Kahan D, Polivy J, & Herman CP (2003). Conformity and dietary disinhibition: a tst of the ego-strenght model of selfregulation. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 33, 165-171. Karwautz A, Rabe-Hesketh S, Hu X, Zhao J, Sham P, Collier DA & Treasure JL (2001) Individual-specific risk factors for anorexia nervosa: A pilot study using a discordant sister-pair design. Psychological Medicine, 31, 317-329. Karwautz A, Troop NA, Rabe-Hesketh S, Collier DA, & Treasure JL (2003). dimensions in anorexia nervosa. Journal of Personality Disorders, 17 (1), 73-85. Personality disorders and personality Katzman AS, & Wolchik SA (1984). Bulimia and binge eating in college women: A comparison of personality and behavioural characteristics. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 52, 423-428. Katzman MA, & Lee S (1997). Beyond body image: the integration of feminist and transcultural the-ories in the understanding of self-starvation. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 22, 385-394. Kay WH, Bastiani AM, & Moss H (1995). Cognitive style of patients with anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 18, 287-290. Kaye WH, Frank GK, Meltzer CC, Price JC, McConaha CW, Crossan PJ, Klump KL & Rhodes L (2001). Altered serotonin 2A receptor activity in women who have recovered from bulimia nervosa. American Journal of Psychiatry, 158 (7), 11521155. Kaye WH, Gwirtsman HE, George DT & Ebert MH (1991). Altered serotonin activity in anorexia nervosa after long-term weight restoration. Archives of General Psychiatry , 48, 556-562. Kaye WH, Lilenfeld LR, Berrettini WH, Strober M, Devlin B, Klump KL, Goldman D, Bulik CM, Halmi KA, Fichter MM, Kaplan A, Woodside DB, Treasure J, Plotnicov KH, Pollice C, Rao R, McConaha CW. (2000). A search for susceptibility loci for anorexia nervosa: methods and sample description. Biol Psychiatry, 47(9):794-803. Kaye WH, Weltzin TE, Hsu LK, Bulik CM: An open trial of fluoxetine in patients with an-orexia nervosa. J Clin Psychiatry 1991; 52:464-471. Keel PK, & Klump KL (2003). Are eating disorders culture-bound syndromes? Implications for conceptualizing their etiology. Psychological Bulletin, 129, 747-769. Kendler KS, Gardner CO, & Prescott CA (1998). A population-based twin study of self-esteem and gender. Psychological Medicine, 28, 1403-1409. Key A, George CL, Beattie D, Stammers K, Lacey H, & Waller G (2002). Body image treatment within an inpatient program for anorexia nervosa: The role of mirror exposure in the desensitization process. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 31, 185-190. Khandelwal S, & Saxena S (1990). Anorexia nervosa in people of Asian extraction. Britisch Journal of Psychiatry, 157, 783784. Kiemle, G., Slade, P.D. & Dewey, M.E. (1987) Factors associated with abnormal eating attitudes and behaviors: Screening individuals at risk of developing an eating disorder. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 6, 713-724. Kingston K, Szmukler G, Andrewes D, Tress B, Desmond P (1996). Neuropsychological and struc-tural brain changes in anorexia nervosa before and after refeeding. Psychological Medicine, 26, 15-28. Klump KL, Bulik CM, Pollice C, Halmi KA., Fichter MM, Berrettini WH, Devlin B, Strober M, Kaplan A, Woodside DB, Treasure J, Shabbout, M., Lilenfeld, L.R., & Plotnicov, K.H. (2000). Temperament and character in women with anorexia nervosa. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 188 (9), 559-567. Klump KL, Miller KB, Keel PK, McGue M & Iacono, WG (2001). Genetic and environmental influences on anorexia nervosa syndromes in a population-based twin sample. Psychological Medicine, 31, 737-740. Kortegaard LS, Hoerder K, Joergensen J, Gillberg C, & Kybik KO (2001). A preliminary population-based twin study of self-reported eating disorder. Psychological Medicine, 31, 737-740. Kucharska-Pietura K, Nikolaou V, Masiak M, & Treasure J (2004). The recognition of emotion in the faces and voice of anorexia nervosa. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 35, 42-47. Kuehnel RH, & Wadden TA (1994). Binge Eating Disorder, weight cycling, and psychopathology. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 15 (4), 321-329. Lacey JH (1986). Pathogenesis. In L.J. Downey & J.C. Malkin (Eds.), Current approaches: Bulimia nervosa (pp. 17-26). Southampton: Duphar. Lacey JH (1993) Self-damaging and addictive behaviour in bulimia nervosa. British Journal of Psychiatry, 163, 190-194. Lacey JH, & Read T (1993) Multi-impulsive bulimia: description of an inpatient eclectic treatment programme and a pilot follow-up study of its efficacy. European Eating Disorders Review, I, 22-31. Lauer CJ, Gorzewski B, Gerlinghoff M, Backmund H, & Zihl, J. (1999). Neuropsychological assessments before and after treatment in patients with anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 33, 129-138. Lazarus RS (1999). Stress and Emotion: A New Synthesis. London: Free Association Books. Leckman JF, Zhang H, Alsobrook JP, & Pauls DL (in press). Symptom dimensions in obsessive-compulsive disorder: Toward quantitative phenotypes. American Journal of Genetics. Lee S (1996). Reconsidering the status of anorexia nervosa as a western culture-bound syndrome. Social Science and Medicine, 42, 21-34. Lee S, Ho P., & Hsu LKG.(1993). Fat phobic and non-fat phobic anorexia nervosa: A comparative study of 70 Chinese patients in Hong Kong. Psychological Medicine, 23, 999-1017. Leung N, Waller G, & Thomas G (1999). Core beliefs in anorexic and bulimic women. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 187, 736-741. Le Grange D, Telch C & Tibbs J (1998). Eating attitudes and behaviours in 1435 South African Caucasian and NonCaucasian college students. American Journal of Psychiatry, 155, 250-254. Lilenfeld LR, Devlin B, Bulik CM, Strober M, Berrettini W, Fichter M, Goldman D, Halmi K, Kaplan AS, Treasure J, Woodside DB, & Kaye WH (2000). Deriving behavioural phenotypes in an international multicenter genetic study of eating disorders. Psychological Medicine, 30, 1399-1410. Lilenfeld LR, Kaye WH, Greeno CG, Merikangas KR, Plotnicov K, Pollice C, Rao R, Strober M, Bulik CM, Nagy L. (1998). A controlled family study of anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa: psychiatric disorders in first-degree relatives and effects of proband comorbidity. Archives of General Psychiatry, 55(7):603-10. Lilenfeld LR., Stein D, Bulik CM, Strober M, Plotnicov K, Pollice C, Rao R, Merikangas KR, Nagy L, Kaye WH (2000). Personality traits among currently eating disordered, recovered and never ill first-degree female relatives of bulimic and control women. Psychological Medicine,30, 1399-1410. Long CG, Hinton C, & Gillepsie NK (1994). Selective processing of food and body size words: application of the Stroop test with obese restrained eaters, anorexics, and normals. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 15, 279-283. Löwe B, Zipfel S, Buchholz C, Dupont Y, Reas DL & Herzog, W (2001). Long-term outcome of anorexia nervosa in a prospective 21-year follow-up study. Psychological Medicine, 31, 881-890. MacLeod C (1999). Anxiety and anxiety disorders. In T.Dalgleish & M. Power (Eds.), Handbook of cognition and emotion (pp. 447-478). Chichester, UK: Wiley. MacLeod C, & Hagan R (1992). Individual differences in the selective processing of threatening information, and emotional responses to a stressful life event. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 30, 151-161. Major B, Testa M, & Blysma WH (1991). Responses to upward and downward social comparisons: the impact of esteemrelevance and perceived control. In J. Suls & T.A. Ashby (Eds.), Social comparison: Contemporary theory and research (pp. 237-260). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Mathias JL, & Kent PS (1998). Neuropsychological consequences of extreme weight loss and dietary restriction in patients with anorexia nervosa. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 20, 548-564. Matsunaga H, Kaye W & McConaha S (2000) Personality disorders among subjects recovered from eating disorders. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 27, 353-357. Matsunaga H, Kaye WH, McConaha C, Plotnicov K, Pollice C, & Rao R (2000). Personality disorders among subjects recovered from eating disorders. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 27, 353-357. Matsunaga H, Miyaga A, Iwasaki H, Matsui T, Fujimoto K, & Kiriike N (1999). A comparison of clinical features among Japanese eating-disordered women with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Comparative Psychiatry, 40, 337-342. Mazure CN, Halmi KA, Sunday SR, Romano S, & Einhorn A (1994). The Yale-Brown Cornell Eating Disorder Scale: Development, use, reliability and validity. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 28, 425-445. McCabe MP, & Ricciardelli LA (2005). A prospective study of pressures from parents, peers, and the media on extreme weight change behaviors among adolescent boys and girls. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 43, 653-668. McManus F, & Waller G (1995). A functional analysis of binge-eating. Clinical Psychology Review, 15, 845-865. McManus F, Waller G, & Chadwick P (1996). Biases in the processing of different forms of threat in bulimic and comparison women. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 184, 547-554. Mela DJ, & Rogers PJ (1998). Food, eating and obesity: The psychobiological basis of appetite and weight control, London: Chapman and Hall. Meyer C, & Waller G (1999). The impact of emotion upon eating behavior: the role of subliminal visual processing of threat cues. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 25, 319-326. Meyer C, & Waller G (2000). Subliminal activation of abandonment- and eating-related schemata: relationship with eating disordered attitudes in a nonclinical population. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 27, 328-334. Meyer C, Waller G, & Watson D (2000). Cognitive avoidance and bulimic psychopathology: the relevance of temporal factors in a nonclinical population. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 27, 405-410. Miller SB (1996). Shame in context. London The Analytic Press. Milligan RJ & Waller G (2000) Anger and bulimic psychopathology among nonclinical women. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 28, 446-450. Mogg K, Bradley BP, Hyare H, & Lee S (1997). Selective attention to food-related stimuli in hunger: are attentional biases specific to emotional and psychopathological state, or are they also found in normal drive states? Behaviour Research and Therapy, 36, 227-237. Moser DJ, Benjamin ML, Bayless JD, BD, McDowell, Paulsen JS, Bowers WA, Arndt S, & Anderson AE (2003). Neuropsychological functioning pretreatment and posttreatment in an inpatient eating disorders program. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 33, 64-70. Muller B, Herpertz S, Heussen N, Neudrorfl A, Wewetzer C, Remschmidt H, & Herpertz-Dahlmann B (2000). Personality disorders and psychiatric morbidity in adolescent anorexia nervosa. Results of a prospective 10 year catmnesis. Z Kinder Jungendpsychiatr Psycother, 28, 81-91. Murphy F, Troop NA & Treasure JL (2000) Differential environmental factors in anorexia nervosa: A sibling-pair study. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 39, 193-203. Neckowitz P, & Morrison TL (1991) Interactional coping strategies of normal-weight bulimic women in intimate and nonintimate stressful situations. Psychological Reports, 69, 1167-1175. Nasser MA, Katzman MA & Gordon RA (2001). Eating Disorders and Cultures in transition. Taylor & Francis: Andover, Hants, pp 201. Newton JR, Freeman CT, & Munro J (1993). Impulsivity and dyscontrol in bulimia nervosa: Is impulsivity an independent phenomenon or a marker of severity? Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavia, 87, 389-394. Ogden J, & Steward J (2000). The role of the mother-daugher relationship in explaining weight concern. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 28, 78-83. Oliver KG, & Huon GF (2001). Eating-related thought suppression in high and low disinhibitors. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 30, 329-337. Oyewumi LK & Kazarian SS (1992a). Abnormal eating attitudes among a group of Nigerian youths: I. Bulimic behaviour. East African Medical Journal, 69, 663-666. Oyewumi LK & Kazarian SS (1992b). Abnormal eating attitudes among a group of Nigerian youths: II. Anorexic behaviour. East African Medical Journal, 69, 667-669. Patch AR (1984). Reflections on perfection. American Psychologist, 39, 386-390. Patton CJ (1992). Fear of abandonment and binge eating: A subliminal psychodynamic activation investigation. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 180, 484-490. Pendleton-Jones B, Duncan CC, Brouwers P, & Mirsky AF (1991). Cognition in eating disorders. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 13, 711-728. Perpina C, Hemsley Treasure J, & De Silva P (1993). Is the selective information processing of food and body words specific to patients with eating disorders? International Journal of Eating Disorders, 14, 359-366. personality. Psychol Rep., 84(2):689-96. Pia C, & Toro J (1999) Anorexia nervosa in a Spanish adolescent sample: An 8-year longitudinal study. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 100, 441-446. Pike KM, & Rodin J (1991). Mothers, daughters, and disordered eating. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100, 198-204. Piran N, Lerner P, Garfinkel P, Kennedy S & Brouillette C (1988) Personality disorders in anorexic patients. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 7, 589-599. Podar I, Hannus A, & Allik J (1999). Personality and affectivity characteristics associated with eating disorders: a comparison of eating disordered, weight-preoccupied, and normal samples. Journal of Personality Assessment, 73, 133-147. Polivy J, & Herman CP (1999). Distress and eating: why do dieters overeat? International Journal of Eating Disorders, 26, 153-164. Polivy J, & Herman CP (2000). The false-hope syndrome: Unfulfilled expectations of self-change. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 9, 128-131. Polivy J, & Herman CP (2002). Causes of eating disorders. Annual Review of Psychology, 53, 187-213. Pryor T, & Wiederman MW (1996). Measurement of nonclinical personality characteristics of women with anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa. Journal of Personality Assessment, 67, 414-421. Purdon C, Antony MM, & Swinson RP (1999). Psychometric properties of the Frost Multidimensional Perfectionism Scale in a clinical anxiety disorders sample. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 55, 1271-1286. Rachman S (1998). The nature of anxiety. In S. Rachman (Ed.), Anxiety (pp. 1-26). Hve, UK: Psychology Press. Rastam M (1992) Anorexia nervosa in 51 Swedish adolescents: Prernorbid problems and comorbid- ity. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 31, 819-829. Reichborn-Kjennerud T, Bulik CM, Tambs K, & Harris JR (2004). Genetic and Environmental influences on binge eating in the absence of compensatory behaviours: a population-based twin study. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 36, 307314. Rieger E, Schotte DE, Touyz SW, Beumont PJV, Griffiths R, & Russel J (1998). Attentional biases in eating disorders: A visual probe detection procedure. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 23, 199-205. Rogers PJ, & Green MW (1993). Dieting, dietary restraint and cognitive performance. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 32, 113-116. Root MPP, & Fallon P (1989). Treating the victimized bulimic. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 4, 90-100. Rorty M, & Yager J (1993). Why and how do women recover from bulimia nervosa? Int J Eat Disord, 14:249-260. Rorty M, Yager J, Buckwalter JG, Rossotto E & Guthrie D (2000a) Development and validation of the Parental Intrusiveness Rating Scale among bulimic and comparison women. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 28, 188-201. Rorty M, Yager J, Rossotto E & Buckwalter G (2000b) Parental intrusiveness in adolescence recalled by women with a history of bulimia nervosa and comparison women. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 28, 202-208. Rosen JC (1990). Body image disturbances in eating disorders. In T.F. Cash, & T. Pruzinsky (Eds.), Body images. Development, deviance and change (pp.190-214). New York: The Guilford Press. Rosen JC, & Ramirez T (1998). A comparison of eating disorders and body dysmorphic disorder on body image and psychological adjustment. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 44, 441-449. Rosen JC, Tracy B, & Howell D (1990). Life stress, psychological symptoms and weight reducing behavior in adolescent girls: A prospective analysis. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 9, 17-26. Rosenvinge JH, Martinussen M, & Ostensen E (2000). The comorbidity of eating disorders and personality disorders: A meta analytical review of studies published between 1983 and 1998. Eating and Weight Disorders, 5, 51-62. Ruggiero GM, Levi D, Ciuna A, & Sassaroli S (2003). Stress situation reveals an association between perfectionism and drive for thinness. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 34, 220-226. Sanftner JL, Barlow DH, Marschall DE & Tangney JP (1995). The relation of shame and guilt to eating disorder symptomatology. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 14, 315-324. Sanftner JL, & Crowther JH (1998) Variability in self-esteem, moods, shame and guilt in women who binge. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 23, 391-397. Sassaroli S, & Ruggiero GM (2002). I costrutti dell’ansia: obbligo di controllo, perfezionismo patologico, pensiero catastrofico, autovalutazione negative e intolleranza dell’ incertezza. (The constructs of anxiety: need for control, perfectionism, catastrophic thought, negative self-evaluation, and intolerance of uncertainty) Psicoterapia Cognitiva e Comprtamentale, 8, 45-60. Saxena S, Brody AL, Schwartz JM, & Baxter LR (1998). Neuroimaging and frontal-subcortical circuitry in obsessivecompulsive disorder. British Journal of Psychiatry, 173, 26-37. Schmidt U, Tiller J, Treasure J (1993b). Self-treatment of bulimia nervosa: a pilot study. International Journal of Eating Disorder, 13:273-277. Schmidt UH, Humfress H & Treasure JL (1997a) The role of general family environment and sexual and physical abuse in the origins of eating disorders. European Eating Disorders Review, 5,184-207. Schmidt UH, Jiwany A & Treasure JL (1993b) A controlled study of alexithymia in eating disorders. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 34, 54-58. Schmidt UH, Tiller JM, Andrews B, Blanchard M & Treasure JL (1997b) Is there a specific trauma precipitating onset of anorexia nervosa? Psychological Medicine, 27, 523-530. Schmidt UH, Tiller JM & Treasure JL (1993c) Setting the scene for eating disorders: Childhood care, classification and course of illness. Psychological Medicine, 23, 663-672. Schmidt UH, Troop NA & Treasure JL (1999) Stress and the onset of eating disorders: Correcting an "age-old" myth. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 25, 83-88. Serpell L, Livingstone A, Lask B & Neiderman M (2002) Anorexia nervosa: Obsessive compulsive disorder, obsessive compulsive personality or neither. Clinical Psychology Review, 22, 647-669. Serpell L, Neiderman M, Bird E, Rashid Z, Roberts V & Lask B (submitted) The shape and weight-based self-esteem inventory (SAWBS) in Adolescent Girls with Eating Disorders and Adolescent Controls. Shafran R, Cooper Z, Fairburn CG (2002). Clinical perfectionism: a cognitive-behavioural analysis. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 40, 773-791. Shafran R, Fairburn CG, Robinson P & Lask B (2003). Body checking and its avoidance in eating disorders. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 35 (1), 93-101. Shaw HE, Stice E, & Springer DW (2004). Perfectionism, Body Dissatisfaction, and Self-esteem in Predicting bulimic symptomatology: lack of replication. International Journal of eating Disorders, 36, 41-47. Shepherd H & Ricciardelli LA (1998) Test of Stice's dual pathway model: Dietary restraint and negative affect as mediators of bulimic behaviour. Behaviour, Research and Therapy, 36, 345- 352. Sherry SB, Hewitt PL, Besser A, McGee BJ, & Flett GL (2004). Self-oriented and socially prescribed perfectionism in the Eating Disorder Inventory perfectionism subscale. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 35, 69-79. Siegle GJ, Granholm E, Ingram RE, & Matt GE (2001). Pupillary and reaction time measures of sustained processing of negative information in depression. Society of Biological Psychiatry, 49, 624-636. Skodol A, Oldham J, Hyler S, Kellman HD, Doidge N & Davies M (1993) Comorbidity of DSM-III-R eating disorders and personality disorders. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 14,403-416. Slade PD (1982) Towards a functional analysis of anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 21, 167-179. Slade PD, Dewey ME, Kiemle G & Newton T (1990) Update on SCANS: A screening instrument for identifying individuals at risk of developing an eating disorder. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 9, 583-584. Slochower JA (1983). Excessive eating: The role of emotions and environment. New York: Human Sciences Press. Slochower JA, Kaplan SP & Mann L (1981). The effects of life stress and weight on mood and eating. Appetite, 2, 115-125. Smeets MAM, Ingleby JD, Hoek HW, & Panhuysen GEM (1999). Body size perception in anorexia nervosa: A signal detection approach. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 46(5), 465-477. Smeets MAM, Smit F, Panhuysen GEM, & Ingleby JD (1998). Body size perception index: Benefits, pitfalls, ideas. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 44, 457-464. Sohlberg S, & Strober M (1994). Personality in anorexia nervosa: An update and a theoretical integration. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 89 (suppl. 378), 1-15. Soukup VM, Beiler ME & Terrell F (1990) Stress, coping style and problem-solving ability among eating disordered inpatients. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 46, 592-599. Spoont MR (1992). Modulatory role of serotonin in neural information processing: Implications for human psychopathology. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 330-350. Srinivasagam NM, Kaye WH, Plotnikov KH, Greeno C, Weltzin TE & Rao R (1995) Persistent perfectionism, symmetry and exactness after long term recovery from anorexia nervosa. American Journal of Psychiatry, 152, 1630-1634. Steiger H, & Séguin JR (1999). Eating disorders : anorexia and bulimia nervosa. In: T. Millon, & P.H. Blaney (Eds.), Oxford textbook of psychopathology (pp. 365-389). Toronto: Oxford Univ. Press. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: Hillsdale, New York. Stern JA, & Dunham DN (1990). The ocular system. In J.T., Cacioppo, & LG Tassinary (Eds), Principles of psychophysiology: Physical, social and inferential elements (pp. 513-553). Cambridge: Cambridge University press. Stice E (1994) Review of the evidence for a socio-cultural model of bulimia nervosa and an exploration of the mechanisms of action. Clinical Psychology Review, 14, 633-661. Stice E (2001). A prospective test of the dual-pathway model of bulimic pathology: mediating effects of dieting and negative affect. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 110, 124-135. Stice E (2002a). Risk and maintenance factors for eating pathology: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 128 (5), 825848. Stice E (2002b). Sociocultural influences on body image and eating disturbance. In C.G. Fairburn & K.D. Brownell, Eating disorders and obesity. Second edition. A comprehensive handbook (pp. 103-107). New York: Guilford Press. Stice E, Akutagawa D, Gaggar A & Agras S (2000) Negative affect moderates the relation between dieting and binge eating. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 27, 218-229. Stice E, Killen JD, Hayward C, & Taylor CB (1998). Support for the continuity hypothesis of bulimic pathology. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 66, 784-790. Stice E, & Shaw HE (2002). Role of body dissatisfaction in the onset and maintenance of eating pathology: a synthesis of research findings. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 53 (5), 961-962. Strauss J, & Ryan RM (1988). Cognitive dysfunction in eating disorders. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 7, 19-27. Strober M (1980). Personality and symptomatological features in young, nonchronic anorexia nervosa patients. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 24, 353-359 Strober M (1984). Stressful life events associated with bulimia in anorexia nervosa. Empirical findings and theoretical speculations. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 3, 3-16. Strober M (1991). Disorders of the self in anorexia nervosa: An organismic-developmental paradigm. In Psychodynamic treatment of anorexia nervosa and bulimia (ed. C. Johnson), pp. 354-373. Guilford Press: New York. Strober M (1995). Family-genetic perspectives on anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa. In Eating Disorders and Obesity (ed. Brownell, K.D. and Fairburn, C.G.), pp. 212-218. The Guilford Press: New York Strober M, Freeman R, Morrell W (1997). The long-term course of severe anorexia nervosa in ad-olescents: survival analysis of recovery, relapse, and outcome predictors over 10-15 years in a prospective study. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 22:339-360. Sutandar-Pinnock K, Woodside DB, Carter JC, Olmsted MP, & Kaplan AS (2003). Perfectionism in anorexia nervosa: a 6-24 month follow-up study. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 33, 225-229. Szmukler GI, Andrewes D, Kingston K, Chen L, Stargatt R, & Stanley R (1992). Neuropsychological impairment in anorexia nervosa: Before and after refeeding. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 14, 347-352. Tangney JP (1993) Shame and guilt. In C.G. Costello (Ed.), Symptoms of Depression (pp. 161-180). New York: John Wiley & Sons. Taylor GJ, Parker JDA, Badbey RM, & Bourke MP (1996). Relationships between alexithymia and psychological characteristics associated with eating disorders. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 41, 561-568. Tchanturia K, Happé F, Godley J, Treasure J, Bara-Carril N, & Schmidt U (2004). ‘Theory of Mind’ in Anorexia Nervosa. European Eating Diorders Review, 12, 361-366. Tchanturia K, Morris RG, Surguladze S & Treasure JL (in press) An examination of perceptual and cognitive set shifting tasks in acute anorexia and following recovery. Eating and Weight Disorders. Tchanturia K, Serpell L, Troop N, & Treasure J (2002) Perceptual illusions in eating disorders: Rigid and fluctuating styles. Behaviour Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 32, 107-115. Telch CF, & Stice E (1998). Psychiatric comorbidity in women with Binge Eating Disorder: Prevalence rates from a nontreatment-seeking sample. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 66 (5), 768-776. Thompson SBN (1993). Implications of neuropsychological test results of women in a new phase of anorexia nervosa. European Eating Disorders Review, 1, 152-165. Tiller JM, Schmidt UH, Ali S & Treasure JL (1995). Patterns of punitiveness in women with eating disorders. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 17, 365-371. Tiller JM, Sloane G, Schmidt UH, Troop NA, Power M & Treasure JL (1997). Social support in patients with anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 21,31-38. tional Journal of Eating Disorders, 17 (2), I 47~152. Treasure JL & Holland A (1995). Genetic factors in eating disorders. In G. Szmukler, C. Dare & J. Treasure (Eds), Handbook of Eating Disorders: Theory, Treatment and Research. Chichester: John Wiley & Sons. Troop NA & Bifulco A (2002). Childhood social arena and cognitive sets in eating disorders. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 41, 205-211. Troop NA, Holbrey A, & Treasure JL (1998). Stress, coping and crisis support in eating disorders. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 24, 157-166. Troop NA, Holbrey A, Trowler R & Treasure JL (1994) Ways of coping in women with eating disorders. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 182, 535-540. Troop NA, Schmidt UH, & Treasure J (1995). Feelings and fantasy in eating disorders: A factor analysis of the Toronto Alexitymia Scale. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 18, 151-157. Troop NA, Schmidt UH, Turnbull S, & Treasure JL (2000) Self-esteem and responsibility for change in women with bulimia nervosa. European Eating Disorders Review, 8, 384-393. Troop NA, & Treasure JL (1997b). Setting the scene for eating disorders II: Childhood helplessness and mastery. Psychological Medicine, 27,531-538. Troop NA, & Treasure JL (1997a) Psychosocial factors in the onset of eating disorders: Responses to life events and difficulties. British Journal of Medical Psychology, 70, 373-385. Turner H, & Cooper M (2002). Cognitions and their origins in women with anorexia nervosa, normal dieters and female controls. Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, 9(4), 242-252. Tuschen-Caffier B, Vögele C, Bracht S, & Hilbert A (2003). Psychological responses to body shape exposure in patients with bulimia nervosa. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 41, 573-586. Tylka TL, & Subich LM (1999). Exploring the construct validity of the eating disorder continuum. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 46, 268-276. Uher R, Murphy T, Phillips M, & Dalgleish TT (2001). Perception of food and emotional stimuli in anorexia nervosa. Neuroimage, 13, S1022-S1022. Vandereycken W, & Noordenbos G (2002). Handboek eetstoornissen, De Tijdstroom, Utrecht. Van Hanswijck, De Jonge P, Van Furth EF, Lacey JH, & Waller G (2003). The prevalence of DSM-IV personality pathology among individuals with bulimia nervosa, binge eating disorder and obesity. Psychological Medicine, 33, 1311-1317. Van Hoeken D, Seidell J, & Hoek HW (2003). Epidemiology. In: J. Treasure, U. Schmidt, & E. van Furth (Eds) Handbook of Eating Disorders. (Second Edition),( pp. 11-35). Chichester, Wiley. Van Strien T (1986). Eating behaviour, personality traits and body mass. Swets & Zeitlinger B.V.: Lisse Van Strien T (2000a). Ice-Cream consumption, tendency toward overeating, and personality. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 28, 460-464. Van Strien T, Cleven A, & Schippers G (2000). Restraint, Tendency Toward Overeating and Ice Cream Consumption. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 28, 333-338. Viken RJ, Treat TA, Nosofsky R.M, McFall RM, & Palmeri TJ (2002). Modeling individual differences in perceptual and attentional processes related to bulimic symptoms. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 111, 598-609. Vitousek K, & Hollon SS (1990). The investigation of schematic content and processing in eating disorders. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 14, 191-214. Vitousek K, & Manke F (1994). Personality variables and disorders in anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103, 137-147. Vohs KD, Bardone AM, Joiner TE, Abramson LY, & Heatherton TF (1999). Perfectionism, perceived weight status, and self-esteem interact to predict bulimic symptoms: A model of bulimic symptom development. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 108, 695-700. Vohs KD, Voelz ZR, Pettit JW, Bardone AM, Katz J, Abramson LY, Heatherton TF, & Joiner TE (2001). Perfectionism, body dissatisfaction, and self-esteem: An interactive model of bulimic symptom development. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 20, 476-497. Wade TD, Bulik CM, Neale M, Kendler KS. (2000). Anorexia nervosa and major depression: shared genetic and environmental risk factors. American Journal of Psychiatry (3), 469-71. Waller G (2003). Schema-level cognitions in patients with binge eating disorder: a case control study. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 33, 458-464. Waller G, Babbs M, Milligan R, Meyer C, Ohanian V, & Leung N (2003). Anger and Core beliefs in the Eating Disorders. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 34, 118-124. Waller G, Mijatovich S (1998). Precounscious processing of threat cues: impact on eating among women with unhealthy eating attitudes. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 24, 83-89. Waller G, Ohanian V, Meyer C & Osman S (2000) Cognitive content among bulimic women: role of core beliefs. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 28, 235-241. Waller G, Watkins H, Shuck V, & McManus F (1996). Bulimic psychopathology and attentional biases to ego threats among non-eating-disordered women. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 20, 169-176. Walters EE, Neale MC, Eaves LJ, Lindon J, & Heath AC (1992). Bulimia nervosa and major depression: A study of common genetic and environmental factors. Psychological Medicine, 22, 617-622. Wassenaar D, le Grange D, Winship J, et al (2000). The prevalence of eating disorder pathology in a cross-ethnic population of female students in South Africa. European Eating Disorders Review, 8, 225-236. Webber EM (1994). Psychological characteristics of bingeing and non-bingeing obese woman. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 66, 768-776. Wells A, & Davies MI (1994). The thought control questionnaire: A measure of indvidual differences in the control of unwanted thoughts. Behaviour Research an Therapy, 32, 871-878. Wentz E, Gillberg C, Gillberg IC, & Rastam M (2001). Ten-year follow-up of adolescent-onset anorexia nervosa: Psychiatric Disorders and overall function scales. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 42, 613-622. Whiteside SP, & Lynam Dr (2001). The Five Factor Model and impulsivity: Using a structural model of personality to understand impulsivity. Personality and Individual Differences, 30, 669-689. Wiederman MW, & Pryor T (1996). Multi-impulsivity in bulimia nervosa. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 20 (4), 359-365. Wilfley DE, Schwartz MB, Spurrell EB, Fairburn CG (2000). EDI to identify the specific psychopathology of BED. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 27, 259-269. Williams JMG, Watts FN, Mac Leod C, & Mathews A (1997). Cognitive psychology and emotional disorders (2nd ed.). Chichester: Wiley. Wilson GT, Fairburn CG, & Agras WS (1997). Cognitive-behavioral therapy for bulimia nervosa. In D.M. Garner & P.E. Garfinkel, Handbook of treatment for eating disorders. Second edition (pp. 67-93). New York: Guilford express. Wonderlich SA, Connolly KM, & Stice E (2004). Impulsivity as a risk factor for eating disorder behaviour: Assessment implications with adolescents. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 36, 172-182. Wonderlich SA, Mitchell JE (1997). Eating disorders and comorbidity: empirical, conceptual and clinical implications. Psychopharmacol Bull, 33:381-390. Wonderlich SA, Swift WJ, Slotnick HB & Goodman S (1990) DSM-III-R personality disorders in eating disorders subtypes. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 9, 607-616. Wood A, Waller G, & Gowers S (1994). Predictors of eating psychopathology in adolescent girls. European Eating Disorders Review, 2, 6-13. World Health Organization (1992, 1993). The ICD-10 classification of mental and behavioral disorders. Clinical descriptions and diagnostic guidelines. Genéve: Author. Wyatt R, & Gilbert P (1998). Dimensions of perfectionism: a study exploring their relationship with perceived social rank and status. Personality and Individual Differences, 24, 71-79. Yager J, Rorty M. & Rossotto E. (1995). Coping styles differ between recovered and nonrecovered 1en with bulimia nervosa but not between recovered women and non-eating disordered control subjects. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 183, 86-94. Young JE (1994). Cognitive therapy for personality disorders: A schema-focused approach (2nd ed.), Sarasota, FL: Professional Resource Press. PART II The cognitive psychology of eating disorders: personality, cognitions and behaviour. Chapter 2: Personality characteristics as trait-dependent vulnerability: study results 1. Binge Eating Disorder and Non Purging Bulimia: more similar than different?4 There is evidence of an overlap of symptoms in binge-eating disordered (BED) and bulimia nervosa non-purging (BN-NP) patients. In addition, indications of an evolution from bulimia nervosa to BED along a continuum of vulnerability have been found. However, DSM-IV categorizes BED and BN-NP as distinct disorders based on clinical characteristics. In this study weight history and personality-related characteristics (TCI; Cloninger, Svrakic, & Przybeck, 1993) were studied in 30 BED and 17 BN-NP patients. BED patients were older, and had a longer duration of illness, a larger weight cycling, a higher current and previous BMI and a lower score for the temperament dimension ‘Persistence’ than BN-NP patients. After correcting for age, differences between weight variables remained, including comparatively higher BMIs and larger weight fluctuations among the BED patients. A possible explanation is that this difference is due to a genetic effect of the propensity to be overweight and the temperamental characteristic of Persistence. This ‘morbid’ starting position and the lack of controlling mechanisms are associated with a more trait- than state-dependent condition. Thus, restrained eating is not to be regarded as eliciting bingeing in BED patients. Due to personality characteristics these patients are not able to restrain, which clearly differentiates them from BN-NP patients. Keywords: binge eating disorder, personality, bulimia nervosa non-purging type Introduction Patients diagnosed with Eating Disorders Not Otherwise Specified (EDNOS) are defined as individuals who have an eating disorder of clinical severity but who do not meet diagnostic criteria for anorexia nervosa or bulimia nervosa. Several studies (Mitchell, Pyle, Hatsukami, & Eckert, 1986; Mitrany, 1992; Norvell & Cooley, 1986-1987, Mizes & Sloan, 1998; Spitzer et al., 1992; Williamson, Gleaves, & Savin, 1992), which were mostly descriptive and based on relatively small sample sizes, have suggested that patients suffering from binge eating disorder (BED) form a subgroup within this EDNOS group. An important issue in the discussion of whether BED should be regarded as a diagnostic entity, is the question whether patients with bulimia nervosa without purging (BN-NP) differ from BED patients. Based on results from a community sample, Striegel-Moore and collegues suggested that BED does not represent a burned out form of BN (any type; Striegel-Moore et al., 2001). 4 Vervaet, M., van Heeringen, C., Audenaert, K. (2004). European Eating Disorders Review, 12, 27-33 DSM-IV criteria for BED imply the absence of inappropriate compensatory behaviours in BED patients. BN-NP patients binge at least twice a week without compensatory behaviours, except for fasting and excessive exercise. They appear to form a homogeneous group sharing however some clinical characteristics with purging (BN-P) bulimics (Garfinkel et al., 1995; Kinzl, Traweger, Trefalt, Mangweth, & Biebl, 1999) including age, gender, a similar determination to compensate for their bingeing, and similar pathways to binge-eating. The main behavioural difference between non-purging and purging bulimics is the frequently self-induced vomiting and/or misuse of laxatives and other drugs to counteract the effects of binge-eating among the latter (Kinzl et al., 1999). In BN, the onset of binge eating is very often preceded by dieting and weight loss (Mussell et al., 1997), whereas among patients with BED, binge eating may be the cause, and not the consequence, of dieting behaviour (Marcus, 1995). However, some cases of bulimia nervosa may evolve into BED, and accumulating evidence indicates the existence of a continuum of vulnerability with a higher frequency of binge-eating reflecting greater clinical severity among obese and bulimia nervosa patients (de Zwaan et al., 1994; Garfinkel et al., 1995; Kendler et al., 1991; Striegel-Moore, Wilson, Wilfley, Elder, & Brownell, 1998). There is, however, less evidence of an association between BMI and severity of binge-eating. Some studies, but not all (Brody, Walsh, & Devlin, 1995; de Zwaan, Nutzinger, & Schoenbeck, 1992; Goldfein, Walsh, LaChausse, Kissileff, & Devlin, 1993; Wilson, Nonas, & Rosenblum, 1993), have reported an association (de Zwaan et al., 1994; Lowe & Caputo, 1991; Spitzer et al., 1993; Telch, Agras, & Rossiter, 1988). Clinical observation and studies of personality characteristics and comorbidity indicate that BED patients show more severe psychopathology than pure restricting anorexics. Moreover, their family environment appears to be more similar to that of bulimic and anorectic purger/binger patients in terms of conflict and chaos (Kanter, Williams, & Cummings, 1992; Marcus et al., 1990). Thus, there is evidence of an overlap of symptoms in BED and BN-NP patients, and, in addition, indications of an evolution from BN to BED along a continuum of vulnerability have been found. However, DSM-IV categorizes BED and BN-NP as distinct disorders based on clinical characteristics. This study aimed at investigating differences in personality-related characteristics between BED and BN-NP patients. Methods Sample A total of 409 patients who were consecutively referred to the in- and out-patient units of the Centre for Eating Disorders at the Department of Psychiatry of the University Hospital Gent between December 1998 and April 2002 and who met the DSM-IV criteria for eating disorders, were enrolled in the study. The study was compliant with the Code of Ethics of the World Medical Association (Declaration of Helsinki) and carried out following the guidelines of the local ethics committee. The referred group included 17 patients (4 per cent) with BN-NP, while 75 patients (18 per cent) were diagnosed with EDNOS. In the BN-NP group, which consisted of 16 females and 1 male, the mean age was 23.8 years (SD=7.2 years) and the mean BMI was 24.5 (SD=4.2). Among the EDNOS patients, the mean age was 26.2 years (SD = 10.9 years) and the mean BMI was 26.7 (SD = 10.1). Ninety-eight per cent (n = 72) of the EDNOS patients were female. Thirty-one patients of the EDNOS group met the criteria for BED (female: 28; male: three). The data on one female of the BED group were excluded from statistical analysis because of mental retardation which could have influenced the reliability of the assessment. The mean age of BED patients was 33.1 years (SD = 10.7 years) with a mean BMI of 37.7 (SD = 6.7). Measures Weight and height, highest and lowest adult weight ever and duration of illness were assessed carefully. The body mass index (BMI) was calculated, as well the highest and lowest BMI (HBMI, LBMI), based on the highest and lowest adult weight ever. Weight fluctuation was defined by the difference between highest and lowest adult weight ever. Patients were asked to describe the frequency of their binging, purging and restricting behaviours according to three categories: never, less than twice per week (‘sometimes’), or more than twice per week (‘frequent’). Bingeing was described as the loss of control over eating, not including any measurement of calories. The Dutch version of the SCAN (Schedules for Clinical Assessment in Neuropsychiatry) was used to assign diagnoses according to DSM-IV criteria (Giel & Nienhuis, 1996). The validated Dutch version of the TCI (Duijsens, Spinhoven, Goekoop, Spermon, & Eurelings-Bontekoe, 2000) was used to assess temperament and character characteristics. The TCI is based on a psychobiological model of personality and assesses both temperament and character. Temperamental aspects of personality are (at least partially) heritable, manifest early in life, and involve preconceptual or unconscious biases in learning (Cloninger et al., 1993). The temperament dimensions include novelty seeking (NS) reflecting behavioural activation, harm avoidance (HA), a tendency toward behavioural inhibition, reward dependence (RD) reflecting behavioural attachment, and persistence (PS), a measure of behavioural maintenance. Character aspects of personality are denoted by individual differences in self-concepts, goals, and values. The character dimensions include self- directedness (SD), referring to a self-accepting, purposeful nature, cooperativeness (CO), a trait associated with acceptance of other people and compassion, and self-transcendence (ST) reflecting spirituality and an acceptance, identification, or spiritual union with nature and its source. Individuals with the same temperament may behave differently as a result of differences in character development (Cloninger et al., 1993). In addition to the self-report measurements, eating behaviours were assessed by means of the Dutch Eating Behaviour Questionnaire (DEBQ; Van Strien, 1986). This questionnaire measures the behavioural characteristics emotional eating (eating elicited by diffuse and clear emotional stimuli), restrained eating (the tendency to diet in order to lose weight), and external eating (eating elicited by external stimuli). Finally, patients were asked for feelings of sadness, sleeping disturbances (without drugs), feelings of anxiety or phobia or panic attacks, feelings of depression, a history of deliberate self-harm, and drug abuse. Obesity or overweight in first degree family members was assessed. Statistical analysis was performed using SPSS 10.0. t-Tests were used to compare BN-NP with BED patients with regard to demographic, clinical and behavioural (DEBQ) and personality (TCI) characteristics. Since a significant difference for age between the two patient groups was found, a correction for age was applied in further statistical analysis. Results The study group included 17 BN-NP and 30 BED patients who all binged at least twice per week. Since purging is an exclusion criteria for the BN-NP and BED diagnoses, patients were included only if they vomited or used laxatives less than twice per week. The BN-NP group included one vomiting patient, and one patient who used laxatives, while in the BED group eight patients reported vomiting and four used laxatives, all less than twice a week. BN-NP patients reported food restriction very commonly (more than twice per week, n=13), while BED patients tried to restrict, but failed (five patients reported restricting more than twice per week). Since excessive exercise was assessed only in the last 14 patients, only differences in restricting were calculated. As shown in Table 1 there are thus small numbers in some cells. Consequently, data on excessive exercise were not included in further analysis. Table 1: Significant differences for age, duration of illness, BMI, HBMI, LBMI and weightfluctuation characteristics between BN-NP and BED patients BN-NP n=17 BED n=30 total Never Sometimes Frequently 0 3 14 10 15 5 10 18 19 Excessive exercise Missing Never 1 1 3 Frequently 13 6 20 7 33 2 1 3 Restricting Sometimes 4 Sometimes, < twice a week; Frequently, >twice a week, Pearson chi-square = 0.000 As shown in Table 2, BN-NP subjects were younger, and had a shorter duration of illness, when compared to BED patients. Table 2: Table 2: Significant differences for age, duration of illness, BMI, HBMI, LBMI, weight-fluctuation, behavioural (DEBQ) and personality characteristics (TCI) characteristics between BN-NP and BED patients Age Duration of illness BMI HBMI LBMI Weight fluctuation Restrained Eating Persistence BN-NP n=17 m sd 23.8 7.2 4.1 3.5 24.5 4.2 27.1 5.5 19.3 4.1 21.7 12.6 4.0 0.8 5.4 2.1 BED n=30 m sd 33.1 10.7 11.4 7.5 37.7 6.7 39.7 7.6 25.4 3.7 40.6 22.1 2.9 0.6 3.4 2.1 t p -3.2 .002 -3.7 .001 -7.3 .000 -5.9 .000 -5,3 .000 -3.2 .002 4.8 .000 3.1 .004 The group of BN-NP patients had a mean lower BMI, adult HBMI and LBMI than BED patients. BED patients showed significantly larger fluctuations in their body weight than patients of the BN-NP group. Patients in the BN-NP group also had a higher score on ‘restrained eating’, than patients in the BED group. With regard to personality dimensions, BN-NP patients scored higher on persistence than BED patients. After correction for age (Table 3), the described differences remained significant. Table 3: Differences between BN-NP and BED after correction for age BMI HBM LBMI Weight fluctuation Restrained eating persistence BN-NP M SE 26.2 1.4 29.1 1.6 19.1 1.0 27.6 4.4 BED M SE 36.7 1.0 38.5 1.2 25.5 0.7 37.1 3.2 R-squared 3.9 0.2 5.0 0.5 2.9 0.1 3.7 0.4 F p-value (Bonferroni) .640 39.1 .000 .561 28.1 .000 .392 14.2 .000 .385 13.8 .000 .371 12.9 .000 .211 5.9 .006 ANOVA with Bonferroni post hoc comparison; corrected for age, evaluated at covariates appeared in the model: AGE = 29.72 There was a significant difference (Pearson chi-square = 0.007) in overweight or obesity in first degree family members. Twenty-two of the BED patients (76 per cent) and seven of the 17 BN-NP patients (47 per cent) reported that their mother or father was overweight. No differences were found with regard to feelings of sadness, anxiety, depression, or the presence of sleep disturbances, deliberate self-harm or drug abuse (Table 4). Table 4: Clinical differences between BN-NP and BED patients ------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------- BN-NP Sensitivity, SleepFeelings Feelings of Deliberate Drug Overweight tension, disturbances of Depression Self-harm abuse or obesity sadness anxiety in family 14 7 5 13 3 0 7 N=17 BED N=30 MannWhitney U Asymp.Sig. (twotailed) 27 19 13 24 9 4 22 217.0 176.5 192.5 209.0 184.5 180.0 161 .977 .233 .467 .740 .403 .129 .020 Finally, three different groups were compared according to the frequency of restricting food (Table 5). Table 5. frequency of non-purging compensating behaviour in BN-NP and BED patients Age BMI HBM LBMI Duration of illness Restrained eating persistence Never Restricting M (SD) (1) 35.5 (9.9) Sometimes Restricting M (SD) (2) 27.8 (9.9) Frequently Restricting M (SD) (3) F p-value (Bonferroni) 28.0 (10.9) 2.1 0.131 38.3 (5.1) 39.8 (6.1) 25.7 (3.6) 13.2 (8.0) 33.5 (8.0) 34.9 (8.6) 23.4 (7.1) 8.3 (5.9) 29.3 (9.7) 32.5 (10.6) 20.2 (4.5) 7.4 (7.5) 4.0 2.1 3.3 2.0 0.026 0.137 0.048 0.148 3<1 2.9 (0.8) 3.1 (0.6) 3.7 (0.9) 3.7 0.034 1,2<3 3.3 (1.5) 3.5 (2.5) 5.4 (2.0) 4.6 0.015 ANOVA with Bonferroni post hoc comparison 1,2<3 3<1 Significant differences between the groups were found for BMI, LBMI and the personality characteristic, persistence. Patients with frequent restricting behaviour had a mean lower BMI and LBMI ever compared to patients who never restricted. The mean score for persistence was higher among the group of frequently-restricting patients compared to the two other groups. There was a significant association between restricting behaviour, when dichotomized to present or absent, and persistence. Discussion This study shows that BED patients differ from BN-NP patients in being older and having a longer duration of illness. The BED group has a mean BMI reflecting the obesity of these patients, in contrast with the BN-NP patients, whose mean BMI can be described as reflecting overweight. The finding that the lowest BMI ever among the BED patients was higher than the ideal level is noteworthy, as this indicates overweight already during puberty. In contrast, the BN-NP patients, reported a highest BMI ever indicating overweight, but these patients apparently succeed in becoming only slightly overweight, although they binged. A possible explanation is that in BN patients, the onset of binge eating is very often preceded by dieting and weight loss (Mussell et al., 1997), whereas among BED patients binge-eating can be the cause, and not the consequence, of dieting behaviour (Marcus, 1995). Moreover, BED patients have learned that dieting has little or no effect since they still remain overweight and most of them have a history of yo-yo dieting. Comparing their weight cycling with the BN-NP patients, there was a significant difference. As previously described by Williamson and colleagues (1992), the average score for ‘restrained eating’ was significantly lower in the BED group when compared to the BN-NP group. As expected, there was no difference in ‘emotional eating’ or ‘external eating’ between the two groups. Noteworthy is the significant difference in ‘persistence’ between the study groups. A strong association between persistence and restricting eating behaviour, as a manifestation of non-purging compensating behaviour was found and significantly more restricting behaviour was found in the BN-NP group. Age has been shown to have an effect on the character dimensions self-directedness and cooperativeness (Cloninger et al., 1993), which develop by means of maturation. With regard to the current findings, however, differences in personality characteristics between the patient groups were limited to the temperament dimension persistence, so that an effect of age is unlikely. Given the possibility, that overweight and persistence are, at the least partially, genetically determined, overweight youngsters with a rather high novelty seeking (compared to anorectic patients) may binge due to a lack of control following exposure to external and emotional stimuli and may fail to compensate, by restrained eating, due to a lack of persistence. Although BED patients differ from BN-NP patients in a number of characteristics, there are also some important shared features, indicating that BED is not merely a body weight-related problem. Previous studies have shown a higher food intake even on non-binge days in BED patients compared to obese non-bingers (Yanovski & Sebring, 1994). A history of more frequent weight fluctuations and a greater amount of time spent dieting was shown in BED patients (Brody, Walsh, & Devlin, 1995; Yanovski, 1993). Moreover, obese BED patients show more psychiatric comorbidity and psychopathology (Striegel-Moore et al., 1998; Telch & Agras, 1995; Yanovski, Nelson, Dubbert, & Spitzer, 1993), and lower self-esteem (de Zwaan et al., 1994; Hawkins & Clement, 1980; Lowe & Caputo, 1991, Striegel-Moore et al., 1998). Striegel-Moore and colleagues (1998) even showed a lower selfesteem in BED patients than in subthreshold BED patients and overeaters. Previous research has shown higher levels of thrill-seeking and excitability, affective instability and impulsivity, and a tendency towards marked dysphoria in response to rejection or non-reward in bulimic patients (Bulik, Beidel, Duchmann, Weltzin, & Kaye, 1992; Casper, Hedeker, & McKlough, 1992; Strober, 1995; Vitousek & Manke, 1994) which may predispose an individual to periodic loss of control and eventual dietary chaos. Increased harm avoidance may be involved in the development of both anorexia and bulimia nervosa, and persist, as a trait, following recovery from these disorders (Casper, 1990; Kaye et al., 1998; Kaye, Strober, Stein, & Gendall, 1999; Srinivasagam et al., 1995). As this study showed no differences between the two groups in scores on the temperament dimensions novelty seeking, harm avoidance and reward dependence, it can be hypothesized that BED patients (and BN-NP patients as well) differ from obese non-bingeing patients. This hypothesis is supported by the absence of a difference in the clinical measures of feelings of sadness, sleeping disturbances (without drugs), feelings of anxiety or phobia or panic attacks, feelings of depression, a history of deliberate selfharm, and drug abuse between the BN-NP and BED patients in this study. The conclusion of this study is that BED patients share many behavioural and personality characteristics with BN-NP patients. However, BED patients are older, and have a longer duration of illness, greater weight cycling and a higher current and previous BMI than BN-NP patients. After correcting for age, differences between restrained eating and weight variables remain together with less restrained eating, higher BMIs and larger weight fluctuations among the BED patients. A possible explanation is that this difference is due to a genetic effect of the propensity to be overweight and the temperamental characteristic, persistence. This ‘morbid’ starting position and lack of controlling mechanisms are associated with a more trait- than state-dependent condition. Thus, restrained eating is not to be regarded as eliciting bingeing in BED patients. Due to personality characteristics these patients are not able to restrain, which clearly differentiates them from BN-NP patients. This could be an important difference between the BN-NP and BED group and may support a categorical diagnostic distinction. Further research on larger samples is clearly needed to confirm these findings. Moreover, since only a small proportion of BED and BN-NP suffers come along to services, there is room for doubt about extrapolation from clinic populations to the generality of those with the disorders. References Brody, M.L., Walsh, B.T., & Devlin, M.Y. (1995). Binge eating disorder: Reliability and validity of a new diagnostic categrory. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 62, 381-386. Bulik, C.M., Beidel, D.C., Duchmann, E., Weltzin, T.E., & Kaye, W.H. (1992). Comparative psychopathology of women with bulimia nervosa and obsessive-compulsive disorder. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 33, 262-268. Casper, R.C. (1990). Personality features of women with good outcome from restricting anorexia nervosa. Psychosomatic Medecine, 52, 156-170. Casper, R.C., Hedeker, D., & McKlough, J.F. (1992). Personality dimensions in eating disorders and their relevance for subtyping. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 31, 830-840. Cloninger, C., Svrakic, D., & Przybeck, T. (1993). A psychobiological model of temperament and character. Archives of General Psychiatry, 30, 975-990. de Zwaan, M., Mitchell, J.E., Seim, H.C., Specker, S.M., Pyle, R.L., Raymond, N.C., & Crosby, R.B. (1994). Eating related and general psychopathology in obese females with binge eating disorder. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 15, 4352. de Zwaan, M., Nutzinger,D.O., & Schoenbeck,,G. (1992). Binge eating in overweight women. Comprehensive Psychiatr, 33, 256-261. Duijsens, I.J., Spinhoven, P., Goekoop, J.G., Spermon, T., & Eurelings-Bontekoe, E.H.M. (2000). The Dutch temperament and character inventory (TCI): dimensional structure, reliability and validity in a normal and psychiatric outpatient sample. Personality and Individual Differences, 28, 487-499. Garfinkel, P.E., Lin, E., Goering, P., Spegg, C., Goldbloom, D.S., Kennedy, S., Kaplan, A.S., & Woodside, D.B. (1995). Bulimia nervosa in a Canadian community sample: Prevalence and comparison of subgroups. American Journal of Psychiatry, 152, 1052-1058. Giel, R., & Nienhuis, F.J. (1996). Vragenschema’s voor de klinische beoordeling in de neuropsychiatrie. World Health Organisation Mental Health, Geneva. Goldfein, J.A., Walsh, B.T., LaChausse, J.L., Kissileff, H.R., & Devlin, M,Y. (1993). Eating behavior in binge eating disorders. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 14, 427-431. Hawkins, R.C., & Clement, P.F. (1980). Development and construct validation of a self-report measure of binge eating tendencies. Addictive Behaviors, 5, 219-226. Kanter, R., Williams, B., & Cummings, C. (1992). Personal and parental alcohol abuse, and victimization in obese binge eaters and nonbingeing obese. Addictive Behaviors, 17, 439-445. Kaye, W.H., Greeno, C.G., Moss, H., Fernstrom, J., Fernstrom, M., Lilenfeld, L.R., Weltzin, T.E.., & Mann, J.J. (1998). Alterations in serotonin activity and psychiatric symptomatology after recovery from bulimia nervosa. Archives of General Psychiatry, 55, 927-935. Kaye, W.H., Strober, M., Stein, D., & Gendall, K. (1999). New Directions in Treatment Research of Anorexia and Bulimia Nervosa. Biological Psychiatry, 45, 1285-1292. Kendler, K.S., MacLean, C., Neale, M., Kessler, R., Heath, A., & Eaves, L. (1991). The genetic epidemiology of bulimia nervosa. American Journal of Psychiatry, 148, 1627-1637. Kinzl, J.F., Traweger, C., Trefalt, E., Mangweth, B., Biebl, W. (1999). Binge eating Disorder in Females: a population-based investigation. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 25, 287-292. Lowe, M.R., & Caputo, G.C. (1991). Binge eating in obesity: Toward the specification of predictors. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 10, 49-55. Marcus, M., Wing, R., Ewing, L., Kern, E., Gooding, W. & McDermott, M. (1990). Psychiatric disorders among obese binge eaters. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 9, 69-77. Marcus, M.D. (1995). Binge eating and obesity. In K.D. Brownell & C.G. Fairburn (Eds.), Eating disorders and obesity. A comprehensive handbook (pp. 441-444). New York: Guilford Press. Mitchell, J.E., Pyle, R.L., Hatsukami, D., & Eckert, E.D. (1986). What are atypical eating disorders? Psychosomatics, 27, 2128. Mitrany, E. (1992). Atypical eating disorders. Journal of Adolescent Health, 13, 400-402. Mizes, J.S., Sloan, D.M. (1998). An empirical analysis of Eating Disorder, Not Otherwise Specified: preliminary support for a distinct subgroup. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 23, 233-242. Mussell, M.P., Mitchell, J.E., Fenna, C.J., Crosby, R,D., Miller, J.P., & Hoberman, H.M. (1997). A comparison of onset of binge eating versus dieting in the development of bulimia nervosa. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 21, 353-360. Norvell, N., & Cooley, E. (1986-1987). Diagnostic issues in eating disorders: Two cases of atypical eating disorder. International Journal of Psychiatry Medicine, 16, 317-323. Spitzer, R.L., Devlin, M.J., Walsh, B.T., Hasin, D., Wing, R., Marcus, M., Stunkard, A., Wadden, T., Yanovski, S., Agras, S., Mitchell, J., & Nonas, C. (1992). Binge eating disorder: A multisided trial of the diagnostic criteria. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 11, 191-203. Spitzer, R.L., Yanovski, S., Wadden, T., Wing, R., Marcus, M., Stunkard, A., Devlin, M.J., Mitchell, J., Hasin, D., & Horne, R.L. (1993). Binge eating disorder: Its further validation in a multiside study. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 13, 137-153. Srinivasagan, N.M., Plotnicov, K.H., Greeno, C., Weltzin, T.E., & Rao, R. (1995). Persistent perfectionism, symmetry, and exactness in anorexia nervosa after long-term recovery. American Journal of Psychiatry, 152, 1630-1634. Striegel-Moore, R.H., Wilson, G.T., Wilfley, D.E., Elder, K.A., Brownell, K.D. (1998). Binge eating in an obese community sample. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 23, 27-37. Strober, M. (1995). Family-genetic perspectives on anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa. In K.D Brownell, and C.G. Fairburn (Eds), Eating Disorders and Obesity, 212-218. New York: The Guilford Press. Telch, C.F., & Agras, W.S. (1995). Obesity, binge eating and psychopathology: Are they related? International Journal of Eating Disorders, 15, 53-62. Telch, C., Agras, W.S., & Rossiter, E.M. (1988). Binge eating increases with increasing adiposity. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 7, 115-119. Van Strien, T. (1986). Eating behaviour, personality traits and body mass. Swets & Zeitlinger B.V.: Lisse. Vitousek K., Manke F. 1994. Personality variables and disorders in anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 103 (1), 137-47. Williamson, D.A., Gleaves, D.H., & Savin, S.S. (1992). Empirical classification of Eating disorder, Not Otherwise Specified: Support for DSM-IV changes. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment , 14, 201-216. Wilson, G.T., Nonas, C., & Rosenblum, G,D. (1993). Assessment of binge-eating in obese patients. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 13, 25-34. Yanovski, S.Z. (1993). Binge eating disorder: Current knowledge and future directions. Obesity Research, 1, 306-324. Yanovski, S.Z., & Sebring, N.G. (1994). Recorded food intake of obese women with binge eating disorders before and after weight loss. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 15, 135-150. Yanovski, S.Z., Nelson, J.E., Dubbert, B.K., & Spitzer, R.L. (1993). Association of binge eating disorder and psychiatric comorbidity in the obese. American Journal of Psychiatry, 150, 1472-1479. 2. Is Drive for Thinness in Anorectic Patients associated with Personality Chararteristics?5 Abstract Objective: The objective was to compare clinical and personality features in anorectic patients (AN) with a high and low drive for thinness (DT). Method: The samples comprised 244 AN in- and out-patients at the Department of Psychiatry and Medical Psychology (University of Ghent) in Belgium. Subjects were assessed on clinical and psychometric parameters. Results: 27% (n=62) of the subjects had low DT as measured by the Eating Disorder Inventory (EDI) of whom 48 were restricting anorectic patients (ANR). This atypical anorectic group appeared to have less severe psychopathology with a lower Harm Avoidance and higher Self Directedness as subscales of the Temperament and Character Inventory (TCI). Discussion: Weight concerns is an important diagnostic criterion, but may have other motivations than a culture-bound drive for thinness. This differentiation may have therapeutic consequences. Introduction Weight phobia is a main criterion to distinguish patients suffering from eating disorders (EDs) from those with other psychiatric diagnoses and from non-clinical populations (Russell, 1970, 1979; DSM-IV, 1994). In anorexia nervosa (AN), this intense fear of fatness perseveres in the face of emaciation and remains present even after weight restoration (Bastiani, Rao, Weltzin, Kaye; 1995). The morbid fear of fat is often measured by clinical means and self-report as the drive for thinness (DT; Ramacciotti, Dell’Osso, Paoli, et al., 2002). Thus, while this feature is considered an important element in the diagnosis of eating disorders, it is surprising to note that fat phobia or a drive for thinness is absent in a substantial proportion of non-Western (Lee, Ho & Hsu, 1993) and Western (Ramacciotti et al., 2002; Strober, Freeman & Morell, 1999) patients. The absence of fat phobia in AN patients has been explained by the comorbid presence of a conversion disorder (Garfinkel, Kaplan, Garner, & Darby, 1983), by the egosyntonic nature of emaciation (Theander, 1995; Lasègue, 1873/1997), as a consequence of the denial of illness, and even as a feature of grandiosity (Orimoto & Vitousek, 1992). Furthermore, Lee et al. (2001) explained the diversity of rationales for food refusal in AN patients as congruous with 5 Vervaet, M., Audenaert, K., & van Heeringen, C. (2004). European Eating Disorders Review, 12, 375-379. historical, clinical, and community studies showing that attributions regarding weight and shape were not static. Instead, they varied with situations (Lee, 1995), across the lifespan (Tiggemann & Stevens, 1999), and with the body weight (Fairburn, Shafran, & Cooper, 1999). We recently showed an association between a substantial number of cognitive and behavioural characteristics and personality traits in ED patients (Vervaet, Audenaert, & van Heeringen, 2003). It can be questioned whether DT is also associated with particular personality characteristics. Recent studies of the personality of ED patients have commonly used Cloningers Temperament and Character Inventory (TCI; Cloninger, Przybeck, Svrakic, & Wetzel, 1994), showing a comparatively high score on Harm Avoidance and low score on Novelty Seeking and Reward Dependence, the data concerning the last dimension not being equivocal (Brewerton, Hand, & Bishop, 1993; Bulik, Sullivan, Weltzin, & Kay, 1995; Cloninger et al., 1994; Klump, Bulik, Pollice et al., 2000), and by low scores on Self-Directedness and high scores on Persistence (Diaz-Marsa, Carrasco, Hollander, Cesar, & Saiz-Ruiz, 2000). The present study aimed at investigating whether the presence or absence of DT is associated with personality characteristics in patients with anorexia nervosa. Methods Subjects The study group consisted of patients who were consecutively referred to the in- and out-patient units of the Centre for Eating Disorders at the Department of Psychiatry of the University Hospital Gent between December 1998 and March 2003, and who met the DSM-IV criteria for eating disorders. All patients gave informed consent compliant with the Code of Ethics of the World Medical Association (Declaration of Helsinki) and the study was conducted following the approval of the local ethics committee. Assessments Weight and height were measured during the first consultation, while highest and lowest adult weight ever and duration of illness were assessed by self-report. The current body mass index (BMI) was calculated, as well the highest and lowest lifetime BMI (HBMI, LBMI), based on the highest and lowest adult weight ever. Age of onset was defined as the age at which eating behaviour changed. Patients were asked to report the presence (i.e. two or more times per week) or absence of bingeing and/or purging behaviours. The Dutch version of the SCAN (WHO-Schedules for Clinical Assessment in Neuropsychiatry) was used to assign diagnoses according to DSM-IV criteria (Giel & Nienhuis, 1996). Patients also completed the Eating Disorders Inventory (EDI; Garner, et al., 1983), the Dutch Eating Behaviour Questionnaire (DEBQ; Van Strien, 1986) and the validated Dutch version of the Temperament and Character Inventory (TCI; Duysens, Spinhoven, Goekoop, Spermon, & Eurelings-Bontekoe, 2000). On the basis of the score obtained on the EDI subscale ‘Drive for thinness’ (DT) and calculated using Garner’s (1991) item-transformation, we divided the sample in two groups. The first group comprised patients with a DT higher than 7 (the “typical” group) and the second group consisted of patients with a DT up to 7 (the “atypical” group). The cutoff score of 7 was similar to that in previous studies (e.g. Ramacciotti et al., 2002) in order to allow for comparison of our results. The six-point Likert scale was recalculated in a score of 0-3 with a range of the total score between 0 and 21. Results Among the total group of referred patients (n=531), 244 (46.0 percent) were diagnosed with anorexia nervosa, including 167 restrictors (68 percent of the AN patients). Assessment data were available for 226 AN patients (93 percent). Using the above-mentioned EDI-DT cut-off score, the “atypical” group comprised 62 (27 percent) AN patients, while 164 (73 percent) AN patients constituted the “typical” group. The atypical group was significantly larger among restricting AN patients (n=48) than among binging/purging anorexics (n= 14; Pearson χ² = 20; p < .001). No significant difference in the proportion of atypical patients was found between in-patients (n=34) and out-patients (n=28; Pearson χ² = .025; p =.874 ). Further statistical analysis pursued the comparison between atypical and typical AN patients. As shown in Table 1, no significant differences were found for the characteristics age, BMI, highest lifetime BMI and duration of illness. Atypical patients reported a later age of onset of their eating disorder and a lower lifetime BMI. Table 1: Clinical characterics, DEBQ and TCI variabels between atypical and typical AN Atypical AN (N=62) Typical AN (N=164) T-test statistics Mean (SD) Mean (SD) 23.7 (7.3) 21.9 (7.7) 1.6 BMI 15 (1.5) 15.1 (1.5) -0.5 HBMI 20.7 (3.1) 21.2 (3.7) -1.1 LBMI 13.9 (1.8) 14.5 (1.7) -2.4* Duration of illness 4.0 (5.0) 3.9 (6.1) 0.1 Age of onset 19.6 (4.8) 18.0 (4.5) 2.3* Clinical Characteristics Dutch Eating Behaviour Questionnaire (DEBQ) Restrained Eating 3.1 (0.9) 4.3 (0.9) -9.3*** External Eating 3.2 (0.7) 2.9 (1.1) 0.1 Emotional Eating 2.4 (0.9) 2.6 (1.2) 0.2 Temperament en Character Inventory (TCI) Novelty Seeking (NS) 15.9 (6.3) 16.2 (6.9) 0.8 Harm Avoidance (HA) 21.8 (6.8) 24.8 (6.6) -3.0** Reward Dependence (RD) 16.6 (3.6) 16.2 (3.8) 0.5 Persistence (P) 5.9 (2.0) 5.6 (1.9) 0.4 Self-Directedness (SD) 25.3 (7.0) 21.1 (7.4) 3.8*** Cooperativeness (C) 33.4 (4.5) 31.8 (6.7) 0.1 Self-transcendence (ST) 12.3 (6.6) 12.7 (6.4) 0.7 P< 0.05; **P<0.005; ***P<0.001 BMI= body mass index; HBMI = highest adult (>16 years) BMI ever; LBMI = lowest adult (>= 16 years) BMI ever Atypical AN patients were also found to score lower on the ‘restrained eating’ subscale of the Dutch Eating Behaviour Questionnaire (DEBQ). No significant differences were found for scores on the ‘external eating ‘ and ‘emotional eating’ subscales. With regard to personality characteristics, atypical patients scored significantly lower on the temperament dimension ‘harm avoidance’ and significant higher on the character dimension ‘self-directedness’ (Table 1). While no significant difference was found with regard to the occurrence of binging behaviour, there were comparatively fewer atypical patients (n= 29; 47 percent) than typical patients (103; 63 percent) who reported purging (Pearson χ² = 4.759; p < 0.05). Discussion This study aimed at investigating the occurrence and personality-related correlates of a low drive for thinness among anorexia nervosa patients by comparing their characteristics with those of anorexia nervosa patients with a high drive for thinness. The findings indicate that a low drive for thinness is present in more than one-quarter of these patients, and particularly common among those of the restricting type. The findings also demonstrate that a low drive for thinness is associated with a higher age of onset of the eating disorder and with less severe behavioural manifestations such as purging and restrained eating. Also, with regard to their personality profile, anorexic patients with a low drive for thinness showed comparatively less severe disturbances, i.e. a relatively less increased harm avoidance and a relatively less decreased self-directedness, than anorexic patients with a high drive for thinness. With regard to the occurrence of a low drive for thinness, the current findings are in keeping with those from previous studies using the same diagnostic criteria for drive for thinness (Ramacciotti et al., 2002; Strober, Freeman & Morrell, 1999). In accordance with the findings of Lee and colleagues (Lee, Lee, Ngai, Lee, & Wing, 2001), no differences between the two groups were found regarding BMI, age at referral and duration of illness. We could not demonstrate a significantly lower premorbid BMI in association with a low drive for thinness. It thus appears that a low drive for thinness among anorexia nervosa patients is associated with less eating-related pathology (lower frequency of purging and less restrained eating) and less severe psychopathology (lower harm avoidance and higher self-directedness). Strober, Freeman and Morrell (1999) have stated in a similar way that atypical anorexics have a comparatively smaller chance of progressing to chronic morbidity, a faster rate of full clinical recovery, and a lower risk of developing a first onset of binge eating. According to current cognitive behavioural theory, an extreme need to control eating is the central feature of AN, with a superimposed tendency to judge self-worth in terms of shape and weight in Western societies (Fairburn, Shafran & Cooper, 1998; Fairburn, Cooper & Shafran, 2003). This need to control eating has been attributed to fat phobia, but recent research findings suggest that reinforcing effects of food restriction per se play a crucial role. Kaye et al. (2003) hypothesize that starvation in anorexia nervosa patients serves to reduce 5-HT neuronal activity in response to their trait-related (i.e. premorbid and persisting after recovery) increase in serotonergic (5-HT) neuronal transmission. This increased 5-HT signal transmission does not respond to normal regulatory mechanisms, and thus contributes to uncomfortable core symptoms such as obsessionality, perfectionism, harm avoidance, and anxiety. Starvation is associated with a dietary-induced reduction of tryptophan (TRP; the precursor of 5-HT), and thus may temporarily reduce this dysphoric behavioural state. The influence of bingeing or purging on the anxiolytic effect of starvation in anorexia nervosa patients is currently not known. Our present findings indicate an association between a low drive for thinness and restricting rather than binging/purging behaviour in anorexia nervosa patients. The drive for thinness in anorexia nervosa patients may in general thus reflect a drive for food restriction as a compensatory mechanism for increased anxiety. In view of the comparatively less severe eating disorder-related symptoms, this mechanism appears to be relatively effective in patients with a low drive for thinness. Our findings suggest that this relative effectiveness is associated with a comparatively lower harm avoidance and a higher self-directedness. Low self-directedness is indeed associated with an external locus of control, and thus with a more outspoken susceptibility to external influences. Knowing that individuals with character traits denoting low self-directedness and high self-transcendence may be particularly reactive and susceptible to societal messages referring to the ideology of slenderness (Gendall, Joyce, Sullivan and Bullik, 1998), it can be hypothesized that typical anorexia nervosa patients translate their food phobia into a weight phobia, characterized by behavioural compensatory mechanisms such as more restrained eating and purging. Ramacciotti et al. (2002) suggest that the treatment of the non-fat phobic group of anorexia nervosa patients should be less cognitively based and less focused on cultural tyranny and fat. However, there are several reasons to include cognitive strategies in the treatment of anorexia nervosa patients with a low drive for thinness. The important interaction between an extreme need for control and the use of food restriction to judge self-worth in order to cope with anxiety in anorexia nervosa patients (Fairburn et al., 1998, 2003) requires cognitive restructuring procedures. Such strategies should be added to behavioural interventions aiming at increasing body weight, perhaps through in vivo exposure to food to attain the extinction of the anxiety-food connection. In order to prevent relapse, and in keeping with previous findings (see e.g. Kaye, Barbarich, Putnam et al., 2003), the current study indicates that traitdependent anxiety regulation should also be addressed. In view of the well-documented role of 5-HT in the modulation of harm avoidance (Cloninger, 2000), serotonergic drugs may be particularly indicated (Frank, Kaye, Weltzin, et al., 2001; Kaye, Greeno, Moss, et al., 1998). References American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). DSM IV, Washington DC: Author. Bastiani, A.M., Rao, R:, Weltzin, T. & Kaye, W. (1995) Perfectionism in anorexia nervosa. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 17(2), 147-52. Brewerton, T.D., Hand, L.D., & Bishop, E.R. (1993). The Tridimensional Personality Questionnaire in eating disorder patients. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 14, 213-218. Bulik, C.M., Sullivan, P.F., Weltzin, T.E., & Kay, W.H. (1995). Temperament in eating disorders. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 17, 251-261. Cloninger, C.R., Przybeck, T.R., Svrakic, D.M., & Wetzel, R.D. (1994). The Temperament and Character Inventory (TCI): A guide to its development and use. St. Louis, MO: Center for Psychobiology of Personality, Washington University. Cloninger, C.R. (2000). Biology of personality dimensions. Current opinion in Psychiatry, 13, 611-616. Diaz-Marsa, M., Carrasco, J.L., & Saiz-Ruiz, J. (2000). A study of temperament and personality in anorexia and bulimia nervosa. Journal of Personal Disorders , 14(4), 352-359. Duijsens, I.J., Spinhoven, P., Goekoop, J.G., Spermon, T., & Eurelings-Bontekoe, E.H.M. (2000). The Dutch temperament and character inventory (TCI): dimensional structure, reliability and validity in a normal and psychiatric outpatient sample. Personality and Individual Differences, 28, 487-499. Fairburn, C.G., Shafran, R., & Cooper, Z. (1998). A cognitive behavioural theory of anorexia nervosa. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 37, 1-13. Fairburn, C.G., Cooper, Z., & Shafran, R. (2003). Cognitive behaviour therapy for eating disorders: a “transdiagnostic” theory and treatment. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 41, 509-528. Frank, G.F., Kaye, W.H., Weltzin, T.E., Perel, J., Moss, H., McConaha, C., & Pollice, C. (2001). Altered response to metachlorophenylpiperazine in anorexia nervosa: support for a persistent alteration of serotonin activity after short-term weight restoration. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 30, 57-68. Garfinkel, P.E, Kaplan, A.S., Garner, D.M., & Darby, P.L. (1983). The differentiation of vomiting and weight loss as a conversion disorder from anorexia nervosa. American Journal of Psychiatry, 140, 1019-1022. Garner, D.M. (1991). Eating Disorder Inventory-2 Manual. Odessa: Psychological Assessment Resourses, Inc. Garner, D.M., Olmstead, M.P, & Polivy, J. (1983). The Eating Disorders Inventory: A measure of cognitive-behaviroural dimensions of anorexia and bulimia. In P. L. Darby (Ed.), Anorexia Nervosa: Recent developments in research (pp. 173-184). New York: Alan R. Liss. Garner, D.M., Olmstead, M.P., & Polivy, J. (1985). Development and validation of a multidimensional eating disorder inventory for anorexia nervosa and bulimia. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 2, 15-34. Gendall, K.A., Joyce, P.R., Sullivan, P.F., & Bulik, C.M. (1998). Personality and dimensions of dietary restraint. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 24, 371-379. Giel, R. & Nienhuis, F.J. (1996). Vragenschema’s voor de klinische beoordeling in de neuropsychiatrie. World Health Organisation Mental Health, Geneva. Hsu, L.K., Lee, S. (1993). Is weight phobia always necessary for a diagnosis of anorexia nervosa? American Journal of psychiatry, 150 (10), 1466-71. Humphrey, L.L. (1991). Object relations and the family system: An integrative approach to understanding and treating eating disorders. In C. Johnson (Ed.), Psychodynamic treatment of anorexia nervosa and bulimia (pp. 322-353). New York: Guilford Press. Kaye, W.H., Greeno, C.G., Moss, H., Fernstrom, J., Fernstrom, M., Lilenfeld, L.R., Weltzin, T.E., & Mann, J.J. (1998). Alterations in serotonin activity and psychiatric symptomatology after recovery from bulimia nervosa. Archives of General Psychiatry, 55, 927-935. Kaye, W.H., Barbarich, N.C., Putnam, K., Gendall, K.A., Fernstrom, J., Fernstrom, M., McConaha, C.W., & Kishor, A. (2003). Anxiolytic effects of acute tryptophan depletion in anorexia nervosa. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 33, 257267. Kleifield, E.I., Sunday, S., Hurt, S.W., & Halmi, K.A. (1993). Psychometric validation of the Tridimensional Personality Questionnaire: Application to subgroups of eating disorders. Comprehensive Psychiatry , 249-253. Klump, K.L., Bulik, C.M., Pollice, C., Halmi, K.A., Fichter, M.M., Berrettini, W.H., Devlin, B., Strober, M., Kaplan, A., Woodside, D.B., Treasure, J., Shabbout, M., Lilenfeld, L.R., & Plotnicov, K.H. (2000). Temperament and character in women with anorexia nervosa. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 188 (9), 559-567. Lasègue, E.C. (1997). On hysterical anorexia. Obesity Research, 5, 492-497. (Original work published in 1873). Lee, S., Ho, P., & Hsu, L.K.G. (1993). Fat phobic and non-fat phobic anorexia nervosa: A comparative study of 70 Chinese patients in Hong Kong. Psychological Medicine, 23, 999-1017. Lee, S. (1995). Self-starvation in contexts: Towards the culturally sensitive understanding of anorexia nervosa. Social Science and Medicine, 41, 25-36. Lee, S., Lee, A.M., Ngai, E, Lee, D.T.S, & Wing, Y.K. (2001). Rationales for food refusal in Chinese patients with anorexia nervosa. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 29, 224-229. Orimoto, L., & Vitousek, K.B. (1992). Anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa. In P. H. Wilson (Ed.), Principles and practice of relapse prevention (pp. 85-127). New York: Guilford Press. Ramacciotti, C.E., Dell’Osso, L., Paoli, R.A., Ciapparelli, A., Coli, E., Kaplan, A.S., & Garfinkel, P.E. (2002). Characteristics of eating disorder patients without a drive for thinness. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 32, 206-212. Regier, E., Touyz, S.W., Swain, T., & Beumont, P.J.V. (2001). Cross-cultural research on anorexia nervosa: Assumptions regarding the role of body weight. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 29, 205-215. Russell, G.F.M. (1970). Anorexia nervosa; Its identity as an illness and its treatment. In J.H. Price (Ed.), Modern trends in psychological medicine (Vol.2, pp. 131-164). London: Butterworths. Russell, G.F.M. (1979). Bulimia nervosa: An ominous variant of anorexia nervosa. Psychological Medicine, 9, 429-448. Strober, M., Freeman, R., & Morell, W. (1999). Atypical anorexia nervosa: separation from typical cases in course and outcome in a long-term prospective study. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 25, 135-142. Tellegen, A. (1985). Structures of mood and personality and their relevance to assessing anxiety with an emphasis on selfreport. In Tuma, A.H. & Maser, J.D (Eds), Anxiety and anxiety disorders (pp. 681-706). Stad-uitgeverij. Tiggemann, M., & Stevens, C. (1999). Weight concern across the life-span: Relationship to self-esteem. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 26, 103-106. Theander, S. (1995); The essence of anorexia nervosa: Comment on Gerald Russell’s Anorexia nervosa through time.” In G. Szmukler, C. Dare, & J. Treasure (Eds.), Handbook of eating disorders: Theory, treatment and research (pp. 19-24). London: Wiley. Van Strien, T. (1986). Eating behaviour, personality traits and body mass. Swets & Zeitlinger B.V.: Lisse. Van Strien, T., Frijters, J.E.R., Bergers, G.P.A., & Defares, P.B. (1986). The Dutch Eating Behaviour Questionnaire (DEBQ) for assessment of restrained, emotional and external eating behaviour. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 5, 295-315. Vervaet, M., Audenaert, K., & van Heeringen, C. (2003). Cognitive and behavioural characteristics are associated with personality dimensions in patients with eating disorders. European Eating Disorders Review, 11, 1-16. Vitousek, K. (1996) The current status of cognitive-behavioral models of anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa. In P. Salkovskis (Ed.), Frontiers of Cognitive Therapy (pp. 383-418). New York: Guilford Press. Waller, D.A., Gullion, C.M., Petty, F., Hardy, B.W. & al. (1993). Tridimensional Personality Questionnaire and serotonin in bulimia nervosa. Psychiatry Research , 48 9-15. 3. Cognitive and behavioral characteristics are associated with personality dimensions in patients with eating disorders6 DSM-IV categorizes eating disorders according to behavioural and cognitive characteristics. Based on personalityrelated and biological research, hypotheses have been formulated to explain differences in the symptomatology between the various types of eating disorders. Therefore, the study of the association between personality-related characteristics and behavioural and cognitive characteristics may contribute to our understanding of the causes and course of eating disorders. This study aimed, first, at describing personality characteristics (using Cloninger’s Temperament and Character Inventory) in a group of eating disordered patients (n= 272) according to the type of eating disorder. Three groups were compared: restricting anorexics (n=71), purging anorexics (n=84) and bulimics (n=118). Secondly, the association between personality characteristics and cognitive and behavioural aspects, using the Eating Disorders Inventory and the Dutch Eating Behaviour Questionnaire, was measured. In bulimics, positive correlations were found between novelty seeking on the one hand and external and emotional eating and bulimia on the other. Contrary to expectation, there was no significant correlation between novelty seeking and body dissatisfaction in bulimics. The significant difference between the restricting and purging type of anorexics regarding self-directedness, and restrained and emotional eating and drive for thinness was corresponding with the significant negative correlation between these characteristics. Introduction Evidence is accumulating that stable personality traits play a major role in the development of eating disorders. It has been shown with remarkable consistency that restricting-type anorexia nervosa, in particular, is characterized by a cluster of such traits. These temperamental characteristics include emotional restraint, avoidance of novelty, anxious worry and self-doubt, weight- and shape- related anxiety, compliancy, obsessionality, rigidity, over-control, perfectionism, and perseverance in the face of non-reward (Beumont, George, & Smart, 1976; Bruch, 1978; Halmi et al., 2000, Lilenfeld, Kaye, & Strober, 1997; Lilenfeld & Kaye, 1998; Pryor & Wiederman, 1996; Strober, 1980, 1995). Retrospective studies indeed suggest that these characteristics commonly predate the onset of the eating disorder, and remain present even after long-term weight restoration (Pollice, Kaye, Greenco, & Weltzin, 1997; Srinivasagan, Kaye, Plotnicov, Greeno, Weltzin, & Rao, 1995; Strober, 1980). 6 Vervaet, M., Audenaert, K., & van Heeringen, C. (2003). European Eating Disorders Review, 11 (5), 363-379. Although the clinical presentation of bulimia nervosa is less consistent than the symptomatology of restricting-type anorexia nervosa, the involvement of a number of behavioural traits in the development of this disorder has been shown to be equally prevalent. Such traits include thrill-seeking and excitability, affective instability and impulsivity, and a tendency towards marked dysphoria in response to rejection or non-reward (Bulik, Beidel, Duchman, Weltzin, & Kaye, 1992; Casper, Hedeker, & McKlough, 1992; Diaz-Marsa, Carrasco, & Saiz, 2000; Vitousek & Manke, 1994). These characteristics may protect individuals against sustained dietary restriction, and, thus, predispose an individual to periodic loss of control and eventual dietary chaos. Some traits may be involved in the development of both anorexia and bulimia nervosa, as over-concern with body image and thinness, increased harm avoidance, dysphoria, obsessions about symmetry or exactness, and perfectionism may persist following recovery from these disorders (Anonymous, 2001; Casper, 1990; Diaz-Marsa et al., 2000; Kaye et al., 1998; Kaye, Strober, Stein, & Gendall, 1999; Srinivasagam et al., 1995). Research on brain neurotransmitter systems has supported the hypothesis that an underlying biological diathesis and temperament may place someone at risk for developing anorexia (Kaye, 1997). For example, increased levels of 5-HIAA, the major metabolite of serotonin, have been found in the cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) of anorexia and bulimia nervosa patients which persist after long-term clinical recovery from (Kaye, Gwirtsman, George, & Ebert, 1991; Kaye et al., 1998). It has been suggested that increased levels of CSF 5-HIAA reflect an increased activity of the serotonergic system that could contribute to behavioural constraint, obsessionality, and inhibition of appetite (Soubrie, 1986; Spoont, 1992). Increased serotonin activity could make people specifically vulnerable to developing an eating disorder as well as certain core phenotypic aspects of these disorders, including anxious dysphoria, obsessional thinking, perseverance, and cognitive distortions of the aversive consequences of eating and weight gain (Kay and Strober, 2003). These processes could be further enhanced by malnutritiontriggered changes in hypothalamic neuropeptides, that modulate conditional learning (Demitrack et al., 1990). Cloninger (1986, 1987a, 1987b) has developed a psychobiological model of personality including clearly defined traits or personality dimensions. The initial model included three temperamental dimensions which are thought to be independently heritable and mediated by separate neurobiological mechanisms. Novelty seeking (NS) reflects behavioural activation, a tendency toward frequent exploratory activity and excitement in response to novel stimuli. Harm avoidance (HA) describes the tendency to respond to aversive stimuli and thus to inhibit behaviour to avoid punishment or frustrating non-reward. Reward dependence (RD) reflects the maintenance of behaviour that has been previously rewarding, especially in the interpersonal sphere. Normative studies using the TPQ confirmed the structure of temperament with the exception that persistence (P) emerged as distinct fourth temperamental dimension, describing perseverance despite frustration or fatigue (Cloninger, Svrakic, & Przybeck, 1993). Further observations suggested that the TPQ did not measure aspects of personality that are related to dimensions of character, or the development of self-concepts. The character dimensions include self-directedness, cooperativeness, and self-transcendence. Self-directedness (SD) is characterized by responsibility and resourcefulness in initiating and organizing steps to achieve personal goals. Cooperativeness (C) describes social tolerance, empathy, helpfulness, compassion, and moral principles. Self-transcendence (ST) involves the spontaneous feeling of participation in one’s surroundings as a united whole. These additional dimensions were included in the “seven factor model”, that can be assessed by means of the Temperament and Character Inventory (Cloninger et al., 1993; Svrakic, Whitehead, Przybeck, & Cloninger, 1993). The diagnosis of eating disorders according to criteria such as those in the DSM classification system is based mainly on cognitive and behavioural characteristics. It might, however, be interesting to study the association between these characteristics and personality dimensions for the following reasons. First, a growing body of evidence supports the existence of separate genetic and neurobiological underpinnings of personality characteristics such as Cloninger’s temperament dimensions. For example, an association between serotonergic dysfunction and the temperamental dimension HA has been described, and evidence of a genetic contribution to the variation in NS is increasing (Kaye et al., 2001). Insight in the association between such personality characteristics and eating disorders can therefore be expected to contribute to our knowledge of the genetic and neurobiological aspects of eating disorders. Second, insight into the role of personality characteristics in the development of eating disorders may provide new approaches in treatment and prevention. Therefore, this study aimed, first, at describing personality dimensions and eating-disordered related cognitive and behavioural aspects in patients who suffer from eating disorders according to the type of eating disorder. Secondly, this study aimed at describing the association between core cognitive and behavioural characteristics of eating disorders and these personality dimensions. Methods Subjects Three hundred eighteen patients who were consecutively referred to the in- and out-patient Eating Disorders Unit at the Department of Psychiatry of the University Hospital Gent between December 1998 and June 2001 and who met the DSM criteria of Eating Disorders were enrolled in the study. Research was compliant with the Code of Ethics of the World Medical Association (Declaration of Helsinki) and performed following the guidelines of the local ethics committee. Clinical measures Weight and height, highest and lowest weight ever and duration of illness were carefully checked. The body mass index (BMI) was calculated, as well the highest and lowest body mass index (HBMI, LBMI), based on the highest and lowest weight ever. Instruments The Dutch version of the SCAN (Schedules for Clinical Assessment in Neuropsychiatry) was used as a structured clinical interview to assign a diagnosis according to DSM-IV criteria (Giel & Nienhuis, 1996). The Dutch translation of the Eating Disorders Inventory was used (EDI; Garner, Olmstead, & Polivy, 1983) with subscales assessing the drive for thinness, bulimia, body dissatisfaction, ineffectiveness, perfectionism, interpersonal distrust, interceptive awareness and maturity fears. The validated Dutch version of the TCI (Duysens, Spinhoven, Goekoop, Spermon, Eurolings-Bontekoe, 2000) was used as a personality questionnaire to assess temperament and character characteristics. The Dutch Eating Behaviour Questionnaire (DEBQ; Van Strien, 1986) was used to assess the behavioural characteristics: emotional eating (eating elicited by diffuse and clear emotional stimuli), restrained eating (the tendency to diet in order to lose weight), and external eating (eating elicited by external stimuli). Statistics Statistical analysis was performed using SPSS 9.0. One way ANOVA with post hoc Bonferroni comparision was used to compare three diagnostic subgroups: anorectic restrictors (AN-R), anorectic binge-eating/purging type (AN-P) and bulimics (B). These groups were compared with regard to demographic, clinical and behavioural (DEBQ), cognitive (EDI) and personality (TCI) characteristics. Correlations between personality dimensions and scores on the EDI and DEBQ were measured by calculating Pearson correlation coefficients. Results The referred group consisted of 318 patients, including 154 (48,4%) anorectic patients of whom 70 (22 %) were restrictor-type anorectics (AN-R) and 84 (26,4 %) were purging-type anorectics (AN-P). Among the bulimic patients (B=118; 37,1 %), 100 (31,4 %) were purging-type bulimics and 18 (5,7 %) were non-purging type bulimics. The category of eating disorders not otherwise specified (EDNOS), contained 46 (14,4 %) patients of whom 16 (5.0 %) met the criteria of binge eating disorder (BED). This paper will report on the findings in anorectic and bulimic patients. In view of the specific nature of the findings in EDNOS patients, these will be reported in a forthcoming paper (Vervaet et al., unpublished data). As no differences were found between the purging and non-purging bulimics, bulimics (B) were described as one group and compared to ‘anorectic restrictors’ (AN-R) and ‘purging anorectics‘ (AN-P). The group of 272 patients contains 268 females (age range: 13 to 49 years; mean = 22.3; sd = 7.0) and 4 males, 1 restricting-anorectic and 3 purging bulimics (age range: 14 to 44 years; mean = 25.5; sd =12.9). Table 1: Age, BMI and duration of illness, by type of eating disorder. Variable Age(years) BMI (kg/m2) LowestBMI After 16 HighestBMI After 16 Duration of ED (years) AN-R N= 70 M SD 21,17 6,49 14,64b 1,50 AN-P N= 84 M SD 23,11 7,66 15,32c 1,34 B N= 118 M SD 22,11 6.88 20,09bc 3,35 13,73b 1,68 19,78ab 2.20 3,23 3,52 14,41c 1,66 21.42ac 3.14 4,67 5,12 18,27bc 2,82 25.12bc 4.34 4,06 4,20 F p 1.51 0.222 206.58 0.000 116.66 0.000 56.194 0.000 2.103 0.124 ED = eating disorder; AN-R = anorexia nervosa of the restricting type AN-P = anorectics of the purging type; B = bulimic women ANOVA with Bonferoni post hoc comparison (significant p<0.05 level) a = comparison between AN-R and AN-P b = comparison between AN-R and B c = comparison between AN-P and B When comparing B patients to AN-R and AN-P patients, there was no difference regarding age and duration of illness. As expected, the BMI at admission (AN-R: m=14.6; sd=1.5; ANP: m=15.3; sd=1.3), lowest BMI ever (AN-R: m=13.7; sd=1.7; AN-P: m=14.4; sd=1.7) and highest BMI ever (AN-R: m=19.8; sd=2.2; AN-P: m=21.4; sd=3.1) were significantly lower (p<0.001) in the ANgroup than in the B-group (BMI: m=20.1; sd=3.3; LBMI: m=18.3; sd=2.8; HBMI: m=25.1; sd=4.3). The only difference between the restricting and purging anorexics was a significant higher BMI ever among patients in the latter group (p=0.014). Comparing the behavioural characteristics, there was no difference regarding eating with diffuse or clear emotions. Thus, only scores on ‘total emotional eating’ were included in the further analyses. Table 2: Behavioural characteristics by type of eating disorders. AN-R AN-P N= 67 N= 81 Variable Restrained eating External eating Total emotional eating M SD 3.59ab 1.16 2.96b 0.94 2.15ab 1.00 M SD 4.20a 1.10 3.02c 0.92 2.79ac 1.18 B N= 109 M SD 4.24b 0.81 3.57bc 1.01 3.76bc 1.00 F p 9.80 0.000 11.56 0.000 51.19 0.000 ED = eating disorder; AN-R = anorexia nervosa of the restricting type; AN-P = anorectics of the purging type; B = bulimic women ANOVA with Bonferoni post hoc comparison (significant p<0.05 level) a = comparison between AN-R and AN-P; b = comparison between AN-R and B; c = comparison between AN-P and B Scores on restrained eating were lower in the AN-R group (p=.000; m=3.6; sd= 1.2) than in the AN-P group (m=4.2; sd=1.1) and the B-group (m=4.24; sd=0.81). Anorexics had a lower score on external eating (p=.000; AN-R: m=3; sd= 0.9; AN-P: m=3; sd=0.9)) in comparison with the bulimic group (m=3.6; sd= 1). The scores on the emotional (p=.000) subscale of the DEBQ were lowest for the AN-R group (m= 2.1; sd=1) and lower for the AN-P group (m=2.8; sd=1.2) when comped to the B-group (m=3.8; sd=1). For the cognitive characteristics only significant differences were found for the ‘drive for thinness’ (p=.000), ‘bulimia’ (p=.000) and ‘body dissatisfaction’ (p=.000), subscales of the EDI. Table 3: Cognitive characteristics by type of eating disorders. Variable EDI Drive thinness Bulimia for Body dissatisfaction Ineffectiveness Perfectionism Interpersonal distrust Interceptive awareness Maturity fears AN-R N= 70 M SD 9.94ab 7.12 0.76ab 1.49 11.50b 6.51 11.50 7.59 7.13 4.95 5.90 4.58 9.33 5.31 7.57 5.67 AN-P N= 83 M SD 13.75a 6.54 4.37ac 5.32 13.48c 7.87 13.34 7.49 8.19 4.79 6.94 4.58 10.69 5.66 6.73 5.21 B N= 115 M SD 15.49b 5.27 9.52bc 5.80 17.02bc 7.46 12.45 8.18 7.25 5.27 5.55 4.43 10.90 5.86 7.34 5.95 F p 17.55 0.000 74.19 0.000 13.34 0.000 1.05 0.352 1.11 0.331 2.36 0.097 1.81 0.165 0.46 0.628 ED = eating disorder; AN-R = anorexia nervosa of the restricting type; AN-P = anorectics of the purging type; B = bulimic women ANOVA with Bonferoni post hoc comparison (significant p<0.05 level) a = comparison between AN-R and AN-P; b = comparison between AN-R and B; c = comparison between AN-P and B The ‘drive for thinness’ was lower in the AN-R group (m=9.9; sd=7.1) when compared to the AN-P group (m=13.7; sd=6.5) and the bulimic group (m=15.5; sd=5.3). Scores on the ‘bulimia’ subscale were lowest (p=.000) in the AN-R group (m=0.8; sd=1.5), and lower among the AN-P patients (m=4.4; sd=5.3) than in the bulimics (m=9.5; sd=5.8). Finally, ‘body dissatisfaction’ was lower (p=.000) in the anorectic group (AN-R: m=11.50; sd=6.51; AN-P: m=13.5; sd=7.9), than in the bulimic group (m=17; sd=7.5). The scores on the Temperament and Character Scales are shown in Table 4. Table 4: Personality characteristics by type of eating disorder Variable TCI Novelty seeking(NS) Exploratory excitability (NS1) Impulsiveness (NS2) Extravagance (NS3) Harm avoidance (HA) Reward Dependence (RD) Persistence (P) Self-directedness (SD) Enlightened Second Nature (SD5) Cooperativeness (CO) Selftranscendence (ST) AN-R N= 70 M SD 15.37b 6.14 5.06 2.79 3.04b 2.15 2.79ab 2.27 23.72 6.90 16.32 3.98 AN-P N= 84 M SD 16.33c 7.09 4.68c 2.53 3.19c 2.48 3.96ac 2.63 24.94 6.29 16.35 3.19 B N= 109 M SD 19.49bc 6.62 5.63c 2.59 4.13bc 2.51 4.84bc 2.40 23.85 7.09 16.39 3.84 5.81b 1.99 23.25ab 7.56 5.59 2.09 20.30a 7.28 6.47ab 2.89 32.60 7.27 12.66 6.81 F p 9.59 0.000 3.14 0.045 5.54 0.004 14.80 0.000 0.77 0.462 0.01 0.994 5.03b 1.95 19.61b 7.01 3.65 0.027 5.56 0.004 5.38a 2.40 4.56b 2.18 12.75 0.000 31.79 5.64 12.99 6.22 31.68 6.07 11.85 7.13 0.49 0.610 0.71 0.495 ED = eating disorder; AN-R = anorexia nervosa of the restricting type; AN-P = anorectics of the purging type; B = bulimic women ANOVA with Bonferoni post hoc comparison (significant p<0.05 level) a = comparison between AN-R and AN-P; b = comparison between AN-R and B; c = comparison between AN-P and B The temperament dimensions ‘Novelty Seeking’ (p=.000) and ‘Persistence’ (p=.027) differed significantly between anorectic and bulimic patients. ‘Novelty Seeking’ and the ‘impulsiveness’ subdimension (p=.004) were lower among patients in the AN-R group (NS: m=15.4; sd=6.1; NS2: m=3; sd=2.1) than among those in the AN-P group (NS: m=16.3; sd=7.1; NS2: m=3.2; sd=2.5) and among bulimic patients (NS: m=19.5; sd=6.6; NS2: m=4.1; sd=2.5). ‘Exploratory excitability’ was lower (p=.045) in the AN-P group (m=4.7; sd=2.5) than in the B group (m=5.6; sd=2.6). The scores on ‘Extravagance’ were lowest (p=.000) in the AN-R group (m=2.8; sd=2.3), but also lower in the AN-P (m=4; sd=2.6) than in the B group (m=4.8; sd=2.4). Persistence was significantly higher in the AN-R group (m=5.8; sd=2) in comparison with bulimic patients (m=5; sd=1.9). Character dimensions differed with regard to ‘Self-directedness’, with a highest score (p=.004) for the restrictor-type of Anorexia Nervosa (m=23.2; sd=7.6), especially due to a higher ‘Enlightened Second Nature’ (p=.000) in this latter group (m=6.5; sd=2.9) compared to the other groups (AN-P: m=5.4; sd=2.4; B: m=4.6; sd=2.2). The group of restrictors (n=71) may not have been homogeneous with regard to duration of illness, so that this specific group was split into a ‘chronic’ group, with a duration of illness of 5 years and more, and an ‘acute’ group with less than 5 years duration of illness. The ‘chronic’ group (n = 18) differed from the ‘acute’ group (n = 53) only in a higher score on reward dependence (m=18.6; sd=3.7 versus m=15.6; sd=3.7; p=.011; df=2). The difference seems to be caused more specifically by a higher score on the subscale ‘attachment’ (m=5.6; sd=2.5 versus m=4; sd=2.2; p=.048; df=2). The same distinction was made for bulimics, but revealed no differences. Bulimics were also divided in two weight groups, with a cut-off score of BMI 20 without showing any differences in personality characteristics between the bulimics with lower and higher weight. Further statistical analyses pursued the assessment of correlations between personality dimensions, cognitive aspects and behavioural characteristics (Table 5). Table 5: Correlations between personality dimensions and cognitive and behavioural characteristics of eating disordered patients NS HA RD P SD CO ST Bulimia .26** -.02 -.07 -.09 -.11 -.12 .02 Drive for thinness Dissatis— faction Ineffecti veness Perfecti onism Distrust .12 .30** -.02 .01 -.44** -.12 .23* -.04 .36** .10 .09 -.46** -.03 .16 -.2 9** .60* -.03 .01 -.50** .02 -.03 -.28** .18 .14 .43** -.10 .05 .11 -.28** .34** -.41** -.09 -.32** -.12 -.03 Interoceptiveness -.05 .43** -.09 .09 -.40** -.08 .18 Maturity -.14 .36** -.03 .10 -.30** -.09 .07 Restrained eating External eating Total emotional -.01 .28** .05 .19 -.26** -.10 .12 .22* -.14 -.08 -.18 -.05 -.10 .12 .235* .082 -.161 -.075 -.219* -.252 -.126 * = p < . 05, ** = p < . 01 The scores on personality dimensions correlated significantly with a number of those rating cognitive and behavioural characteristics. Temperament factors correlated in particular strongly with cognitive characteristics. Novelty Seeking was found to correlate with ‘bulimia’, ‘ineffectiveness’, ‘perfectionism’ and ‘distrust’, characteristics in particular, reflecting a loss of control, as found in behavioural characteristics such as emotional and external eating. Harm Avoidance particularly correlated with characteristics reflecting as ‘drive for thinness’, ‘dissatisfaction’, ‘ineffectiveness’, ‘distrust’, ‘interoceptiveness’ and ‘maturity’ and behavioural inhibition. Reward Dependence correlated significantly with ‘distrust’, Persistence with ‘perfectionism’, and Self-Transcendence with ‘drive for thinness’. Discussion The main findings from this study can be summarized as follows. First, within the group of eating disordered patients, personality characteristics differ between types of eating disorders to such an extent that, when compared to bulimic patients, anorectic patients have significantly lower scores on novelty seeking, more specifically on the subdimensions ‘impulsiveness’ and ‘extravagance’. The findings regarding novelty seeking and impulsiveness are in keeping with those from previous studies (Brewerton, Dorn, & Bishop, 1992. Brewerton, Hand, & Tellegen, 1993; Bulik, Sullivan, Weltzin, & Kaye, 1995; Kleifield, Sunday, Hurt, & Halmi, 1993; Tellegen, 1985; Waller et al., 1993). Purging anorectics also differ from bulimics in lower ‘exploratory excitability’, which parallels previous findings on the role of impulsivity in BN. Restricting anorexics score higher on the character scale Selfdirectedness and the subscale of ‘enlightened second nature’, in comparison with the purging type of anorexia. The scores of this latter group are higher on ‘extravagance’ compared to the restrictor-type of anorexia nervosa. The higher score of the ‘chronic’ anorectic restrictor group on reward dependence is in keeping with the study of Bulik and colleagues (1995). Secondly, the types of eating disorders studied differ with regard to cognitive and behavioural characteristics. In general, bulimic patients show higher scores on measures of behavioural characteristics, including eating induced by emotional and external stimuli, and more restrained eating when compared to the restrictor type of anorexia. Higher scores are also found for cognitive characteristics such as ‘bulimia’ and ‘body dissatisfaction’, compared to anorexics, and a higher ‘drive for thinness’, compared to the restrictor type. Within the group of anorectic patients, the restricting type scores lower on restrained and emotional eating and on the drive for thinness and bulimia when compared to the purging type. Thirdly, the study of the association between personality characteristics and cognitive or behavioural characteristics shows three main patterns of correlations. Novelty seeking correlates positively with behavioural characteristics, such as eating following emotional or external triggers, and with bulimia, while negative correlations are found with cognitive factors, including ineffectiveness, perfectionism and distrust. Harm avoidance correlates positively with all assessed cognitive characteristics, except for bulimia and perfectionism with restrained eating. Reward Dependence correlates negatively with interpersonal distrust, and Persistence positively with perfectionism. Finally, self-directedness correlates negatively with nearly all behavioural (except for external eating) and cognitive characteristics (except for bulimia and perfectionism), and self-transcendence positively with the drive for thinness. This latter finding corresponds with the results of a recent study in a group of 101 women in a random general population sample, using the TCI, showing that individuals with character traits denoting low ‘Self-directedness’ and high ‘Self-transcendence’ may be particularly reactive and susceptible to societal messages referring to the ideology of slenderness (Gendall, Joyce, Sullivan, & Bulik, 1998). There is a paucity of studies on the association between personality characteristics, using the TCI, and cognitive or behavioural aspects. Our finding of patterns of correlations between, on the one hand, novelty seeking and self-directedness, and, on the other hand, the assessed behavioural and cognitive characteristics as described above, are in keeping with clusters of characteristics found in bulimics such as higher novelty seeking, more external and emotional eating, and bulimia. However, contrary to expectations no significant correlation between novelty seeking and body dissatisfaction in bulimics can be demonstrated. The significant difference between the restricting and purging type of anorexics corresponds with the significant negative correlation between self-directedness, restrained and emotional eating and drive for thinness. The lower score on extravagance, a subscale of novelty seeking, can probably explain the lower bulimia, since the tendency toward extravagance may reflect a liability to spend money on food or the ‘all or nothing’ thoughts (I’ve taken a bite/blown my diet, so I may as well binge) that characterize disinhibition (Gendall et al., 1998). With regard to the cognitive and behavioural characteristics, we found, as expected, that eating induced by emotions or external stimuli is more commonly reported by bulimic than by anorectic patients, and that, among the anorectic patients, emotional eating is more common among the purging type than the restricting anorectics. The finding that mean scores on restrained eating are higher among the purging than among the restricting anorectic patients was not expected. This can however, be explained by the lower drive for thinness in the latter group. Clinical experience indeed suggests that the wish to eat as little as possible is more consciously present among girls who show loss of control over eating. Purely anorectic patients commonly do not even experience the fact that they eat so little. The more outspoken body dissatisfaction among bulimic when compared to anorectic patients is probably related to this finding. Clinical experience and the scarce literature on the subject suggest a difference in the meaning of dieting between restricting and bulimic patients. The former group of patients may regard dieting as an achievement, like doing something that others are not able to do, while the motive for dieting among the latter group of patients may include a wish to be reinforced as a person by having an attractive body. Among anorectic patients the denial of bodily needs is more prominent than dissatisfaction with their appearance. Due to a similar dissatisfaction bulimic patients experience an increasing wish for slenderness following loss of control, based on the fear of not being regarded as beautiful (conform social pressure), and extremely afraid of being overweight (as before). Among purging anorectics, loss of control may induce feelings of guilt concerning their perceived indolence. The greater body dissatisfaction experienced by bulimics can of course, also be an expression of a low self-esteem, characterized by emotional chaos. It appears that the combination of high Harm Avoidance, compared to normals, and higher Novelty Seeking among bulimics (Waller et al., 1993), compared to anorexics, becomes manifest through the instability of affects and impulses, as seen in bulimics (Humphrey, 1991). In case of the anorexics, Strober (1991) postulated in a similar way that the “core of Anorexia Nervosa lies in genotypic personality structures that predispose…to rigid…avoidance behaviours with marked obsessional, anxious-depressive colouring” (Steiger & Séguin, 1999). As described above, the implications for treatment and prevention are an important impetus for the study of personality characteristics in eating disordered patients. Cloninger and colleagues (1993) have described therapeutic implications of their psychobiological model by stating that, assuming that character and temperament involve concept-based and percept-driven memory, respectively, stable personality change probably requires that conceptual insights modify habits by discipline practice, perhaps facilitated by combined pharmacotherapy. Personality change has both rational and emotional components (Cloninger, et al., 1993). This means that behavioural and cognitive techniques, aiming at a normalization of eating patterns and at an increase in self-esteem and in social skills by promoting conflict-handling and problem solving capacities have their place in cognitive behavioural treatment of eating disorders, if they include perceptual (i.e. temperamental) and conceptual (i.e. character-based) learning processes. By including the potential of perceptual learning, i.e., repeated exposure in combination with control, predictability and efficacy could thus be effective components of cue exposure to binge food, or systematic desensitisation using lists of forbidden food. As indicated by Cloninger and colleagues (1993), combined pharmacotherapy could well have a facilitating effect on such learning procedures. As biological underpinnings of several of these dimensions have been demonstrated, including associations between novelty seeking and the dopaminergic system, and between harm avoidance and the serotonergic system (Cloninger, 1987b), eating disordered patients could thus benefit from such pharmacological facilitative effects. The benefits of serotonergic drugs in eating disordered patients have indeed been demonstrated (Mayer & Walsh, 1998), although the mechanism of action is not yet clear. The results of the present study suggest that personality dimensions should be included as outcome parameters in psychopharmacological studies in ED patients. The role of drugs targeting the dopaminergic system in the treatment of eating disorders is yet to be established, and particularly their effect on cognitive and behavioural characteristics as identified in this study. References APA: Anonymous. (2001). Deriving behavioural phenotypes in an international, multi-centre study of eating disorders. Psychological Medicine, 31(4): 635-645. Beumont, P.J.V., George, G.C.W., Smart, D.E. (1976). ‘Dieters’ and ‘vomiters and purgers’ in anorexia nervosa. Psychological Medicine , 6, 617-622. Brewerton, T.D., Dorn, L.J., Bishop, E.R. (199). The Tridimensional Personality Questionnaire in eating disorder patients. Biological Psychiatry , 31, 91A. Brewerton, T.D., Hand, L.D., Bishop, E.R. (1993). The Tridimensional Personality Questionnaire in eating disorder patients. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 14, 213-218. Bruch, H. (1978). The golden cage. Cambridge: Harvard University Press. Bulik, C.M., Beidel, D.C., Duchmann, E., Weltzin, T.E., Kaye, W.H. (1992). Comparative psychopathology of women with bulimia nervosa and obsessive-compulsive disorder. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 33, 262-268. Bulik, C.M., Sullivan, P.,F., Weltzin, T.E., Kaye, W.H. (1995). Temperament in Eating Disorders. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 17 (3), 251-261. Casper, R.C. (1990). Personality features of women with good outcome from restricting anorexia nervosa. Psychosomatic Medicine, 52, 156-170. Casper, R.C., Hedeker, D., McKlough, J.F. (1992). Personality dimensions in eating disorders and their relevance for subtyping. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 31, 830-840. Cloninger, C.R. (1986). A unified biosocial theory of personality and its role in the development of anxiety states. Psychiatric Developments, 3, 167-226. Cloninger, C.R. (1987a) . Neurogenetic adaptive mechanisms in alcoholism. Science, 236, 410-416. Cloninger, C.R. (1987b). A systematic method for clinical description and classification of personality variants. Archives of General Psychiatry, 44, 573-588. Cloninger, C.R, Svrakic, D.M., Przybeck, R. (1993). A psychobiological model of temperament and character. Archives of General Psychiatry, 50, 975-990. Demitrack, M.A., Lesem, M.D., Listwak,, S.J., Brandt, H.A., Jimerson, D.C., Gold, P.W. (1990). CSF oxytocin in anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa: clinical and pathophysiologic consideration. American Journal of Psychiatry, 147, 882-886. Diaz-Marsa, M., Carrasco, J.L., Saiz, J. (2000). A study of temperament and personality in anorexia and bulimia nervosa. Journal of Personal Disorders, 14(4), 352-359. Duijsens, I.J., Spinhoven, P., Goekoop, J.G., Spermon, T., Eurelings-Bontekoe, E.H.M. (2000). The Dutch temperament and character inventory (TCI): dimensional structure, reliability and validity in a normal and psychiatric outpatient sample. Personality and Individual Differences, 28, 487-499. Garner, D.M., Olmstead, M.P., Polivy, J. (1983). The Eating Disorders Inventory: A measure of cognitive-behaviroural dimensions of anorexia and bulimia. In Anorexia Nervosa: Recent developments in research (ed. PL Darby), pp. 173-184. New York: Alan R. Liss. Gendall, K.A., Joyce, P.R., Sullivan, P.F., Bulik, C.M. (1998). Personality and dimensions of dietary restraint. International Journal of Eating Disorder, 24(4), 371-9. Giel R, Nienhuis FJ. 1996. Vragenschema’s voor de klinische beoordeling in de neuropsychiatrie. World Health Organisation Mental Health, Geneva. Halmi, K.A., Sunday, S.R., Strober, M., Kaplan, A., Woodside, D.B, Fichter, M., Treasure, J., Berrettini, W.H., Kay, W.H. (2000). Perfectionism in anorexia nervosa: variation by clinical subtype, obsessionality, and pathologic eating behaviour. American Journal of Psychiatry, 157(11), 1799-1805. Humphrey, L.L. (1991). Object relations and the family system: An integrative approach to understanding and treating eating disorders. In Psychodynamic treatment of anorexia nervosa and bulimia (ed. C Johnson), pp. 322-353. New York: Guilford Press. Kaye, W.H., Gwirtsman, H.E., George, D.T., Ebert, M.H. (1991). Altered serotonin activity in anorexia nervosa after longterm weight restoration. Archives of General Psychiatry, 48, 556-562. Kaye, W.H. (1997). Anorexia nervosa, obsessional behavior, and serotonin. Psychopharmacological Bulletin, 33(3), 335-344. Kaye, W.H., Greeno, C.G., Moss, H., Fernstrom, J., Fernstrom, M., Lilenfeld, L.R., Weltzin, T.E., & Mann, J.J. (1998). Alterations in serotonin activity and psychiatric symptomatology after recovery from bulimia nervosa. Archives of General Psychiatry, 55, 927-935. Kaye, W.H., Strober, M., Stein, D., Gendall, K. (1999). New Directions in Treatment Research of Anorexia and Bulimia Nervosa. Biological Psychiatry, 45: 1285-1292. Kaye, W.H., Strober, M. (2003). Neurobiology of eating disorders. In D. Charney, E., E. Nestler, & W. Bunney (Eds.), Neurobiological Foundations of Mental Illness. Oxford: Oxford University Press (in press). Kaye, W.H., Frank, G.K., Meltzer, C.C., Price, J.C., McConaha, C.W., Crossan, P.J., Klump, K.L., Rhodes, L. (2001). Altered serotonin 2A receptor activity in women who have recovered from bulimia nervosa. American Journal of Psychiatry, 158(7): 1152-1155. Kleifield, E.I., Sunday, S., Hurt, S.W., Halmi, K.A. (1993). Psychometric validation of the Tridimensional Personality Questionnaire: Application to subgroups of eating disorders. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 34 (4): 249-253. Lilenfeld, L.R., Kaye, W.H., Strober, M. (1997). Genetics and family studies of anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa. In D. C. Jimerson, & W.H. Kaye (Eds.), Bailliere’s Clinical Psychiatry. Eating Disorders (Vol. 3 pp.177-197). London: Bailliere Tindall. Lilenfeld, L.R., Kaye, W.H. (1998). Genetic Studies of Anorexia and Bulimia Nervosa. In H.W. Hoek, J.L. Treasure, & M.A. Katzman (Eds), Neurobiology in the Treatment of Eating Disorders. Chichester: Wiley & Sons. Mayer, L.E.S., Walsh, B.T. (1998). Pharmacotherapy of eating disorders. In H.W. Hoek, J.L. Treasure, & M.A. Katzman (Eds), Neurobiology in the Treatment of Eating Disorders. Chichester: Wiley & Sons. Pollice, C.P., Kaye, W.H., Greeno, C.G., Weltzin, T.E. (1997). Relationship of depression, anxiety, and obsessionality to state of illness in anorexia nervosa. International Journal of Eating Disorder, 21, 367-376. Pryor, T., Wiederman, M.W. (1996). Measurement of nonclinical personality characteristics of women with anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa. Journal of Personality Assessment, 67, 414-421. Soubrie, P. (1986). Reconciling the role of central serotonin neurosis in human and animal behavior. Behavioural Brain Science , 9, 319-363. Spoont, M.R. (1992). Modulatory role of serotonin in neural information processing: implications for human psychopathology. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 330-350. Srinivasagan, N.M., Kaye, W.H., Plotnicov, K.H., Greeno, C., Weltzin, T.E., & Rao, R. (1995). Persistent perfectionism, symmetry, and exactness in anorexia nervosa after long-term recovery. American Journal of Psychiatry, 152, 1630-1634. Steiger, H., Séguin, J.R. (1999). Eating disorders : anorexia and bulimia nervosa. In T. Millon, & P.H. Blaney (Eds), Oxford textbook of psychopathology (pp. 365-389). Toronto: Oxford University Press. Strober, M. (1980). Personality and symptomatological features in young, nonchronic anorexia nervosa patients. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 24, 353-359. Strober, M. (1991). Disorders of the self in anorexia nervosa: An organismic-developmental paradigm. In C. Johnson (Ed.), Psychodynamic treatment of anorexia nervosa and bulimia (pp. 354-373). New York: Guilford Press. Strober, M. (1995). Family-genetic perspectives on anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa. In K.D. Brownell, & C.G. Fairburn (Eds), Eating Disorders and Obesity (pp. 212-218). New York: The Guilford Press. Svrakic DM, Whitehead C, Przybeck TR, Cloninger CR. 1993. Differential diagnosis of personality disorders by the seven factor model of temperament and character. Archives of General Psychiatry 50, 991-999. Tellegen, A. (1985). Structures of mood and personality and their relevance to assessing anxiety with an emphasis on selfreport. In A.H. Tuma, & J.D. Maser (Eds), Anxiety and anxiety disorders (pp. 681-706). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Van Strien, T.(1986). Eating behaviour, personality traits and body mass. Swets & Zeitlinger B.V.: Lisse Vitousek, K., & Manke, F. (1994). Personality variables and disorders in anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103 (1), 137-47. Waller, D.A., Gullion, C.M., Petty, F., Hardy, B.W., Murdock, M.V., & Rush, A.J. (1993). Tridimensional Personality Questionnaire and serotonin in bulimia nervosa. Psychiatry Research, 48 (1), 9-15. 4. Personality-Related Characteristics in Restricting versus Binging and Purging Eating Disordered Patients7 Abstract The current study sought to examine differences in personality-related characteristics between restricting anorexia nervosa (RAN), binging/purging anorexia (BPAN), and purging bulimia nervosa (BN) patients. Scores on the Temperament and Character Inventory (TCI) and on the Dutch Eating Behaviour Questionnaire (DEBQ) were compared for 166 RAN, 78 BPAN and 151 BN patients. Bingers/purgers showed a higher score for restrained, external and emotional eating and a lower score for a subscale of ‘selfdirectedness’ compared to patients who never binge and purge. Personality differences were most marked between RAN and BN patients, while personality characteristics of BPAN patients tended to be similar to those of BN patients. BN patients showed higher scores on ‘novelty seeking’ and the subscales ‘exploratory excitability’, ‘impulsivity’ and ‘extravagance’, and lower scores on ‘self-directedness’, compared to restricting patients. Moreover RAN patients also showed higher scores on subscales of character dimensions, compared to BN patients. The higher the scores on novelty seeking, the lower the scores on several character scales, which has important implications for the reinforcement of behaviour and the treatment of eating disorders. According to current diagnostic nosological convention, eating disorders comprise two major categories, i.e. anorexia nervosa (AN) and bulimia nervosa (BN) (DSM-IV, 1994). However an alternative diagnostic approach has been proposed, which distinguishes between eating disordered patients who restrict food intake without binging and purging (i.e. always anorexic) and those who binge and purge (i.e. sometimes anorexic, sometimes bulimic; DaCosta & Halmi, 1992). This distinction between restrictors and bingers/purgers is thought to correspond to important phenomenological and aetiological differences, including those related to predominant personality features, family-interaction styles, neurobiological abnormalities, and patterns of genetic transmission. Such differences imply that the restrictor versus binger/purger distinction may delineate subgroups of eating disorders in a more meaningful way with regard to treatment and prevention than the distinction between AN and BN (Steiger & Séguin, 1999). There is a paucity of studies of a distinction between restrictors and bingers/purgers based on personality characteristics which could however lead to innovative treatment approaches. Vervaet M, van Heeringen C, & Audenaert K (2004). Personality-related characteristics in restricting versus binging and purging eating disordered patients. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 45 (1), 37-43. Conventional treatments for bulimic syndromes usually include cognitive-behavioural elements that encourage patients to decrease attitudinal and behavioural restraints upon eating behaviour. As in the restraint theory, the assumption is that this will ultimately reduce the pressure to binge (Fairburn et al., 1993b). Indeed, a substantial number of controlled trials have shown that such interventions are effective (Laessle, Zoettle & Pirke, 1987; Fairburn et al., 1993a; Garner et al., 1993; Mitchell et al., 1993). On the other hand, other studies have consistently found that bulimic symptoms in highly impulsive bulimics can hardly be influenced by treatment (Keel & Mitchell, 1997). In these individuals, the urge to binge may indeed be attributable rather to generalized impulse dysregulation than to the effects of dietary overcontrol (Lacey, 1984; Vitousek & Manke, 1994; Yates et al., 1989, Steiger et al., 1999). The study of Van Strien (1997) among highly restrained eaters resulted in a similar way in a distinction between patients with low and those with high susceptibility toward failure of restraint, the differences being associated with many features of psychopathology. As Van Strien (2000b) explained, the escape theory of eating (Heatherton & Baumeister, 1991) incorporates elements of two major theories about the aetiology of binging: the externality theory (Schachter & Rodin, 1974) and the psychosomatic theory (Bruch, 1973). According to the escape theory, a shift to low levels of thinking will result in a reliance on immediate stimuli, as well as a dampening of affect. Following escapes from self-awareness, eating and a reduced affect co-occur (Heatherton & Baumeister, 1991). It thus appears that the occurrence of binging or, on the contrary, sustaining restricting is associated with personality characteristics. This study aimed at further exploring the effect of personality characteristics on the occurrence of binging/purging in eating disorder patients. Methods Sample Five hundred twenty-seven patients who were consecutively referred to the inpatient and outpatient units of the Centre for Eating Disorders at the University Department of Psychiatry of the Ghent University Hospital between December 1998 and January 2003 and who met the DSM-IV criteria for Eating Disorders were enrolled in the study. The assessments were done during the first week following referral before start of the treatment. Research was compliant with the Code of Ethics of the World Medical Association (Declaration of Helsinki) and performed following the guidelines of the local ethics committee. The referred group included 244 (46 %) anorectic patients of whom 166 were restrictor-type anorectics (RAN), while 78 were purgers/bingers (BPAN). Among the bulimic patients (B=179; 34 %), 151 were purgers/bingers (BN) and 28 were non-purging type bulimics. The group of patients diagnosed with eating disorders not otherwise specified (EDNOS) contained 104 (20 %) patients of whom 38 met the criteria for binge eating disorder (BED). The current study focused on patients of the RAN, BPAN and BN group, with respectively 3 and 7 males in the RAN and in the BN group. All bulimic/purging patients met the criteria of binging (large amounts of food with a sense of lack of control) and purging (self-induced vomiting or the misuse of laxatives, diuretics, or enemas) at least twice per week for three months. In the RAN group, only 115 patients reported to never binge or purge, while 51 patients reported some bulimic and/or purging behaviour, but with a lower frequency than twice a week or involving smaller, and thus subjective binges. This latter group is described in this paper as the RAN* group. Measures Weight and height, highest and lowest adult weight, and duration of illness were assessed carefully. The body mass index (BMI) was calculated, as well the highest and lowest lifetime BMI (HBMI, LBMI), based on the highest and lowest adult weight (16 years). HBMI and LBMI can thus reflect an underestimation as current lengths were used to calculate these indexes; patients were not reliable in remembering their precise length at the moment of their highest and lowest weight. Patients were asked to describe the frequency of their binging, purging and restricting eating according to three categories: never, more than two times per week, or less than two times per week. Binging was described as losing control over eating, not including any measurement of calories, but as ‘involving larger amounts of food than most people would eat during a similar period of time and under similar circumstances’. The Dutch version of the Schedules for Clinical Assessment in Neuropsychiatry (SCAN) was used as a structured clinical interview to assign a diagnosis according to DSM-IV criteria (Giel & Nienhuis, 1996). The validated Dutch version of the Temperament and Character Inventory (TCI; Duysens et al., 2000) was used to assess temperament and character characteristics. The TCI is based on a psychobiological model of personality and assesses both temperament and character. Temperamental aspects of personality are (at least partially) heritable, manifest early in life, and involve preconceptual or unconscious biases in learning (Cloninger et al., 1993). The temperament traits include “novelty seeking” reflecting behavioural activation, “harm avoidance”, a tendency toward behavioural inhibition, “reward dependence” reflecting behavioural attachment, and “persistence”, a measure of behavioural maintenance. Characterologic aspects of personality are denoted by individual differences in selfconcepts, goals, and values. The character traits are “self-directedness”, referring to a self-accepting, purposeful nature, “cooperativeness”, a trait associated with acceptance of other people and compassion, and “self-transcendence” reflecting spirituality and an acceptance, identification, or spiritual union with nature and its source. Individuals with the same temperament may behave differently as a result of differences in character development (Cloninger et al., 1994). The Dutch Eating Behaviour Questionnaire (DEBQ; Van Strien, 1986) was used to assess the behavioural characteristics “emotional eating” (eating elicited by diffuse and clear emotional stimuli), “restrained eating” (the tendency to diet in order to lose weight), and “external eating” (eating elicited by external stimuli). Statistics Statistical analysis was performed using SPSS 10.0 (Spss Inc., Chicago, IL). One-way analysis of variance ANOVA comparison was used to compare the three diagnostic subtypes: RAN, BPAN, BN. When significant, a post hoc Bonferroni comparison was used with alpha set at 0.05. comparison between two specific subtypes, the t test for independent samples was used. For the Results As shown in Table 1, restricting and binging/purging patients differed on a number of behavioural characteristics and weight variables. Table 1: Age, weight-related characteristics, duration of illness, behavioural and personalityrelated characteristics in eating disorder patients, by type of eating disorder1 Binging/ F p Restricting Purging PurgingBonf post hoc Anorexia Anorexia Bulimia comparison RAN BPAN BN N=166 N=78 N=151 Age 21.5 24.3 22.7 3.6 .027 RAN < BPAN 7.6 7.8 7.1 BMI 14.8 15.7 20.7 326.5 .000 RAN < BPAN < BN 1.5 1.1 3.4 HBMI ever had 20.3 22.4 24.5 50.2 .000 RAN < BPAN < BN 2.9 4.1 4.0 LBMI ever had 14.1 14.8 18.4 110.0 .000 RAN, BPAN < BN 1.7 1.6 2.4 Duration of illness 3.0 5.4 3.8 5.0 .007 RAN < BPAN 5.3 6.1 4.9 Restrained eating 3.7 4.1 4.1 6.0 .003 RAN < BPAN, BN 1.2 1.0 0.9 External eating 2.9 3.3 3.7 32.5 .000 RAN < BPAN < BN 0.9 1.1 0.8 Emotional eating 2.2 3.2 3.8 107.2 .000 RAN < BPAN < BN (total) 1.0 1.1 0.9 Novelty seeking (NS) 15.9 17.0 19.8 13.2 .000 RAN, BPAN < BN 6.5 6.8 6.9 Exploratory 4.8 4.8 5.6 3.6 .027 RAN < BN excitability (NS1) 2.8 2.5 2.6 Impulsiveness (NS2) 3.2 3.5 4.3 7.1 .001 RAN < BN 2.4 2.5 2.5 Extravagance (NS3) 3.3 3.8 5.0 17.1 .000 RAN, BPAN < BN 2.4 2.4 2.5 Selfdirectedness (SD) 23.3 21.6 20.2 6.5 .002 BN < RAN 7.7 8.0 7.1 Enlightened Second 6.6 5.5 4.9 18.1 .000 BN, BPAN < RAN Nature (SD5) 2.7 2.7 2.2 Helpfulness (C3) 6.5 6.1 6.1 3.6 .029 1.3 1.5 1.5 Compassion (C4) 8.0 7.8 7.0 5.7 .004 BN < RAN 2.2 2.8 3.0 Transpersonal 3.0 2.7 2.4 3.5 .032 BN < RAN Identification (ST2) 2.1 2.4 2.0 NOTE: Values are mean (SD). Only characteristics with significant differences between subtypes are included in the table; * ANOVA with Bonferoni post hoc comparison, with level of significance set at P < .05 Characteristics ED; n=395 When compared to binging/purging (BPAN and BN) patients, RAN had a lower BMI and lifetime HBMI, and showed a significant lower score on restrained, external and emotional eating. RAN and BPAN patients did not differ with regard to scores on personality dimensions, except for a higher score on the “Enlightened Second Nature” subscale of the character dimension “self-directedness”. Scores on personality dimensions of the BPAN patients tended to be more similar to those of BN patients than to those of AN patients. BPAN patients differed from BN patients only by having a lower score on the dimension “novelty seeking” and its subscale “extravagance”. Differences on personality dimensions between RAN patients and BN patients were more marked. Patients of the RAN group showed a comparatively lower score on the dimension “novelty seeking” and nearly all its subscales, e.g. “exploratory excitability”, “impulsiveness” and “extravagance”, and a higher score on the dimension self-directedness and its subscale “enlightened second nature”. In addition, the comparison between these two groups revealed higher scores on the “helpfulness” and “compassion” subscales of the cooperativeness dimension and for the “transpersonal identification” subscale of self-transcendence. When comparing RAN and BPAN patients, a significant difference was found for age and duration of illness. Patients in the latter group were older and reported a longer duration of illness. A comparison of the characteristics of the RAN* group and those of the other patients groups revealed a number of differences. When compared to RAN patients, RAN* patients reported a longer duration of illness (RAN: mean 2.4, SD 5.0; RAN*: mean 4.4, SD 5.8; P = .030), a higher score on the “extravagance” subscale of “novelty seeking” (RAN: mean 3.0, SD 2.2; RAN*: mean 4.2, SD 2.6; P = .003), and a lower score on the “self-acceptance” subscale of “self-directedness” (RAN: mean 6.6, SD 2.6; RAN*: mean 5.6, SD 2.6; P = .029). When compared to BPAN patients, RAN* patients showed differences on behavioural and weight-related characteristics (see Table 2). Table 2: Significant difference in age, weight-related characteristics, duration of illness, behavioural and personality-related characteristics in anorectic patients by difference in frequency and/or amount of binging and purging Characteristics Restricting Binging/ t Anorexia with Purging Some binging and Anorexia purging RAN* N=51 BMI HBMI ever had LBMI ever had External eating Emotional eating (total) 15.1 1.5 20.6 3.0 14.1 2.0 3.0 1.0 2.3 1.0 BPAN N=78 15.7 1.1 22.4 4.1 14.8 1.6 3.3 1.1 3.2 1.1 p Bonf post hoc comparison 2.5 .012 RAN* < BPAN 2.6 .010 RAN* < BPAN 2.2 .026 RAN*, BPAN 2.2 .032 RAN *< BPAN 4.7 .000 RAN *< BPAN NOTE. Values are mean (SD). * t-test with independent variables P < .05. Finally, when comparing the characteristics of the “pure” RAN patients (i.e. those without any binging and/or purging) with those of the BPAN and BN patients, similar differences were found, except for a significant higher score (P = .047) for the dimension “persistence” in patients of the RAN group (mean 5.8, SD 1.9) compared to patients of the BN group (mean 5.2, SD 1.9), and a higher score (P = .012) for the subscale ‘”self-acceptance” (SD4) for this pure RAN group (mean 6.6, SD 2.6) compared with patients of the BN group (mean 5.6, SD 2.6). Discussion This study aimed at investigating an alternative approach to the classification of eating disorders, based on behavioural characteristics and their association with dimensions of personality. The findings from this study indeed indicate that a distinction between restricting and binging/purging eating disordered patients parallels behavioural and personality-related characteristics. These findings may lead to a better understanding of eating disorders, and thus to innovative approaches to their treatment and prevention. AN patients as a group differed from BN patients only in having a lower adult BMI and in lower scores on external eating, emotional eating and the temperamental dimension “novelty seeking”. However, the subdivision of eating disordered patients in general, and AN patients in particular, on the basis of their eating behavior revealed a number of important additional behavioral and personalityrelated differences. When compared to binging/purging patients, restricting patients showed lower scores on external eating and emotional eating, and had a lower current BMI, while their highest BMI ever was lower than that in binging/purging patients. Among AN patients, a binging/purging eating pattern was associated not only with being older and having a longer duration of illness, but also with higher scores on emotional eating and external eating and on a subscale of the character dimension selfdirectedness, when compared to RAN. Moreover, BPAN had a higher BMI at the moment of the assessment and a higher BMI ever than RAN. Differences on personality characteristics were most outspoken between RAN and BN patients: bulimic patients showed relatively higher scores on “novelty seeking” and most of its subscales, and lower scores on “self-directedness” and its subscale “enlightened second nature”, lower scores on the “helpfulness” and “compassion” subscales of “cooperativeness”, and finally a lower score on the “transpersonal identification” subscale of the dimension “self-transcendence”. When a subgroup of RAN patients who showed limited binging/purging behaviour was compared to purely restricting anorectic patients, it was shown that even this limited binging/purging behaviour was associated with personality characteristics, i.e. with higher scores on the “extravagance” subscale of “novelty seeking” and with lower scores on “self-directedness”. Preceding a discussion of these findings, a number of limitations of this study should be noted. First, there was no accounting for an effect of malnutrition. Mood (Smoller et al., 1987; Wing et al., 1984) and personality (Keys et al., 1950) may indeed be influenced by malnutrition, although this was not confirmed in other studies (Tuschl et al., 1989; Koeppl et al., 1992; Klump et al., 2000). Second, scores on personality dimensions may be influenced by mood. Particularly harm avoidance scores covary with mood (Svrakic et al, 1992). The results of this study suggest that a diagnostic classification of eating disorders based on a behavioural distinction (i.e. restricting versus binging/purging) is, at the least, as useful as the current categorisation approach (i.e. AN, restricting and binging/purging subtypes, v BN) for the understanding and treatment of eating disorders. The usefulness of a delineation of subtypes of anorectic patients has, however, been questioned because of ambiguous diagnostic differences in early studies and possible changes in subgroup status, as predominant symptoms may change over time (Pryor, Wiederman, & McGilley, 1996; Eddy et al., 2002). Although cross-sectional in nature, the current study provides some support for this latter argument. When compared to RAN patients, BPAN patients were older and reported a longer duration of illness. Moreover, RAN* patients shared a substantial number of cognitive, behavioural and personality-related characteristics with PBANs. Patients may thus move along a continuum from a purely restricting type to a binging/purging type of anorexia nervosa. The current data suggest that novelty seeking is involved in the evolution from anorexia nervosa with limited binging/purging to higher frequencies (more than 2 times a week as in BPAN patients). Indeed, while no significant difference in novelty seeking scores between RAN and BPAN was found, RAN* patients had a higher score on a subscale of novelty seeking than purely RAN. This score was not significantly different from that in BPAN patients, which suggests that relatively higher scores on novelty seeking (or its subscales), in combination with low scores on character dimensions such as self-directedness, among RAN may predispose them to develop the binging purging type of AN. The current findings indeed indicate that binging/purging is associated with a relatively lower score on (subscales of) selfdirectedness, cooperativeness and self-transcendence, with higher scores on behavioural characteristics such as emotional and external eating, and, consequently, with a higher current and lifetime BMI. Thus it appears that the higher the score on novelty seeking is, the higher the probability of failure of selfcontrol with increased restrained, emotional and external eating as a consequence. These findings are in keeping with the results of previous studies. More pronounced impulsivity in bulimic patients compared to patients with other eating disorders was found in a number of studies (Diaz-Marsa, Carrasco & Saiz, 2000; Nagata et al., 2000; Steiger et al., 2001; Fassino, et al., 2002). The association between novelty seeking and disinhibition, as assessed by external and emotional eating in this study, is in keeping with findings in female BN patients (Brewerton, Hand & Bishop, 1993; Kleifield et al., 1994; Bulik et al., 1995) and restrained binge eaters (Edwards & Nagelberg, 1986). Another study showed that RAN indeed differed notably from purging AN and BPAN by having a low score on novelty seeking (Klump et al., 2000). Poor impulse control, excitability, and the desire for sensory stimulation are likely to be counterproductive in the maintenance of inhibition over eating, particularly when confronted with palatable food, in the context of dysphoric mood or alcohol intoxication. The tendency toward extravagance, a novelty seeking subscale, may reflect a liability to spend money on food or to the ‘all or nothing’ thoughts (such as “I’ve taken a bite/blown my diet, so I may as well binge”), which characterize disinhibition (Gendall et al., 1998). The finding that novelty seeking may play an important role in the disability to sustain dietary restraint can be explained in biological and cognitive-behavioural ways. Steiger and collegues (1999) postulated in their study that highly impulsive binge eaters suffer from a syndrome due to a marked serotonergic (5-HT) dysregulation, and that this explains the concurrent propensities toward problems of impulse control and satiation in these patients. If outspoken, 5-HT dysregulation may be sufficient to disrupt normal satiety mechanisms, in the absence of antecedent episodes of excessive dietary restraint. Second, these authors hypothesized that individuals, once aware of an increased urge to binge, might be expected to increase dietary control in an effort to maintain control over impending dysregulation. In other words, people may become more vigilant about eating in an attempt to keep the mounting impulse to eat under control. An increasing urge to binge in these individuals may have been balanced by no corresponding increase in inhibitory controls (i.e., dietary restraint). Because character is believed to develop during life in contrast with temperament (Cloninger et al., 1993), individuals may not be temperamentally predisposed towards dietary restraint, but rather ‘learn’ or become susceptible to restrained eating patterns as a function of their character development, and attempt to control their emotions. Dependent of their temperament dimension “novelty seeking”, they may or may not sustain this restraint. In the absence of impulsivity, as in restrictors, refraining from eating is rewarding, since their negative emotions can be denied. Due to a deficient impulse control in bulimics and owing to the rewarding effects of binging and purging, these eating patterns become strategies to cope with tension. As known already from the early laboratory work of Herman and colleagues in the 1970s and 1980s there indeed is evidence for a link between dietary restraint and binge eating (Herman & Polivy, 1975, 1980, 1988, 1990). However, several studies have shown that dieting is not a necessary condition for binge eating to occur in patients with either BN (Bulik, Sullivan, Carter, & Joyce, 1997; Mussell et al., 1997) or BED (Abbott et al., 1998; Berkowitz, Stunkard, & Stallings, 1993; Grilo & Masheb, 2000; Spitzer, Devlin, Walsh, & Hasin, 1992; Spitzer, Stunkard, et al., 1993; Spitzer, Yanovski, et al., 1993; Spurrel, Wilfley, Tanofsky, & Brownell, 1997; Wilson, Nonas, & Rosenblum, 1993). Other studies found that violations of dietary restraint do not necessarily lead to binge eating in normal weight women (Charnock, 1989) and obese subjects (Ruderman & Wilson, 1979; Spencer & Fremouw, 1979). In addition to the argued notion of purging as a reinforcer of binge eating (by decreasing anxiety about weight gain), other explanations are possible. The suggestion of Byrne and McLean (2002) that the initial impetus for the binge-purge cycle may come from purging rather than from binge eating was in keeping with the results of previous studies (Leitenberg, Gross, Peterson, & Rosen, 1984; Leitenberg & Rosen, 1989; Leitenberg, Rosen, Gross, Nudelman, & Vara, 1988; Rosen & Leitenberg, 1982, 1985, 1988). According to this hypothesis purging triggers the bulimic cycle, because purging can control anxiety about weight gain, and this removes inhibitions against a binge. Finally, our findings suggest that restraining and binging are associated in case of a low score on novelty seeking. Moreover, the higher the novelty seeking, the lower the self-directedness and the more restraining, binging and purging. In that way, impulsive bulimics may show poor response to interventions which focused only on relaxing dietary restraint, because such treatments target a dimension, that is only peripheral to the maintenance of such individuals’ binge eating behaviours. Interventions that target impulsivity (and its implications upon eating behaviours) may thus be pertinent. In view of the above mentioned roles of 5-HT and impulse control, we suggest (a) adjunctive pharmacotherapy (Brewerton, 1995) with anti-impulsive/anti-bulimic agents (like selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors) and/or (b) interventions aimed at improving self-control as exposure and response prevention and impulse-control skills, like those suggested by Linehan and collegues (1991) for the treatment of borderline personality disorder. References Abbott DW, de Zwaan M, Mussell MP, Raymond NC, Seim HC, Crow SJ, Crosby RD, & Mitchell JE. Onset of binge eating and dieting in overweight women: Implications for etiology, associated features and treatment. J Psychos Res, 1998; 44 (3-4) : 367-374. Berkowitz R, Stunkard AJ, & Stallings VA. Binge-eating disorder in obese adolescent girls. An NY Acad of Sc 1993; 699: 200-206. Brewerton TD, Hand LD, & Bishop ER. The Tridimensional Personality Questionnaire in eating disorder patients. Int J Eat Disord 1993; 14: 213-8. Brewerton TD. Toward a unified theory of serotonin dysregulation in eating and related disorders. Psychoneuroendocr 1995; 20:561-590. Bruch H. Eating Disorders. New York: Basic Books, 1973. Bulik CM, Sullivan PF, Weltzin TE, & Kay WH. Temperament in eating disorders. Int J Eat Disord 1995; 17: 251-261. Bullik CM, Sullivan PF, Carter FA, & Joyce PR. Initial manifestations of disordered eating behaviour: Dieting versus binging. Int J Eat Disord 1997; 22: 195-201. Byrne SM, & McLean NJ. The Cognitive-Behavioral Model of bulimia nervosa: A direct evaluation. Int J Eat Disord 2002; 31: 17-31. Charnok DJK . A comment on the role of dietary restraint in the development of bulimia nervosa. Br J Cl Psych 1989; 28: 329-340. Cloninger CR, Przybeck TR, Svrakic DM, & Wetzel RD. The Temperament and Character Inventory (TCI): A guide to its development and use. St. Louis, MO: Center for Psychobiology of Personality, Washington University, 1994. Cloninger CR, Svrakic DM, & Przybeck TR. A psychobiological model of temperament and character. Arch Gen Psychiatr 1993; 50: 975-990. DaCosta M, & Halmi KA. Classification of anorexia nervosa: Question of subtypes. Int J Eat Disord, 1992; 11: 305-313. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th Ed. Washington DC: American Psychiatric Association: 1994; 539545. Diaz-Marsa M, Carrasco JL, & Saiz J. A study of temperament and personality in anorexia and bulimia nervosa. J Personal Disord 2000,14: 352-9. Duijsens IJ, Spinhoven P, Goekoop JG, Spermon T, & Eurelings-Bontekoe EHM. The Dutch temperament and character inventory (TCI): dimensional structure, reliability and validity in a normal and psychiatric outpatient sample. Pers and Ind Diff 2000;28: 487-499. Eddy KT, Keel PK, Dorer DJ, Delinsky SS, Franko DL & Herzog DB. Longitudinal comparison of anorexia nervosa subtypes. Int J Eat Disord 2002; 31: 191-201. Edwards FE, & Nagelberg DB. Personality characteristics of restrained/binge eaters versus unrestrained/nonbinge eaters. Add Beh 1986; 11: 207-211. Fairburn CG, Jones R, Peveler RC, Hope RA, O'Connor M: Psychotherapy and bulimia nervosa: longer-term effects of interpersonal psychotherapy, behavior therapy, and cognitive behavioral therapy. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1993a; 50: 419-428. Fairburn CG, Marcus M, & Wilson GT. Cognitive-behavioral therapy for binge eating and bulimia nervosa: A comprehensive treatment manual. In C. Fairburn & G.T. Wilson (Eds.): Binge eating: nature, assessment and treatment. New York: Guilford, 1993b, 361-404. Fassino S, Abbate-Daga G, Amianto F, Leombruni P, Boggio S, & Rovera GG. Temperament and character profile of eating disorders: a controlled study with the temperament and character inventory. Int J Eat Disord 2002; 32: 412-425. Garner DM, Olmstead MP & Polivy J. The Eating Disorders Inventory: A measure of cognitive-behaviroural dimensions of anorexia and bulimia. In Anorexia Nervosa: Recent developments in research (ed. P. L. Darby), pp. 173-184. New York: Alan R. Liss, 1983. Garner DM, Rockert W, Davis R, Garner MV, Olmsted MP, Eagle M: A comparison of cog-nitive-behavioral and supportive-expressive therapy for bulimia nervosa. Am J Psychiatry 1993; 150: 37-46. Garner DM. Eating Disorders Inventory-2. Professional manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources, 1990. Gendall KA, Joyce PR, Sullivan PF, & Bulik CM. Personality and dimensions of dietary restraint. Int J Eat Disord 1998; 24: 371-379. Giel R & Nienhuis FJ. Vragenschema’s voor de klinische beoordeling in de neuropsychiatrie. World Health Organisation Mental Health, Geneva, 1996. Grilo CM, & Masheb RM. Onset of dieting versus binge eating in outpatients with binge eating disorder. Int J Obes and Rel Metab Dis 2000; 24 (4): 404-409. Heatherton TF & Baumeister RF. Binge eating as escape from self-awareness. Psychol Bull 1991; 110: 86-108. Herman CP & Polivy J. Anxiety, restraint and eating behaviour. J Abn Psych 1975; 84: 666-672. Herman CP, & Polivy J. Stress-induced eating and eating-induced stress: A response to Robbins and fray. Appetite 1980; 1: 135-139. Herman CP, & Polivy J. Psychological factors in the control of appetite. In M. Winick (Ed.), Control of appetite. Current concepts in nutrition (Vol. 16, pp. 41-51). New York: Wiley, 1988. Herman CP, & Polivy J. From dietary restraint to binge eating: Attaching causes to effects. Appetite 1990; 14 (2): 123-125. Keel PK & Mitchell JE. Outcome in bulimia nervosa. Am J of Psych 1997; 154: 313-321. Keys A, Brozek J, Honschel A, Mickelson O & Taylor HL. The biology of human starvation. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press, 1950. Kleifield EE, Sunday S, Hurt S & Halmi KA. The tridimensional personality questionnaire: An exploration of personality traits in eating disorders. J of Psych Res 1994; 28: 413-423. Klump K, Bulik CM, Pollice C et al. Temperament and character in women with anorexia nervosa. J Nerv Ment Dis 2000; 188 (9): 559-567. Koeppl PM, Heller J, Bleecker ER, Meyers DA, Goldberg AP & Bleecker ML. The influence of weight reduction and exercise regimens upon the personality profiles of overweight males. J Clin Psych 1992; 48: 463-471. Lacey JH. Moderation of bulimia. J Psychosom Res 1984; 24: 259-269. Laessle RG, Zoettle C, Pirke KM: Meta-analysis of treatment studies for bulimia. Int J Eat Disord 1987; 6: 647-654. Leitenberg H, & Rosen JC. Cognitive-behavioral therapy with and without exposure plus response prevention in treatment of bulimia nervosa: comment on Agras, Schneider, Arnow, Raeburn and Telch. J Cons Clin Psych 1989; 57: 776-777. Leitenberg H, Gross J, Peterson J, & Rosen JC. Analysis of an anxiety model and the process of change during exposure plus response prevention treatment of bulimia nervosa. Beh Ther 1984; 15: 3-20. Leitenberg H, Rosen JC, Gross J, Nudelman S, & Vara LS. Exposure plus response-prevention treatment of bulimia nervosa. J Cons Clin Psych 1988; 56: 535-541. Linehan MM, Armstrong HE, Suarez A, Allmon D & Heard HL. Cognitive-behavioral treatment of chronically parasuicidal borderline patients. Arch Gen Psych 1991; 48: 1060-1064. Mitchell JE, Pyle RL, Pomeroy C, Zollman M, Crosby R, Sein H, Eckert ED, Zimmerman R: Cognitive-behavioral group psychotherapy of bulimia nervosa: importance of logistical variables. Int J Eat Disord 1993; 14: 277-287. Mussell MP, Mitchell JE, Fenna CJ, Crosby RD, Miller JP, & Hoberman HM. A comparison of onset of binge eating versus dieting in the development of bulimia nervosa. Int J Eat Disord 1997; 21: 353-360. Nagata T, Kawarada Y, Kiriike N, & Iketani T. Multi-impulsivity of Japanese patients with eating disorders : primary and secondary impulsivity. Psychiatry Res 2000; 94: 239-50. Pryor T & Wiederman MW. Measurement of nonclinical personality characteristics of women with anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa. J of Pers Assess 1967; 414-421. Rosen JC, & Leitenberg H. Bulimia nervosa: Treatment with exposure and response prevention. Beh Ther 1982; 13: 117124. Rosen JC, & Leitenberg H. Exposure plus response prevention treatment of bulimia. In D.M. Garner & P.E. Garfinkel (Eds.), A handbook of psychotherapy for anorexia nervosa and bulimia (pp. 193-209). New York: Guilford Press, 1985. Rosen JC, & Leitenberg H. The anxiety model of bulimia nervosa and treatment with exposure plus response prevention. In KM Pirke, W Vandereycken, & D Ploog (Eds.), The psychobiology of bulimia nervosa (pp. 146-151). Berlin: SpringerVerlag, 1988. Ruderman AJ, & Wilson GT. Weight, restraint, cognitions and counterregulation. Beh Res Ther 1979; 17 (6): 581-590. Schachter S & Rodin J. Obese humans and rats. Washington, DC: Erlbaum/Halsted, 1974. Smoller JW, Wadden TA & Stunkard AJ. Dieting and depression: A critical review. Journal of Psychos Res 1987; 31: 429440. Spencer JA, & Fremouw WJ. Binge eating as a function of restraint and weight classification. J Abn Psych 1979; 88: 262267. Spitzer RL, Devlin MJ, Walsh BT, Hasin D, Wing R, Marcus M, Stunkard A, Wadden T, Yanovski S, Agras S, Mitchell J, & Nonas C. Binge eating disorder: A multisided trial of the diagnostic criteria. Int J Eat Disord, 1992; 11: 191-203. Spitzer RL, Stunkard A, Yanovski S, Marcus MD, Wadden T, Wing R, Mitchell J, & Hasin D. Binge eating disorder should be included in DSM-IV: A reply to Fairburn et al.’s “the classification of recurrent overeating: The binge eating disorder proposal. Int J Eat Disord 1993; 13: 161-169. Spitzer RL, Yanovski S, Wadden T, Wing R, Marcus M, Stunkard A, Devlin MJ, Mitchell J, Hasin D, & Horne RL. Binge eating disorder: Its further validation in a multiside study. Int J Eat Disord 1993; 137-153. Spurrell EB, Wilfley DE, Tanofsky MB, & Brownell KD. Age of onset for binge eating: Are there different pathways to binge eating? Int J Eat Disord 1997; 21: 55-65. Steiger H & Séguin JR. Eating disorders : anorexia and bulimia nervosa. In: T. Millon, & P.H, 1999. Steiger H, Lehoux PM & Gauvin L. Impulsivity, dietary control and the urge to binge in bulimic syndromes. Int J Eat Disord 1999b; 26: 261-274. Steiger H, Young SN, Kin NM, Koerner N, Israel M, Lageix P, & Paris J. Implications of impulsive and affective symptoms for serotonin function in bulimia nervosa. Psychol Med 2001; 31: 85-95 Svrakic DM, Przybeck TR, & Cloninger CR. Mood states and personality traits. J Affect Dis 1992; 24:217-226. Tuschl RJ, Laessle RG, Kothaus BC & Pirke KM. Behavioral and biological correlates of restrained eating. Ann NY Acad Sc 1989; 575: 580-581. Van Strien T, Cleven A & Schippers G. Restraint, Tendency Toward Overeating and Ice Cream Consumption. Int J Eat Disord 2000a; 28: 333-338. Van Strien T, Frijters JER, Bergers GPA & Defares PB. The Dutch Eating Behaviour Questionnaire (DEBQ) for assessment of restrained, emotional and external eating behaviour. Int J Eat Disord 1986; 5: 295-315. Van Strien T. The concurrent validity of a classification of dieters with low versus high susceptibility toward failure of restraint. Add Beh 1997; 22: 587-597. Van Strien T. Ice-Cream Consumption, Tendency Toward Overeating, and Personality. Int J Eat Disord 2000b; 28: 460-464. Vitousek K & Manke F. Personality variables and disorders in anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa. J Abn Psych 1994; 103: 137-147. Wilson GT, Nonas C, & Rosenblum, GD. Assessment of binge-eating in obese patients Int J Eat Disord 1993; 13: 25-34. Wing RR, Epstein LH, Marcus MD & Kupfer DJ. Mood changes in behavioral weight loss programs. J Psychosom Res 1984; 28: 189-196. Yates WR, Sieleni B & Bowers WA. Clinical correlates of personality disorder in bulimia nervosa. Int J Eat Disord 1989; 8: 473-477. 5. Attempted suicide and personality in eating disorders8 Abstract Objective: To assess the occurrence and associated characteristics of attempted suicide in eating disorders. Method: The study group consisted of 300 ED in- and out-patients at the Department of Psychiatry (University Hospital of Ghent) in Belgium. Personality dimensions, and behavioural and cognitive characteristics were compared between ED patients with and these without a history of attempted suicide (AS).Results: A history of suicide attempts was reported by 44 (14.6 %) patients. Suicide attempts were more common among bulimia nervosa (BN, 23%) patients than in anorexia nervosa (AN, 13.5%) or EDNOS patients (7%). AS among AN patients appeared to differ from that in BN or EDNOS patients in terms of associated personality dimensions and behavioural and cognitive characteristics. Conclusion: The findings support a stress-diathesis model for attempted suicide and provide guidelines for the prevention of AS among ED patients, using a psychotherapeutic approach, perhaps facilitated by psychopharmacological strategies. Eating disorders – attempted suicide – personality dimensions Introduction Eating disorders are associated with a strongly increased mortality. The mortality rate for anorexia nervosa (AN) is 5.6% per decade, and major causes of death are starvation and suicide (Sullivan, 1995). Indeed, most (Patton, 1988; Eckert, Halmi, Marchi, Crove, & Crosby, 1995; MollerMadsen, Nystrup, & Nielsen, 1996; Nielsen, Moller-Madsen, Isager, Jorgensen, Pagsberg, & Theander, 1998; Apter & Ofek, 2001) though not all (Coren & Hewitt, 1998) studies in AN patients suggest that suicide is the main cause of increased mortality rates, with an increased risk of premature death occurring up to 8 years after the initial assessment (Patton, 1988; Nielsen et al., 1998). AN may indeed have the highest standardized mortality ratio (SMR) for suicide among all psychiatric disorders (Harris & Barraclough, 1997). The crude mortality rate in BN is lower than that in AN, ranging from 0.3 to 0.9% (Keel & Mitchel, 1997; Herzog et al, 2000). Studies of suicide in bulimia nervosa (BN) are fewer in number. A recent review of the deaths among BN patients in multiple studies showed that 29% were the result of suicide (Franko et al, 2004). 8 Vervaet M, van Heeringen C, & Audenaert K. Journal of Affective Disorders (submitted 2005). There are very few reports of (attempted) suicide among patients suffering from atypical eating disorders, i.e. eating disorders not otherwise specified (EDNOS). Fullerton and co-workers (1995) reported a prevalence rate of 23.2 %. Attempted suicide (AS) is regarded as the most robust clinical predictor of suicide (Sakinofsky, 2000), and the study of the occurrence and risk factors of AS may thus contribute to the prevention of suicide among eating disordered patients. In a large series of eating disordered outpatients, 13% reported at least one suicide attempt (Favaro & Santonastaso, 1996, 1997). A recent longitudinal study showed that 15% of ED patients reported a history of AS characterized by at least some intent to die during the 8.6-year follow-up period, including 22.1% of AN patients and 10.9% of BN patients, with severity of depressive symptoms and drug use predicting AS in AN and a history of drug use and laxative use predicting AS in BN (Franko et al, 2004). The prevalence of AS among outpatient anorexics was recently estimated at 11.3% (Sansone & Levitt, 2002). No data are available with regard to the occurrence of AS among inpatient anorexics. Bulik and colleagues (1999) found that the occurrence of AS among women with AN was independent of a lifetime history of major depression, although for bulimia, those women with comorbid depression may be at greater risk. Favaro and Santonastaso (1997) reported that anorexic subjects who attempted suicide were older, had a longer duration of illness, weighed less, and were more likely to use substances, as compared to non-attempters. In addition, Corcos and co-workers (2002) found that a history of attempted suicide among AN patients was associated with more severe depressive symptoms and impulsive behaviours. With regard to EDI-measured characteristics, AN patients attempting suicide during a follow-up period differed from those who did not attempt suicide only by a higher score on the ‘Ineffectiveness’ subscale (Franko et al, 2004). The lifetime prevalence of AS in BN has been estimated at 22%, with attempts commonly being multiple and serious (Keel, Dorer, Eddy, Franko, Charatan, & Herzog, 2003; Sansone & Levitt, 2004). Contrary to the findings from retrospective studies in AN patients (Favaro & Santonastaso, 1996; Pompili, Mancinelli, Girardi, Ruberto, & Tatarelli; 2004), the risk of AS among BN patients appeared to be independent of the presence or severity of core ED symptoms such as binging and purging, but associated with the duration of illness, more severe depressive symptoms, and impulsive behaviours including a history of drug use (Corcos, Taïeb, Benoit-Lamy, Paterniti, Jeammet, & Flament, 2002; Franko et al, 2004). With regard to EDI-measured characteristics, BN patients attempting suicide during a follow-up period differed from those who did not attempt suicide by higher scores on the ‘Ineffectiveness’, ‘Interpersonal distrust’, ‘Interoceptive Awareness’, and ‘Maturity fears’ subscales (Franko et al, 2004). These studies have thus demonstrated that suicidal behaviour is relatively common among ED patients and that its occurrence may be related to axis-I disorders, to core ED-symptoms and to personality characteristics. It has recently become clear that suicidal behaviour is to be regarded as the consequence of an interaction between stressors and a diathesis or predisposition to suicidal behaviour. These stressors may be disease-related, e.g. due to depression or other axis-I disorders. With regard to the diathesis a role of trait-dependent impulsivity and aggression has been suggested (Mann et al, 1997). Also in ED it has been suggested that among anorexic and bulimic patients suicide attempts are part of a cluster of impulsive behaviours (Franko et al, 2004). Cloninger’s TCI has been used to study trait-dependent temperamental correlates of AS, suggesting a role of increased harm avoidance and decreased reward dependence in the development of attempted suicide (Van Heeringen et al, 2000; 2003). In addition, the temperament and character traits of high persistence, low self-directedness and high self-transcendence are associated with a reported history of SA in the ED diagnostic groups (Bulik et al., 1999). Cloninger and co-workers (unpublished manuscript and cited in Bulik et al., 1999) have indicated that self-transcendence makes an important contribution to both positive and negative aspects of mental health, and that the valence of the contribution is determined by the configuration of the other character traits. For example, high selftranscendence combined with low self-directedness and low cooperativeness is associated with increased schizotypy, psychosis-proneness and depression, whereas high self-transcendence combined with high self-directedness and high cooperativeness leads to hyperthymia or cheerful creativity. The current study was undertaken in an attempt to fill a number of gaps in our knowledge of the role of personality-related risk factors for AS among ED patients. First, a majority of previous studies were conducted in the USA, due to which the definition of AS differs from the definition used in Europe. Indeed, according to US definitions AS is characterized by an intent to die, which is not the case in definitions used in European research. It is therefore unclear to what extent previous findings are applicable to European ED patients. Secondly, as little information is available on the occurrence and risk factors for AS among EDNOS patients, these patients were included in the current study. Thirdly, this study aimed at assessing whether these personality characteristics are involved in the previously described effect of ED-related cognitive and behavioural characteristics (see above) on the occurrence of AS among ED patients by studying (1) the association between AS and ED-related cognitive and behavioural characteristics, and (2) the correlation between the TCI personality traits and these cognitive and behavioural characteristics in the three types of ED. Patients and methods The study group consisted of ED patients, who were consecutively referred for inpatient or outpatient treatment to the Center for Eating Disorders of the University Department of Psychiatry at the University Hospital in Gent, Belgium. Psychiatric diagnoses were assigned according to DSM-IV criteria. A history of attempted suicide was assessed carefully. Attempted suicide was defined as an act with a non-fatal outcome, in which an individual initiates a non-habitual behaviour that will cause self harm, or deliberately ingests a substance in excess of the prescribed or generally recognized therapeutic dosage, or ingests a substance which is not meant for human consumption. Motives or suicidal intent are thus not included in the definition of attempted suicide, which is in keeping with the definition of attempted suicide as used in the “WHO/EURO Multicentre Study of Suicidal Behaviour” (Schmidtke, Weinacker, Apter, et al., 1999). Weight and height, highest and lowest lifetime weight, and duration of illness were assessed carefully. Body mass index (BMI) and the highest and lowest lifetime BMI (HBMI, LBMI) were calculated, the latter based on the highest and lowest adult weight (i.e. 16 years or older). Patients were asked to describe the frequency of their binging, purging and restricting eating patterns. Binging was defined as losing control over eating, not including any measurement of calories, but involving ‘larger amounts of food than most people would eat during a similar period of time and under similar circumstances’. The Dutch Eating Disorders Inventory (EDI; Garner, Olmstead & Polivy, 1983) was used to assess cognitive characteristics including drive for thinness, bulimia, body dissatisfaction, ineffectiveness, perfectionism, interpersonal distrust, interoceptive awareness and maturity fears. The validated Dutch version of the TCI (Duijsens, Spinhoven, Goekoop, Spermon & EurelingsBontekoe, 2000) was used to assess temperament and character dimensions. The TCI is based on a psychobiological model of personality and assesses both temperament and character. Temperamental aspects of personality are (at least partially) heritable, manifest early in life, and involve pre-conceptual or unconscious biases in learning (Cloninger, Svrakic & Przybeck, 1993). The temperament traits include novelty seeking (NS) reflecting behavioural activation, harm avoidance (HA), i.e. a tendency toward behavioural inhibition, reward dependence (RD) reflecting behavioural attachment, and persistence (PS), which is a measure of behavioural maintenance. Character dimensions are denoted by individual differences in self-concepts, goals, and values. The character traits in the TCI are self-directedness (SD), referring to a self-accepting and purposeful nature, cooperativeness (CO), a trait associated with acceptance of other people and compassion, and self-transcendence (ST) reflecting spirituality and acceptance, identification, or spiritual union with nature and its sources. Individuals with the same temperament may behave differently as a result of differences in character development (Cloninger, Przybeck, Svrakic & Wetzel, 1994). The Dutch Eating Behaviour Questionnaire (DEBQ; Van Strien, Frijters, Bergers & Defares, 1986) was used to assess the behavioural characteristics ‘emotional eating’ (eating elicited by emotional stimuli), ‘restrained eating’ (the tendency to diet in order to lose weight), and ‘external eating’ (eating elicited by external stimuli). The validated Dutch translation (Van der Does, 2002) of the Beck Depression Inventory (BDIII-NL; Beck AT, Steer RA, & Brown GK, Swets & Zeitlinger), was used to assess the severity of depressive symptoms. Results The study group consisted of 300 patients, including 96 AN patients (32 %), 104 BN patients (34.6 %), and 100 EDNOS patients (33.3 %). The patient group consisted of 290 females (96.6 %) and 10 males (3.4 %). In view of the small proportion of males, statistical analyses were performed without stratification by gender. A history of AS was reported by 44 (14.6 %) ED patients. AS was significantly more commonly reported by BN patients (n= 24; 23.1 %) than by AN patients (n= 13; 13.5 %) or EDNOS patients (n= 7; 7%; df = 2; χ² = 10.671; p = .005). In the anorectic group, no differences were found for scores on the BDI-II between patients with and those without a history of suicide attempt. BN and EDNOS patients with a history of AS had higher scores on the BDI-II (35,7 and 39,5 respectively), than those without such a history (27, 8 and 21.7; p=.007 and p = .003 respectively). As shown in Table 1, a history of AS among AN patients was associated with a higher score on novelty seeking. Patients diagnosed with BN or EDNOS with a history of AS had a significantly higher score on harm avoidance and a significantly lower score on self-directedness than those without such a history. Table 1: Scores on personality dimensions according to type of eating disorder and to history of attempted suicide Personality AN BN EDNOS dimension AS- AS+ AS- AS+ AS- AS+ Novelty seeking 14.8 19.9** 20.7 20.5 19.8 19.4 Harm avoidance 24.9 25.5 23.3 27.7** 21.8 29.7** Reward dependence 15.6 16.8 17.1 16.2 17.1 14.7 5.7 5.1 4.9 4.9 4.8 4.6 Self-directedness 22.9 19.5 20.1 13.7** 24.4 29.1 Cooperativeness 31.4 29.9 32.2 30.9 32.1 30.7 Self-transcendence 10.3 11.1 10.7 10.7 11.9 13.7 Persistence * p < .05 ** p < .001 Among ED patients with a history of AS, BN patients had significantly lower scores on SD than AN patients. No other differences between ED categories on TCI-dimensions were found among ED patients with a history of AS. ED patients without a history of AS showed more differences according to type of ED. AN patients scored significantly lower than BN and EDNOS patients on novelty seeking and reward dependence, higher than EDNOS patients on harm avoidance, and higher than BN and EDNOS patients on persistence. BN patients scored significantly lower on self-directedness than EDNOS patients. Table 2 shows the scores on cognitive and behavioural characteristics of patients according to their type of ED and to the presence or absence of a history of AS. Among the AN patients, a history of AS was not associated with cognitive and behavioural characteristics. BN patients with a history of AS showed higher scores on ineffectiveness, interoceptive awareness and maturity fears. Finally, EDNOS patients with a history of AS differed from those without such a history in that they showed relatively higher scores on drive for thinness, body dissatisfaction, ineffectiveness and perfectionism, and a lower score on eating due to external stimuli. Table 2: Scores on cognitive and behavioural characteristics according to type of ED and to history of AS Cognitive/behavioural AN BN EDNOS characteristic AS- AS+ AS- AS+ AS- AS+ drive for thinness 12.7 12.5 16.7 15.6 11.7 17.1* bulimia 1.4 2.2 11.0 12.0 4.1 3.7 body dissatisfaction 12.3 15.1 29.6 20.7 16.0 22.1* ineffectiveness 12.4 14.5 12.7 19.6** 9.4 19.6** perfectionism 6.5 6.6 5.9 8.5* 5.8 9.7* interpers distrust 6.6 6.7 4.9 7.3* 4.5 4.1 interocept. awar. 1.1 9.0 10.7 15.0** 7.7 13.3* maturity fears 6.9 7.4 5.3 9.5** 4.9 6.6 restrained eating 4.0 3.9 3.8 3.8 3.5 3.8 external eating 2.4 2.4 3.5 3.7 3.0 2.0** emotional eating 2.2 2.2 4.0 4.1 3.0 1.9* * p < .05 ** p < .001 Finally, statistical analysis pursued the study of the correlation between scores on TCIpersonality dimensions with an effect on the occurrence of AS and cognitive or behavioural characteristics in the ED categories. Among AN patients, scores on novelty seeking correlated negatively with those on ineffectiveness (r = .237; p = .020), interpersonal distrust (r = -.287; p = .005) and interoceptive awareness (r = -.230; p= .024). Table 3: Significant correlations between personality dimensions and cognitive characteristics in AN patients Ineffectiveness Novelty Seeking r (p) -.237 (.020) Interpersonal Distrust r (p) -.287 (.005) Interoceptive awareness r (p) -.230 (.024) Among BN patients, scores on harm avoidance correlated significantly with those on drive for thinness (r = .205; p = .036), body dissatisfaction (r = 321; p = .001), ineffectiveness (r = .541; p = .000), perfectionism (r = .294; p = .002), interpersonal distrust (r = .400; p = .000), interoceptive awareness (r = .425; p = .000) and maturity fears (r = .362; p = .000). Scores on self-directedness among BN patients correlated significantly with scores on bulimia (r = -.321; p = .001), ineffectiveness (r = .504; p = .000), perfectionism (r = -.361; p = .000), interpersonal distrust (r = -.409; p = .000), interoceptive awareness (r = -.375; p = .000), maturity fears (r = -.310; p = .001), external eating (r = .245; p = .012) and emotional eating (r = -.335; p = .001). Table 4: Significant correlations between personality dimensions and cognitive characteristics in BN patients DT r (p) B r (p) BD r (p) INE r (p) P r (p) IP r (p) IA r (p) MF r (p) EE r (p) EME r (p) Harm Avoidance .205 (.036) - 321 (.001) .541 (.000) .294 (.002) .400 (.000) .425 (.000) .362 (.000) - - Self directedness - -.321 (.001) - -.504 (.000) -.361 (.000) -.409 (.000) -.375 (.000) -.310 (.001) -.245 (.012) -.335 .001 DT = drive for thinness; B = bulimia; BD = body dissatisfaction; INE = ineffectiveness; P = perfectionism; IP = interpersonal distrust; IA = interoceptive awareness; MF = maturity fears; EE = external eating; EME = emotional eating Scores on harm avoidance among EDNOS patients correlated significantly with those on drive for thinness (r = .404; p = .000), bulimia (r = .279, p = .005), body dissatisfaction (r = .578; p = .000), ineffectiveness (r = .755, p = .000), perfectionism (r = .293; p = .003), interpersonal distrust (r = .436; p = .000), interoceptive awareness (r = .543, p = .000), maturity fears (r = .365, p = .000) and emotional eating (r = .312, p = .002). Finally, scores on self-directedness among EDNOS patients correlated significantly with those on drive for thinness (r = -.249; p = .013), bulimia (r = -.307; p = .002), body dissatisfaction (r = -.461; p = .000), ineffectiveness (r = -.599; p = .000), interpersonal distrust (r = -.351; p = .000), interoceptive awareness (r = -.401; p = .000), maturity fears (r = -.337; p = .001) and emotional eating (r = -.316; p = .001). Table 5: Significant correlations between personality dimensions and cognitive characteristics in EDNOS patients Harm Avoidance Self directedness DT r (p) .404 (.000) B r (p) .279 (.005) BD r (p) .578 (.000) INE r (p) .755 (.000) P r (p) .293 (.003) IP r (p) .436 (.000) IA r (p) .543 .000 MF r (p) .365 (.000) EE r (p) - EME r (p) .312 (.002) -.249 (.013) -.307 (.002) -.461 (.000) -.599 (.000) - -.351 .000) -.401 (.000) -.337 (.001) - -.316 .001 DT = drive for thinness; B = bulimia; BD = body dissatisfaction; INE = ineffectiveness; P = perfectionism; IP = interpersonal distrust; IA = interoceptive awareness; MF = maturity fears; EE = external eating; EME = emotional eating Discussion The results from this study can be summarized as follows. First, 14.6 % of this large group of eating disordered patients reports a history of attempted suicide, with a significant effect of the type of eating disorder involved, as such a history is more common among bulimia nervosa patients (23 %) than among anorexia nervosa (13 %) or atypical eating disorder patients (7 %). Secondly, a history of attempted suicide is associated with trait-dependent personality characteristics, reflecting increased novelty seeking among anorexia nervosa patients and increased levels of harm avoidance and reduced levels of self-directedness among patients suffering from bulimia nervosa or an eating disorder not otherwise specified. Thirdly, among patients with a history of suicide attempt, bulimic patients scored lower than anorectic patients on self-directedness. Finally, these personality characteristics correlate significantly with a number of eating disorder-related cognitive and behavioural characteristics, which have previously been shown to be involved in the occurrence of attempted suicide among eating disordered patients. Taken together, these findings indicate an important effect of personality characteristics on the occurrence of attempted suicide among the three major categories of eating disordered patients, and thus suggest that a stress-diathesis model can be used to explain the development of suicidal behaviour in association with eating disorders. While the ‘stress’ component of the model was not assessed in this study, the findings indicate that the ‘diathesis’ component of this model can be described in terms of personality characteristics, and that previously described risk factors for attempted suicide among eating disordered patients, such as cognitive and behavioural characteristics, actually reflect the effect of these personality characteristics. The results of the current study may have been biased by the small number of patients reporting a history of attempted suicide in the three categories of eating disorders and by its retrospective nature. However, the proportion of eating disordered patients reporting a history of attempted suicide in the current study is in keeping with that in previous retrospective studies. A recent longitudinal study, however, showed a higher rate of attempted suicide in anorexia nervosa than bulimia nervosa (Franko et al, 2004). It should be noted that attempted suicide in this prospective study was defined by at least some intent to die. Thus, while the current study suggests that risk factors for attempted suicide in terms of personality characteristics in anorexia nervosa differ from those in bulimia nervosa, these findings indicate that attempted suicide in anorexia nervosa may also differ from that in bulimia nervosa by a relatively more marked intent to die. Unfortunately, intent to die was not assessed in the current study. The current findings confirm previous reports of a common occurrence of attempted suicide among ED patients. It appears that the impact of the difference in operational definitions between Europe and the USA on prevalence rates of AS in ED is limited. It is currently not clear how this common occurrence can be explained. It has been suggested that eating disorders themselves constitute a version of self-harm and may exist along a continuum with other self-harm behaviours (Anderson & Bulik, 2004). AS and ED share a more common prevalence among females, their onset often occurs during adolescence, and both behaviours can be used as a means of taking control over one’s own body (Anderson & Bulik, 2004). However, the current study also showed that the occurrence of AS among ED patients is associated with a particular temperamental make-up, which may constitute a diathesis for attempted suicide among eating disordered patients. The diathesis among anorexia nervosa patients appears to differ from that in bulimia nervosa and EDNOS patients. More particularly, it appears that a disturbed trait-dependent regulation of anxiety, or increased behavioural inhibition, in combination with a low score on self-directedness is involved in suicidal behaviour among the latter categories of eating disorders but not in anorexia nervosa. The lower score on self-directedness indicates a reduced ability to control, regulate and adapt behaviour to fit the situation in accordance with individually chosen goals, and a limited development of skills and confidence in solving problems (Cloninger et al., 1993). The current findings indicate that increased harm avoidance and low self-directedness among eating disordered patients may become manifest through cognitive and behavioural problems including increased drive for thinness, bulimia, body dissatisfaction, ineffectiveness, perfectionism, interpersonal distrust, interoceptive awareness, maturity fears and emotional eating. While no causal interpretation of this association can be inferred from the data, the results suggest that these characteristics can be regarded as cognitive coping strategies reflecting a shift from the interpersonal to the personal domain and from a psychological to a somatic focus in an attempt to increase perceived control. A combination of low novelty seeking and high harm avoidance will lead to increased behavioral inhibition due to which behaviours will not be reinforced by external stimuli but instead be repetitive and automatic. The results suggest a pivotal role for novelty seeking. Increased levels of harm avoidance reflect behavioural inhibition, but may nevertheless be associated with cognitive and behavioural disinhibition when combined with increased levels of novelty seeking. This appears to be the case in AN patients who show increased levels of harm avoidance and, in case of a history of AS, additionally increased levels of novelty seeking. The scarce literature and case reports indeed suggest an important effect of anxiety on the development of suicidal behaviour in general, and in anorexia nervosa in particular. For example, Ellen West described the role of anxiety in the development of suicidal behaviour in association with anorexia nervosa in a compelling way in her diary (summarized by Bruch, 1979). Before committing suicide, this anorexia nervosa patient wrote that ‘the most horrible thing about my life is that it is filled with fear. Fear of eating, but also fear of hunger and fear of fear itself. Only death can liberate me from this dread’. In the same line of reasoning, Williams and Pollock (2001) have pointed at the role of Gilbert’s (1989) ethological concept of ‘arrested flight’ in the development of suicidal behaviour, describing the behaviour of a bird experiencing entrapment and the lack of escape, and the similarities between this concept and the effect of increased behavioural inhibition have been described elsewhere (Van Heeringen, 2001). It is tempting to point at the similarities between Gilbert’s description of a bird in a cage experiencing ‘arrested flight’ and Hilde Bruch’s ‘The Golden Cage’ (1979), suggesting that suicidal behaviour may indeed be the only escape from anxiety and entrapment. Taking into account the stress-diathesis model of suicidal behaviour, the current findings suggest that the anxiety involved in the development of suicidal behaviour in anorexia nervosa relates more to illnessrelated stress than to the trait-dependent diathesis for suicidal behaviour. Although more research is clearly needed to support the application of the stress-diathesis model to suicidal behaviour among eating disordered patients, a number of implications for treatment and prevention can be formulated. From a psychotherapeutic point of view, the current findings suggest that an increase in self-directedness or self-control should be an important issue in the treatment of eating disordered patients. Fairburn and colleagues (1998) have described a cognitive behavioural approach to enhance self-control in eating disordered patients. The findings from the current study suggest that such an approach is particularly important in preventing attempted suicide. From a psychopharmacological point of view, the current findings suggest a role of serotonergic agonists in the prevention of attempted suicide among eating disordered patients, particularly in patients suffering from bulimia nervosa and EDNOS in view of the role of harm avoidance in the development of attempted suicide in these types of eating disorders. Indeed, using a functional neuro-imaging approach we have recently demonstrated a reduced prefrontal binding potential of prefrontal serotonin (5-HT)-2a receptors in association with attempted suicide (Audenaert, Goethals, Van Laere, van Heeringen & Dierckx, 2002), which correlated negatively with levels of harm avoidance (Van Heeringen et al., 2003). There might, however, also be a role for these pharmacological agents in the treatment of anorexia nervosa as we could show in another study that anorexia nervosa patients have a lower 5-HT2a receptor binding in the left prefrontal cortex, in the left and right parietal cortex and the left and right occipital cortex (Audenaert et al., 2002). An association between these findings and the occurrence of attempted suicide or related cognitive and behavioural characteristics as assessed in the current study remains however to be demonstrated. Moreover, as both attempted and completed suicide appear to be, at least in part, under genetic control, further study is needed with regard to genetic influences on the occurrence of suicidal behaviour in eating disordered patients. The genetic effects of 5-HT2a receptor functioning might provide a focus for such studies. The results of studies of the effect of a 5-HT2a receptor polymorphism on the development of anorexia nervosa have not been equivocal (Ohara, Nagai, Tsukamoto, Tani, Suzuki & Ohara, 1998; Fairburn, Shafran & Cooper, 1998), but its effect on the occurrence of suicidal behaviour among eating disordered patients clearly needs more study. In conclusion, this study shows that attempted suicide is a common problem among eating disordered patients and that its occurrence is associated with personality characteristics, which in turn may become manifest as cognitive and behavioural problems related to eating disorders. The findings thus provide support for the application of a stress-diathesis model of suicidal behaviour, and indicate that the diathesis for attempted suicide among anorexia nervosa patients differs from that in patients suffering from bulimia nervosa and atypical eating disorders. References Audenaert, K., Van Laere, K., Goethals I., Lahorte, P., Brans, B., Beelaert, B., van Heeringen, C., & Dierckx, R. (2002). SPECT neuropsychological activation procedure with the verbal fluency test in depressed suicide attempters. Nuclear Medicine Communications, 23, 907-916. Bruch, H. (1979). The golden cage: the enigma of anorexia nervosa. New York, Vintage. Bulik, C., Sullivan, P.F., & Joyce, P.R. (1999). Temperament, character and suicide attempts in anorexia nervosa, bulimia nervosa and major depression. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 100, 27-32. Cloninger, C.R., Przybeck, T.R., Svrakic, D.M., & Wetzel, R.D. (1994). The Temperament and Character Inventory (TCI): A guide to its development and use. St. Louis, MO: Center for Psychobiology of Personality, Washington University. Cloninger, C.R, Svrakic, D.M, & Przybeck, T.R. (1993). A psychobiological model of temperament and character. Archives of General Psychiatry, 50, 975-990. Corcos, M., Taïb, O., Benoit-Lamy, S., Paterniti, S., Jeammet, P., & Flament, M.F. (2002). Suicide attempts in women with bulimia nervosa: frequency and characteristics. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 106, pp 381-386. Coren, S., & Hewitt, P.L. (1998). Is anorexia nervosa associated with elevated rates of suicide? American Journal of Public Health, 88, 1206-1207. Duijsens, I.J., Spinhoven, P., Goekoop, J.G., Spermon, T., & Eurelings-Bontekoe, E.H.M. (2000). The Dutch temperament and character inventory (TCI): dimensional structure, reliability and validity in a normal and psychiatric outpatient sample. Personality and Individual Differences, 28, 487-499. Eckert, E.D., Halmi, K.A., Marchi, P., Crove, E., & Crosby, R. (1995). Ten-year follow-up study of anorexia nervosa: clinical course and outcome. Psychological Medicine, 25, 143-156. Fairburn, C.G., Shafran, R., & Cooper, Z. (1998). A cognitive behavioural theory of anorexia nervosa. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 37, 1-13. Favaro, A. & Santonastaso, P. (1996). Purging behaviors, suicide attempts, and psychiatric symptoms in 398 eating disordered subjects. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 20, 99-103. Favaro, A. & Santonastaso, P. (1997). Suicidality in eating disorders: clinical and psychological correlates. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 95, 508-514. Franko DL, Keel PK, Dorer DJ, Blais MA, Delinsky SS, Eddy KT, Charat V, Renn R, & Herzog (2004). What predicts suicide attempts in women with eating disorders ? Psychological Medicine, 34, 843-853. Fullerton, D.T., Wonderlich, S.A., & Gosnell, B.A. (1995). Clinical characteristics of eating disorder patients who report sexual or physical abuse. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 17, 243-249. Garner, D.M., Olmstead, M.P, & Polivy, J. (1983). The Eating Disorders Inventory: A measure of cognitive-behaviroural dimensions of anorexia and bulimia. In: DARBY PL, ed. Anorexia Nervosa: Recent developments in research.. New York: Alan R. Liss. Gilbert, P. (1989). Human nature and suffering. Hove and London, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Keel, P.K., Dorer, D.J., Eddy, K.T., Franko, D., Charatan, D.L., & Herzog, D.B. (2003). Predictors of mortality in eating disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry, 60, pp 179-183 Mann, J.J., Waternaux, C., Malone, K., …. (1997?) Towards a clinical model of suicidal behaviour in psychiatric patients. American Journal of Psychiatry, …. Moller-Madsen, S., Nystrup, J. & Nielsen, S. (1996). Mortality in anorexia nervosa in Denmark during the period 1970-1987. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 94, 454-459. Nielsen, S., Moller-Madsen, S., Isager, T., Jorgensen, J., Pagsberg, K. & Theander, S. (1998). Standardized mortality in eating disorders: a quantitive summary of previously published studies and new evidence. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 44, 413-434. Ohara, K., Nagai, M., Tsukamoto, T., Tani, K., Suzuki, Y., Ohara, K. polymorphism – 1438G/A and mood disorders. Neuroreport, 9, 1139-1141. (1998). 5-HT2A receptor gene promoter Patton, G.C. (1988). Mortality in eating disorders. Psychological Medicine, 18, 947-951. Pompili M, Mancinelli I, Girardi P, Ruberto A, & Tatarelli R (2004). Suicide in Anorexia Nervosa: A Meta-Analysis. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 36: 99-103. Sakinofsky, I. (2000) Repetition of suicidal behaviour. In: Hawton K. & Van Heeringen C, eds. The international handbook of suicide and attempted suicide, Wiley, England. Sansone, R.A., & Levitt, J.L. (2002). Self-harm behaviors among those with eating disorders. Eating Disorders: The Journal of Treatment & Prevention, 10, 205-213. Sansone, R.A., & Levitt, J.L. (2004). The prevalence of self-harm behavior among those with eating disorders. In: L. Levitt, R.A. Sansone & L. Cohn (eds). Self-harm behavior and eating disorders: dynamics, assessment, and treatment. New York: Brunner & Routledge. Schmidtke, A., Weinacker, B., Apter, A., Batt, A., Berman, L., Bille-Brahe, U., Botsis, A., De Leo, D., Doneux, A., Goldney, R., Grad, O., Haring, K., Hawton, K., Hjelmeland, H., Kelleher, M., Kerkhof, A., Leenaars, A., Lönnqvist, J., Michel, K., Ostamo, A., Salander-Renberg, E., Sayil, I., Takahashi, Y., Van Heeringen, C., Vrnik, A., & Wasserman, D. (1999). Suicide rates in the world: an update. Archives of Suicide Research, 5, 81-89. Sullivan, P.F. (1995). Mortality in anorexia nervosa. American Journal of Psychiatry, 152, 1073-1074. Van Heeringen C. (2001). Understanding suicidal behaviour: the suicidal process approach to research, treatment and prevention. Chichester, Wiley. Van Heeringen, C., Audenaert, K., Van Laere, K., Dumont, F., Slegers, G., Mertens, J., & Dierckx, R.. (2003). Prefrontal 5HT2a receptor binding index, hopelessness and personality characteristics in attempted suicide. Journal of Affective Disorders, 74, 149-158. Van Strien, T., Frijters, J.E.R., Bergers, G.P.A. & Defares, P.B. (1986). The Dutch Eating Behaviour Questionnaire (DEBQ) for assessment of restrained, emotional and external eating behaviour. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 5, 295-315. Wildman, P., Lilenfeld L.R.R., & Marcus M.D. (2004). Axis I comorbidity Onset and parasuicide in women with eating disorders. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 35, 190-197. Williams, J.M.G. & Pollock, L. (2001). Psychological aspects of the suicidal process. In Van Heeringen C. (ed.) Understanding suicidal behaviour: the suicidal process approach to research, treatment and prevention. Chichester, Wiley. PART II The cognitive psychology of eating disorders: personality, cognitions and behaviour. Chapter 3: Integration of personality characteristics in a psychobiological model of eating disorders: general outcome (paper in preparation) Introduction This chapter will describe the association between ED and personality dimensions in a large sample of ED- patients. The findings will be discussed in the context of the hypothetical model for the prediction by eating-disordered behaviour as it was described in the last sections of Part I. Based on the differences of the personality characteristics between the subcategories of ED patients and the measured clinical differences between these groups, it could be expected that the higher the Novelty Seeking (positive correlation) and the lower the Self-directedness (negative correlation), the higher the BMI, HBMI, the higher the scores on external and emotional eating and on drive for thinness, bulimia and body dissatisfaction, and the higher the number of patients who vomited and/or abused laxative, will be, since these characteristics scored highest in the BP group. Methods Eight hundred patients who were consecutively referred to the in- and out-patient units of the Centre for Eating Disorders at the University Department of Psychiatry of the Ghent University Hospital between December 1998 and September 2004 and who met the DSM-IV criteria for Eating Disorders were enrolled in the study. The assessments were done during the first week following referral before start of the treatment. Research was compliant with the Code of Ethics of the World Medical Association (Declaration of Helsinki) and performed following the guidelines of the local ethics committee. The referred group included 329 (41 %) anorectic patients of whom 239 were restrictor-type anorectics (RAN), while 90 were purgers/bingers (BPAN). Among the bulimic patients (B=273; 34 %), 225 were purgers/bingers (BP) and 48 were non-purging type bulimics (B-NP). The group of patients diagnosed with eating disorders not otherwise specified (EDNOS) contained 198 (25 %) patients of whom 58 met the criteria for binge eating disorder (BED). Most of the patients were female as only 3,5 % (n = 28) were male, with 5 male patients in the RAN group, 1 in the BPAN, 9 in the BP group, 3 in the B-NP and 10 in the EDNOS group of whom 3 were BED patients. The control group (n = 326) consisted of 178 female students in the first year of their training in Psychology or Medicine, and 148 women from a general population sample (van Heeringen & Meerschaert, unpublished data) (see Table 1). Table 1. Frequencies of study population N % RAN 239 29.9 BPAN 90 11.3 BP 225 28.1 B-NP 48 6.0 EDNOS 140 17.5 BED 58 7.3 ED 800 100 Measures Weight and height, highest and lowest adult weight and duration of illness were assessed carefully. The body mass index (BMI) was calculated, as well the highest and lowest lifetime BMI (HBMI, LBMI), based on the highest and lowest adult weight (16 years or older). HBMI and LBMI can thus reflect an underestimation as current body lengths were used to calculate these indices, as patients may not be reliable in remembering their precise length at the moment of their highest and lowest weight. Patients were asked to describe the frequency of their binging, purging and restricting eating according to three categories: never, more than two times per week or less than two times per week. Binging was described as losing control over eating, not including any measurement of calories, but as ‘involving larger amounts of food than most people would eat during a similar period of time and under similar circumstances’. As in the five studies described before, the validated Dutch version of the TCI (Duysens et al., 2000) was used to assess temperament and character characteristics. The Dutch Eating Behaviour Questionnaire (DEBQ; Van Strien, 1986) was used to assess the behavioural characteristics ‘emotional eating’ (eating elicited by diffuse and clear emotional stimuli), ‘restrained eating’ (the tendency to diet in order to lose weight), and ‘external eating’ (eating elicited by external stimuli). The Eating Disorder Inventory (EDI; Garner et al., 1983) was used to assess body dissatisfaction, perfectionism and interpersonal distrust. Statistical analysis was performed using SPSS 10.0. The reliability and validity of the assessment of symptoms and diagnoses using self-administered questionnaires is variable. Self-reports can be associated with demand characteristics, response bias and defensiveness (Vitousek & Orimoto, 1993). With regard to eating disorders, studies in different populations have consistently shown that self-report generates higher scores than the Eating Disorders Examination (EDE; Fairburn & Cooper, 1993), which is the golden standard for the assessment of complex features such as binge eating and concerns about shape, weight, and dietary restraint (Celio, Wilfley, Crow, Mitchell, Walsh, 2004; De Zwaan, Mitchell, Swan-Kremeier, McGregor, Howell, Roerig, Crosby, 2004). The EDE is advantageous because it includes a detailed description of binge episodes and thus provides more valid assessments of the amount of food eaten, feelings of loss of control, and frequency of episodes that are essential to diagnose a clinical disorder. However, the assessment of eating disorders by means of self-reports is brief, inexpensive, and highly efficient. Drawbacks in using the EDE indeed include the amount of time it takes to administer (30-60 min) and the extensive training of personnel that is required (Goldfein, Devlin, Kamenetz, 2005). Although the current study used self-reports, the data can be regarded as reliable. All patients were treated in the same department so that obvious discrepancies between self-reported and clinical data were corrected during the first weeks of treatment. Moreover the main target was the study of the relationship between usually measured cognitive and behavioural characteristics and personality dimensions in order to the construction of a hypothetical neuro-bio-psychological model associated with eating disordered symptoms. Results In this part of the study, data on typical ED patients (RAN, BPAN, BP and B-NP) were compared to those of control women. First age and personality characteristics were compared between AN and B patients and control women (CW) (see table 2). Table 2: Age and personality characteristics in eating disordered patient compared to control women Variable AN N=32 9 (320) m sd 22.5 8.1 16.1 6.5 4.7 2.6 3.3 2.4 3.5 2.4 4.5 2.1 24.0 6.5 B N=273 (264) m sd CW N=326 (326) m sd 23.1 7.2 20.0 6.8 5.6 2.6 4.4 2.6 5.0 2.1 4.9 2.1 23.8 7.2 23.4 8.4 20.3 6.2 6.7 2.3 4.4 2.7 5.1 2.0 4.2 2.2 17.1 7.0 Persistence (P) 7.7 2.4 5.2 1.7 5.3 2.2 5.8 2.4 16.1 3.6 7.4 2.0 4.2 2.3 4.6 1.2 5.6 1.9 7.8 2.5 4.9 2.0 5.1 2.4 5.9 2.4 16.3 3.6 7.3 2.0 4.5 2.3 4.5 1.1 5.1 2.0 Selfdirectedness 22.6 7.6 19.8 7.1 Age Novelty seeking(NS) Exploratory excitability (NS1) Impulsiveness (NS2) Extravagance (NS3) Disorderliness (NS4) Harm Avoidance(HA) Anticipatory worry (HA1) Fear of uncertainty (HA2) Shyness (HA3) Fatigability (HA4) Reward dependence (RD) Sentimentality (RD1) Attachment (RD3) Dependence (RD4) F p 1.0 .360 ns 32.0 .000 AN < B, CW 41.3 .000 AN < B < CW 14.2 .000 AN < B, CW 46.0 .000 AN < B, CW 10.0 .000 AN, CW < B 103.1 .000 CW < ED 5.0 2.6 4.5 1.8 4.0 2.3 3.5 2.2 18.5 3.4 7.9 1.7 6.2 2.0 4.5 1.1 4.4 2.0 121.0 .000 CW < ED 11.9 .000 CW < ED 30.6 .000 CW < ED 103.6 .000 CW < ED 45.1 .000 ED < CW 7.5 .001 ED < CW 75.3 .000 ED < CW .352 .725 ns 34.0 .000 CW < ED B < AN 31.4 7.1 210.0 .000 ED < CW B < AN (SD) Responsibility (SD1) Purposefulness (SD2) Resourcefulness (SD3) Self-acceptance (SD4) Enlightened Second Nature (SD5) Cooperativeness (C) Social acceptance (C1) Empathy (C2) Helpfulness (C3) Compassion (C4) Pure-hearted Conscience (C5) Selftranscendence (ST) Self-forgetful (ST1) Transpersonal identification(ST2) Spiritual Acceptance (ST3) 3.9 2.2 4.1 1.9 2.2 1.6 6.2 2.7 6.2 2.7 3.5 2.2 3.9 2.0 2.1 1.6 5.7 2.5 4.6 2.4 6.0 2.0 5.7 1.9 3.6 1.3 7.1 2.4 8.9 2.4 122.3 .000 ED < CW 79.8 .000 ED < CW 106.6 .000 ED < CW 24.6 .000 223.8 .000 ED < CW B < AN ED < CW B < AN 31.8 6.7 31.3 6.4 34.4 5.2 23.0 .000 ED < CW 6.5 1.7 5.0 1.6 6.3 1.5 7.7 2.6 6.5 1.5 12.4 6.3 6.4 1.7 5.1 1.5 6.2 1.4 7.2 2.8 6.4 1.6 11.2 6.6 7.2 1.2 5.9 1.2 6.9 1.3 7.6 2.5 6.8 1.5 13.0 6.7 26.0 .000 ED < CW 36.2 .000 ED < CW 27.9 .000 ED < CW 2.6 .074 ns 5.9 .003 ED < CW 5.5 .004 B < CW 4.6 2.7 2.7 2.1 4.7 3.1 4.6 2.7 2.3 2.0 4.3 3.4 4.9 2.5 2.6 2.1 5.5 3.4 1.1 .323 ns 3.1 .047 B < AN 9.5 .000 ED < CW AN = Anorectic patients; B = Bulimic patients; CW = Control women; ED = AN + B ED patients showed comparatively high scores on HA (on all the sub-dimensions) and low scores on SD (on all the sub-dimension). Mean scores on RD and cooperativeness in all ED categories were lower than the mean scores of control women except for the RD4 ‘dependence’ and C4 ‘compassion’ sub-dimensions. To the contrary, ED patients showed a comparatively higher score on the temperament dimension Persistence (P). ED patients also showed a comparatively lower score on the sub-dimension ‘Exploratory excitability’ (NS1) of the ‘Novelty Seeking’ temperament dimension (NS) and a comparatively lower score on the sub-dimension ‘Spiritual Acceptance’ (ST3). Differences between the anorectic and bulimic categories of ED patients included a lower score on Novelty Seeking among AN patients than among bulimic patients. Bulimic patients had also a lower score on Persistence (P), when compared to AN patients. Furthermore, lower scores were found for bulimic patients for ‘Self-directedness’ (SD), especially for the sub-dimensions ‘Self-acceptance’ (SD4) and ‘Enlightened Second Nature’ (SD5). Bulimic patients showed lower scores on ‘Self-Transcendence’ (ST) compared to control women, and lower scores on ‘Transpersonal Identification’ (ST2) compared to anorectic patients. Comparing diagnostic subtypes of ED’s, excluding the non-purging bulimics, significant differences were mentioned in Table 3. Table 3: Significant differences in personality characteristics between eating disordered patients Variable Novelty seeking(NS) Exploratory excitability (NS1) Impulsiveness (NS2) Extravagance (NS3) Disorderliness (NS4) Persistence (P) Selfdirectedness (SD) Responsibility (SD1) Self-acceptance (SD4) Enlightened Second Nature (SD5) RAN N=23 9 (232) M 15.7 6.4 4.7 2.7 3.2 2.3 3.4 2.3 4.3 2.0 5.7 1.8 23.1 7.4 BPAN N=90 (88) SD BP N=225 (218) M 17.1 6.8 4.7 2.5 3.5 2.5 4.0 2.4 4.9 2.4 5.5 2.1 21.4 8.0 3.9 2.2 6.3 2.6 6.5 2.7 F p 20.0 609 5.6 2.6 4.4 2.6 5.0 2.5 5.0 2.2 5.1 2.0 19.6 7.0 23.9 .000 6.9 .001 12.8 .000 AN < BP AN < BP 26.5 .000 AN < BP 5.7 .004 RAN < BP 5.2 .006 BP < RAN 12.5 .000 BP < RAN 3.7 2.3 3.3 2.1 3.9 .020 BP < RAN 5.9 2.8 5.5 2.7 5.6 2.5 4.6 2.4 4.5 .011 BP < RAN 28.7 .000 AN < BP BP < BPAN < RAN Although all anorectic patients scored lower than bulimics on the Novelty Seeking dimension, the scores of the restrictor group (RAN) were lower than those of the BP group only on the subdimension ‘Disorderliness’ (NS4). This was combined with higher scores, though only for the RAN patients, on all dimensions of the ‘Self-directedness’ and ‘Persistence’. Finally, the scores of the RAN patients were even higher than those of the BPAN group on the sub-dimension ‘Enlightened Second Nature’ (SD5). Further statistical analysis included the calculation of correlations between the measured personality characteristics and cognitive and behavioural aspects related to the eating pathology. Major findings regarding these aspects are summarized in Table 4 and Table 5. Table 4: Age, weight-related characteristics, duration of illness, behavioural and cognitive characteristics in eating disorder patients, by type of eating disorder. BP RAN BPAN p N=239 N=90 N=225 F m m m sd sd sd Age 21.9 24.1 23.0 2.7 .07 ns 8.2 7.7 7.3 Duration of illness 3.9 5.3 4.6 2.0 .137 ns 6.3 6.2 6.2 Age at onset 18.0 18.8 18.3 0.9 .40 ns 5.1 4.8 5.0 BMI 15.0 15.6 21.1 373.4 .000 AN < B-P 1.5 1.1 3.5 Highest life time BMI 20.5 22.2 24.9 68.4 .000 RAN < BPAN < B-P 3.0 4.0 5.0 Lowest life time BMI 14.3 14.8 18.5 153.1 .000 AN < B-P 1.8 1.5 3.6 Restrained eating 3.8 4.1 4.0 4.6 .011 RAN < BPAN 1.1 1.0 0.8 External eating 2.5 2.9 3.4 86.7 .000 RAN < BPAN < B-P 0.7 0.9 0.7 Emotional eating 2.2 3.2 3.8 183.2 .000 RAN < BPAN < B-P 1.0 1.1 0.9 Drive for thinness 11.6 13.5 15.6 23.2 .000 RAN < BPAN, B-P 7.2 6.6 5.1 Bulimia 1.1 6.5 10.9 268.8 .000 RAN < BPAN < B2.5 6.0 5.5 P Body dissatisfaction 11.8 13.0 17.9 36.9 .000 AN < B-P 7.6 7.8 8.1 Ineffectiveness 12.0 12.9 12.9 1.04 .353 ns 7.3 7.5 7.6 Perfectionism 7.0 7.1 7.0 0.02 .980 ns 4.6 4.6 4.8 distrust 6.2 6.6 5.5 2.3 .100 ns 4.5 4.5 4.4 Interoceptive awareness 10.2 10.9 11.1 1.8 .171 ns 5.2 5.5 5.4 Maturity fears 7.2 6.5 6.6 1.1 .347 ns Characteristics 5.5 5.0 5.3 ANOVA with Bonferoni post hoc comparison, (df =2), with level of significance set at p < .05 Table 5: Compensatory behaviour by type of eating disorder RAN BPAN BP N=239 N=90 N=225 N N N % % % p Purging 40 17 90 100 225 100 .000 Vomiting 19 7 24 10 69 29 73 81 21 23 15 17 181 80 85 38 58 26 .000 Laxative abuse Excessive exercise .000 .050 Pearson Chi-Square No significant differences between the three ED groups were found for age, duration of illness, age of onset, ineffectiveness, distrust, interoceptive awareness and maturity fears. AN patients had a lower current BMI, as expected, but also a lower highest and lowest lifetime BMI, and lower scores on external and emotional eating, bulimia and body dissatisfaction. Among AN patients, the RAN group, showed the lowest scores on external and emotional eating, bulimia and the highest lifetime BMI. Restrictive anorectic patients (RAN) scored lower compared to both binger/purger groups (BPAN and BP) on ‘restrained eating’ and lower compared to the BPAN group on ‘drive for thinness’. As shown in Table 5, patients in the three ED categories differed significantly with regard to the presence of vomiting, purging, abusing laxatives and excessive exercise more than two times per week. Table 6 shows the correlations between scores on personality dimensions and those on cognitive and behavioural characteristics. Table 6: Correlations between personality dimensions and cognitive and behavioural characteristics NS HA RD P SD CO ST .18** .07 .09* -.23** -.10** -.02 .09* .10** .03 -.17** -.09* -.04 -.11** .19** .02 -.24** -.05 -.04 -.10** -.08* .34** .03 -.12** BMI HBMI .06 LBMI .06 -.31** -.04 .005 Drive for thinness Bulimia .22** .07* Body dissatisfaction .01 -.00 -.14** -.30** -.10** -.03 .37** .06 -.05 -.36** -.06 -.07 -.13** -.02 .02 Restrained eating -.15** .18** -.02 .18** External eating .24** -.01 Total emotional .18** .11** -.04 -.11** -.32** -.09* -.07 Laxative abuse -.02 .14** .03 .04 -.13** -.02 -.01 Vomiting .15** .02 .00 -.05 -.15** -.02 .06 Excessive exercise -.02 -.04 -.05 .09** .04 -.02 -.06 -.15** -.18** -.11** .05 .01 * Correlation is significant at the 0.05 level (2-tailed)= p < . 05 ** Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed) = p < . 01 A number of correlations between key characteristics of ED, i.e. body dissatisfaction, perfectionism and interpersonal distrust, and personality dimensions were found. As shown in Table 7, body dissatisfaction correlated positively with HA and negatively with self-directedness, while perfectionism correlated positively with HA, RD, P and ST and negatively with NS and SD. Interpersonal distrust correlated negatively with NS, RD, SD and C, and positively with HA and P Table 7: Positive and Negative correlations between specific cognitive characteristics and personality dimensions NS HA RD P SD + Body Dissatisfaction C ST - Perfectionism - + +* + - Interpersonal distrust - + - +* - + - Correlation is only significant at the 0.05 level (2-tailed)= p < . 05 The correlations between personality dimensions and behavioural and cognitive characteristics according to ED categories are shown in Table 8, 9 and 10. Table 8 : Correlations between personality dimensions and cognitive and behavioural characteristics in the RAN group NS Body dissatisfaction HA RD +0..33** SD -0.38** Perfectionism -0.14* Interpersonal distrust -0.39** +0.39** -0.46** * Correlation is significant at the 0.05 level (2-tailed)= p < . 05 ** Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed) = p < . 01 +0.14* P +0.16* +0.39** -0.19** -0.24** C ST Table 9 : Correlations between personality dimensions and cognitive and behavioural characteristics in the BPAN group NS HA Body dissatisfaction Perfectionism RD P SD +0.51** -0.38** -0.24* Interpersonal distrust C ST +0.44** -0.26* -0.50** +0.30** -0.34** * Correlation is significant at the 0.05 level (2-tailed)= p < . 05 ** Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed) = p < . 01 Table 10 : Correlations between personality dimensions and cognitive and behavioural characteristics in the BP group NS Body dissatisfaction -0.14* HA RD P SD C ST +0.38** -0.21** Perfectionism -0.27** +0.32** -0.35** Interpersonal distrust -0.30** +0.42** -0.51** +0.15* -0.41** -0.23** -0.17** * Correlation is significant at the 0.05 level (2-tailed)= p < . 05 ** Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed) = p < . 01 A correlation between body dissatisfaction and HA (+) and SD (-) was found in the three diagnostic groups (RAN, BPAN and BN). Only among the BP patients there was a negative correlation between NS and body dissatisfaction. A positive correlation was found between perfectionism and HA (not for BPAN), RD (not for BPAN and BP), P (not for BP), and ST (not for RAN and BP). A negative correlation between perfectionism and NS and SD was found in the three groups. Finally, for interpersonal distrust a negative correlation was found with NS and RD in all three groups, and additionally with SD (except for the BPAN group) and with, C (only for the BP group). A positive correlation was found between interpersonal distrust and HA (not for BPAN) and only with P for the BP group. Finally correlations between cognitive and behavioural characteristics were calculated. Table 11: Correlations between cognitive and behavioural characteristics BMI .02 Restrained eating .62** Emotional eating .18** vomiting .18** Laxative abuse .31** Excessive exercise .11** .32** -.03 .72** .44** .17** -.03 Body dissatisfaction Ineffectiveness .37** .26** .25** .10** .24** .07 -.02 .19** .14** .06 .17** .04 Perfectionism -.07 .21** .03 .03 .20** .01 Interpersonal distrust Interoceptive Awareness Maturity fears -.03 .10* .02 .04 .09* .01 -.02 .23** .19** .10** .18** .07* -.08* .17** .06 -.02 .04 .02 Drive for Thinness Bulimia * Correlation is significant at the 0.05 level (2-tailed)= p < . 05 ** Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed) = p < . 01 Table 11 shows that only positive correlation between the eating pathology-related cognitive and behavioural characteristics were found, with the exception of a negative correlation between maturity fears and BMI. More specifically, ‘Drive for thinness’ and ‘Interoceptive awareness’ were positively correlated with emotional eating and all assessed compensatory behaviours: restrained eating, vomiting, laxative abuse and excessive exercise, bulimia correlated with BMI, emotional eating, vomiting and laxative abuse. ‘Body dissatisfaction’ was positively correlated with BMI, restrained and emotional eating, vomiting, laxative abuse but not with excessive exercise. ‘Ineffectiveness’ correlated with restrained and emotional eating and laxative abuse, and Perfectionism with restrained eating and with laxative abuse. Although a strong correlation was found between ‘interpersonal distrust’ and most TCI personality dimensions, no correlation between this cognitive factor and the behavioural dimensions was found except for restrained eating and laxative abuse and this only at the p < 0.05 level. Finally, ‘Maturity Fears’ was correlated positively with restrained eating. Discussion ED patients showed comparatively high scores in HA and low scores on SD These personality traits might thus describe the basic personality of the “ED’s spectrum” (Brewerton, Hand, & Bishop, 1993; Bulik, Sullivan, Weltzin, & Kay, 1995; Cloninger, Przybeck, Svrakic, & Wetzel, 1994; Diaz-Marsa, Carrasco, & Saiz-Ruiz, 2000; Klump, Bulik, Pollice et al., 2000; Mizushima, Ono, & Asai, 1998; Nagata et al., 2003) and appear to have a negative prognostic value (Bulik, Sullivan, Joyce, Carter, & McIntosh, 1998). HA is a temperamental trait strictly linked to the individual’s predisposition to react to stressful situations with behavioural inhibition, fear, anxiety, and depression (Cloninger & Svrakic, 1999). A high HA is usually found in depressive and anxiety disorders (Cloninger et al., 1994). The fact that EDs are characterized by a higher HA than the controls may mean that patients with EDs have a temperamental predisposition toward the development of an ED as a reaction to stressors (Cloninger & Svrakic, 1999). ED patients show a comparatively high score on all sub-dimensions ‘Anticipatory Worrying’ (HA1), ‘Fear of uncertainty’ (HA2), ‘Shyness’ (HA3) and ‘Fatigability’ (HA4), which indicates that they are cautious, nervous, timid, doubtful, discouraged, insecure, passive, negativistic, or pessimistic even in situations that do not worry other people. They have less energy and a strong tendency to tiredness when compared to other people, with a slow recovery from stressors and minor illnesses (Cloninger et al., 1994). This temperamental predisposition is associated with a low serotonergic tone (Cloninger et al., 1994; Diaz-Marsa et al., 2000; Waller, Petty, Hardy, Gullion, Murdock, & Rush, 1993) and might represent a factor which predisposes to EDs through a familial transmission (Fassino, Abbate-Daga, Amianto, Leombruni, Boggio, & Rovera, 2002a; Fassino, Svrakic, Abbate Daga, Amianto, Leombruni, & Stanic, 2002b; Kaye, Strober, Stein, & Gendall, 1999; Strober, Freeman, Lampert, Diamond, & Kaye, 2000). On the other hand, a comparatively low Self-Directedness reflects a character element which promotes expression of eating disorder (Bulik et al., 1995; Bulik, Sullivan, Joyce, & Carter, 1995; Cloninger, 2000; Diaz-Marsa, Carrasco, Hollander, Cesar, & Saiz-Ruiz, 2000; Svrakic, Whitehead, Przybeck, & Cloninger, 1993). Individuals displaying low Self-Directedness are described as immature, self-humbling, and unable to fix and pursue goals (Cloninger et al., 1994). Low Self-Directedness is associated with personality disorder in a high percentage of individuals (Svrakic, et al., 1993; Cloninger, 2000) and appears to be a negative prognostic factor for the effect of psychotherapy in EDs (Bulik et al., 1998). ED patients are in particular characterized by a tendency to consider other people and situations responsible for their frustrations through the perception that their behaviour is largely determined by influences beyond their control or free will (SD1 significantly lower in all ED groups; Fassino et al., 2002). Such individuals might be characterized by a tendency to anticipate pain and failure with pessimistic thoughts and by not being able to bear humbling and embarrassing experiences, to which they would react with long lasting “ruminations” (Cloninger et al., 1994). The lower scores on ‘purposefulness’ (SD2) display a marked detachment from social relationships (Diaz-Marsa et al., 1998). It is possible that the difficulty in pursuing self-chosen goals and values and the tendency to look for an external guide - which are characteristics of poorly self-directing individuals (Cloninger et al., 1994), represent elements promoting the expression of what Gendall and co-workers defined (1998) as “ a susceptibility to social messages on thinness”. Partially recovered, chronically ill anorexics reported significantly higher HA (controlled for the prevalence of lifetime major depression or depression at the time of follow-up), lower SD and lower CO than either healthy controls or fully recovered AN patients (Bulik, Sullivan, Fear, & Pickering, 2000). These authors found that high SD is associated with a good outcome (Bulik, Sullivan, Carter, McIntosh, & Joyce, 1998) and predicts an rapid response to cognitivebehavioural therapy also in bulimic patients (Bulik, Sullivan, Carter, McIntosh, & Joyce, 1999). It has been suggested that low character development is a fundamental psychopathologic factor for all EDs (Bulik et al., 1998; Diaz-Marsa et al., 1998; 2000). Mean scores on RD (the data about this dimension are controversial; Brewerton, Hand, & Bishop, 1993) and cooperativeness in all ED categories were lower than the mean scores of control women except for the RD4 ‘dependence’ and C4 ‘compassion’ sub-dimensions. ED patients thus showed a greater tendency to intolerance, criticism, and opportunism than controls. They can be described as social insensitive and as having difficulties in finding something in common with other people showing less worry about other people’s feelings, and allowing higher levels of aggressiveness toward other people and the external environment. Low scorers enjoy getting revenge on people who hurt them, either overt (active-aggressive behaviour) or disguised (passive-aggressive), while showing a tendency to treat other people unfair, in a biased, self-serving manner that usually reflects their own profit. To the contrary, ED patients showed a comparatively higher score on the temperament dimension Persistence (P) was found. A highly persistent individual tends to be a perfectionist, and when contingencies change rapidly, perseveration becomes maladaptive. ED patients also showed a comparatively lower score on the sub-dimension ‘Exploratory excitability’ (NS1) of the ‘Novelty Seeking’ temperament dimension (NS) compared to control females. ED patients thus usually demand very good practical reasons before they are willing to change the way they do something. Finally, ED patients showed a comparatively lower score on the sub-dimension ‘Spiritual Acceptance’ (ST3). This is a disadvantage when facing situations over which there is no control or possibility for evaluation by rational objective means, such situations may include inevitable death, suffering, or unjust punishments. Differences between the anorectic and bulimic categories of ED patients included a lower score on Novelty Seeking among AN patients than among bulimic patients (Brewerton et al., 1993; Casper, 1990; Casper, Hedeker, & McClough, 1992; Bulik et al., 1995a; 1995b; Nagata, Oshima, Wada, Yamada, Iketani, & Kiriiki, 2003; Waller et al., 1993) and compared to control women (Klump et al., 2000). Bulimic patients are thus in general, less organized, methodical, and systematic than AN patients. Although other studies found AN patients to be similar to non-patient controls with regard to NS (Kleifield, Sunday, Hurt, & Halmi, 1993; Kleifield, Sunday, Hurt, & Halmi, 1994; Brewerton et al., 1993; Bulik, Sullivan, Weltzin, et al., 1995), we only found this similarity for the sub-dimension ‘Disorderliness’ (NS4). Bulimic patients had also a lower score on Persistence (P), when compared to AN patients. Furthermore, lower scores were found for bulimic patients for ‘Selfdirectedness’ (SD), especially for the sub-dimensions ‘Self-acceptance’ (SD4) and ‘Enlightened Second Nature’ (SD5). This means that bulimic patients tend to manifest behaviours, which are unfavourable in pursuit of their goals, and show a poor strength of will, thus being unable to resist their temperamental impulses (Diaz-Marsa et al., 1999; Klump et al., 2000). Usually they are described as manipulative. Bulimic patients showed lower scores on ‘Self-Transcendence’ (ST) compared to control women, and lower scores on ‘Transpersonal Identification’ (ST2) compared to anorectic patients. In that way, they are less able to tolerate ambiguity and uncertainty, rather materialistic, and unable to be satisfied with what they have. Although studies comparing traits between diagnostic subtypes of ED’s are scarce, it seems that RAN and BP patients have some opposite traits and BPAN patients share features with RAN and BP patients (Van der Ham, Meulman, Van Strien, & Van Engeland, 1997). Patients of the B-NP group were excluded from the following comparative analysis, since they were studied in comparison with the BED patients separately (paper above) and they are rather low in frequency. These results show that personality characteristics of patients with eating disorders differ from those of healthy control females. Anorectic and bulimic patients differed from control women by having relatively higher scores on HA and P and lower scores on RD. This temperament constellation was combined with a poor character development as reflected by lower scores on SD and C. When comparing patients in the ED categories with control women, particularly for AN patients differences on the scores of temperament dimensions were found. Besides higher scores on HA and P and lower scores on RD, AN patients and particularly the RAN group, also scored lower on Novelty Seeking,. On the contrary, no difference for this dimension was found between bulimics and healthy controls. Bulimic patients scored lower than the AN patients, particularly the RAN group, on character dimensions, including SD and its sub-dimension SD5, and the ‘Transpersonal identification’ subdimension of ST. No significant differences between the three ED groups were found for age, duration of illness, age of onset, ineffectiveness, distrust, interoceptive awareness and maturity fears. Although for the anorectic group, a higher score on perfectionism was found by Garner and colleagues (1993), no difference between the categories was found in this study. As we have mentioned in earlier described results (Part4article3), the motive for fasting can be different in the RAN group, since there lower score for restrained eating and drive for thinness. The hypothesis, as formulated in the introduction of this chapter, was confirmed with the exception of a negative correlation between drive for thinness and NS, and the absence of a correlation between body dissatisfaction and NS. The negative correlation between cognitive and behavioural characteristics and SD, also supports this hypothesis. Since the RAN group has the highest score on Persistence, a negative correlation with HBMI, Restrained, External and Emotional Eating, drive for thinness and bulimia was hypothesized. This was also confirmed except for a positive relation between Persistence and restrained eating and the absence of a correlation between persistence and drive for thinness. Of interest was also the positive correlation between excessive exercise (mostly seen in the RAN group) and Persistence. It can thus be concluded that the three key characteristics of eating disorders show a clear correlation with relevant personality dimensions. As hypothesized, body dissatisfaction plays a central role in the basic spectrum of all three ED categories and correlated especially with HA (+) and SD (-). Perfectionism appears to play a more important role in the RAN type, whereas interpersonal distrust correlated with all personality dimensions in the BP group. As mentioned before, the BPAN group shares features with the RAN and the BP group. There is a paucity of studies on the association between personality characteristics, using the TCI, and cognitive or behavioural aspects. Our finding of patterns of correlations between, on the one hand, novelty seeking and self-directedness, and, on the other hand, the assessed behavioural and cognitive characteristics as described above, are in keeping with findings of clusters of characteristics found in bulimics such as increased novelty seeking, more outspoken external and emotional eating, and bulimia. The current findings suggest that the occurrence of AN, especially the RAN group, is determined more by temperamental factors, i.e. predisposition, while the occurrence of BP is influenced more by immature character development. A comparable conclusion was formulated in a recent Japanese study (Nagata et al., 2003), suggesting the importance of environmental factors in the pathogenesis of bulimic symptoms in a culture, which differs much from Western ones. This could clarify why cognitive behavioural therapy (CBT) is (more) effective in the treatment of bulimia nervosa, since CBT is based on increasing self-control and changing maintaining factors. However, personality development is not static and little is known about the interaction between distinct temperament characteristics and the environment. Only one study using a longitudinal design to examine the role of childhood temperament in the later development of eating concerns (Martin, Wertheim, Prior, Smart, Sanson, & Oberklaid, 2000). It was found that high negative emotionality (negative reactivity) and low task persistence assessed during childhood, were the factors most strongly associated with the risk of the later development of eating disorders, particularly among girls. Another study found that bulimic individuals were found more likely than control subjects to perceive their fathers as less affectionate and more controlling toward them than they were toward their siblings (Wonderlich, Ukestad, & Perzacki, 1994). Very interesting is the study of Berg and colleagues (2000), in which the relationship between temperament and perception of non-shared environment in BN patients was measured. Among bulimics, high levels of Harm Avoidance were more likely to be associated with increased maternal affection and decreased levels of maternal control. One possible explanation for this association is that in families of bulimic individuals, increased HA in the bulimic child is responded to with a rather protective and non-demanding maternal response. Similarly, high levels of NS were more likely to be associated with decreased paternal control (i.e. discipline). Again, it is plausible that these findings reflect a pattern in which fathers of bulimic individuals respond to temperamental extremes with less limiting and structuring behaviour. Johnson and Connors (1987) also reported parental under-involvement in the families of bulimic individuals. In summary of this study it can be stated that eating-disordered patients describe themselves as more anxious and less socially sensitive, self-directed and cooperative than healthy volunteers of similar age and educational level. AN patients show a lower score than BP patients and healthy volunteers on NS. RAN patients score higher than BP patients on persistence and self-directedness. Scores on the temperamental dimension NS correlate positively with levels of body dissatisfaction and negatively with levels of perfectionism and interpersonal distrust, while sores on persistence correlate positively with levels of perfectionism. Scores on the character dimension self-directedness correlate negatively with the cognitive attitudes body dissatisfaction, perfectionism and interpersonal distrust. Lower scores on NS in combination with increased scores on P and SD become manifest through behaviour that is less reactive to external and emotional stimuli, which in turn may lead to a comparatively lower BMI. A major outcome of these studies is that the demonstrated associations between core cognitive and behavioural symptoms and these personality dimensions create the possibility to bridge the gap between psychological theories and neurobiological functioning of eating-disordered patients. Consequently, there was an impetus for further psychobiological research, which takes into account the nurture-related temperamental and the development-related character dimensions of eating disorders in general, and RAN and BP in particular. Part 3 describes two such studies followed by conclusions and suggestions for further research. References Berg ML, Crosby RD, Wonderlich SA, & Hawley D (2000). Relationship of temperament and perceptions of non-shared environment in bulimia nervosa. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 28, 148-154. Brewerton TD, Hand LD, & Bishop ER (1993). The Tri-dimensional Personality Questionnaire in eating disorder patients. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 14, 213-218. Bulik C, Sullivan P, Carter FA, McIntosh VV, Joyce PR (1998). The role of exposure with response prevention in the cognitive-behavioral therapy for bulimia nervosa. Psychological Medicine, 28, 611-623. Bulik CM, Sullivan PF, Carter FA, McIntosh VV, Joyce PR (1999). Predictors of rapid and sustained response to cognitivebehavioral therapy for bulimia nervosa. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 26, 611-623. Bulik CM, Sullivan PF, Fear JL, Pickering A (2000). Outcome of anorexia nervosa: eating attitudes, personality, and parental bonding. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 28(2), 139-47. Bulik CM, Sullivan PF, Joyce PR, & Carter FA (1995). Temperament, character, and personality disorder in bulimia nervosa. Journal of Nervous & Mental Disease, 183, 593-598. Bulik CM, Sullivan PF, Joyce PR, Carter FA, & McIntosh VV (1998). Predictors of 1-year treatment outcome in bulimia nervosa. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 39, 206-214. Bulik CM, Sullivan PF, Weltzin TE, & Kay WH (1995). Temperament in eating disorders. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 17, 251-261. Casper RC (1990). Personality features of women with good outcome from restricting anorexia nervosa. Psychosomatic Medicine , 52, 156-170. Casper RC, Hedeker D, & McClough JF (1992). Personality dimensions in eating disorders and their subtyping. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 31 (5), 830-840. Celio AA, Wilfley DE, Crow SJ, Mitchell J, Walsh BT. A comparison of the Binge Eating Scale, Questionnaire for Eating and Weight Patterns-Revised, and Eating Disorder Examination Questionnaire with Instructions with the Eating Disorder Examination in the assessment of binge eating disorder and its symptoms. International Journal of Eating Disorders 2004; 36: 434-444. Cloninger CR, Svrakic DM. (1997). Psychiatry, 60(2), 120-41. Integrative psychobiological approach to psychiatric assessment and treatment. Cloninger CR, Svrakic DM, & Przybeck TR (1993). A psychobiological model of temperament and character. Archives of General Psychiatry, 50, 975-990. Cloninger CR (1987b). A systematic method for clinical description and classification of personality variants. Archives of General Psychiatry , 44, 573-588. Cloninger CR (2000). A practical way to diagnosis personality disorders: a proposal. Journal of Personality Disorders, 14, 99108. Cloninger CR, Przybeck TR, Svrakic DM, & Wetzel RD (1994). The Temperament and Character Inventory (TCI): A guide to its development and use. St. Louis, MO: Center for Psychobiology of Personality, Washington University. Cloninger CR, & Svrakic DM (1999). Personality Disorders. In B.J. Sadock & V.A. Sadock (Eds.), Kaplan and Sadock’s comprehensive textbook of psychiatry (7the ed.). London: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. Cloninger CR, Svrakic DM, & Przybeck TR (1993). A psychobiological model of temperament and character. Archives of General Psychiatry, 50, 975-990. De Zwaan M, Mitchell JE, Swan-Kremeier L, McGregor T, Howell ML, Roerig JL, Crosby RD. A comparison of different methods of assessing the features of eating disorders in post-gastric bypass patients: a pilot study. European Eating Disorders Review 2004; 12(6): 380-386. Diaz-Marsa M, Carrasco JL, Hollander E, Cesar J, & Saiz-Ruiz J (2000). Decrease platelet monoamine oxidase activity in female anorexia nervosa. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 101, 226-230. Diaz-Marsa M, Carrasco JL, Prieto Lopez R, & Saiz -Ruiz J (1999). Personality profile of eating disorders, Actas Esp Psiquiatr, 27, 43-50. Diaz Marsa M, Carrasco JL, Lopez-Ibor JJ, Saiz J (1998). A study of personality in eating disorders. Article in Spanish. Actas Luso Esp Neurol Psiquiatr Cienc Afines, 26(5), 288-96. Diaz-Marsa M, Carrasco JL, & Saiz-Ruiz J (2000). A study of temperament and personality in anorexia and bulimia nervosa. Journal of Personal Disorders, 14(4), 352-359. Duijsens IJ, Spinhoven P, Goekoop JG, Spermon T, & Eurelings-Bontekoe EHM (2000). The Dutch temperament and character inventory (TCI): dimensional structure, reliability and validity in a normal and psychiatric outpatient sample. Personality and Individual Differences 28, 487-499. Fairburn CG, & Cooper Z (1993). The eating disorders examination (12th ed.). In C.G. Fairburn & G.T. Wilson (Eds.), Binge eating: Nature, assessment, and treatment (pp. 317-360). New York: Guilford Press. Fassino S, Abbate-Daga G, Amianto F, Leombruni P, Boggio S, & Rovera GG. (2002a). Temperament and character profile of eating disorders: a controlled study with the temperament and character inventory. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 32, 412-425. Fassino S, Svrakic D, Abbate Daga G, Amianto F, Leombruni P, & Stanic S (2002b). temperament and character data. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 43(2), 114-120. Anorectic family dynamics: Garner DM, Olmstead MP, & Polivy J (1983). Development and validation of a multidimensional Eating Disorder Inventory for anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 2, 15-24. Garner DM, Rockert W, Davis R, Garner MV, Olmsted MP, Eagle M (1993). A comparison of cognitive-behavioral and supportive-expressive therapy for bulimia nervosa. American Journal of Psychiatry, 150, 37-46. Gendall KA, Joyce PR, Sullivan PF, & Bulik CM (1998). Personality and dimensions of dietary restraint. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 24, 371-379. Goldfein JA, Devlin MJ, Kamenetz C. Eating Disorder Examination-Questionnaire with and without Instruction to assess binge eating in patients with binge eating disorder. International Journal of Eating Disorders 2005; 37: 107-111. Humphrey LL (1991). Object relations and the family system: An integrative approach to understanding and treating eating disorders. In: Psychodynamic treatment of anorexia nervosa and bulimia (ed. C. Johnson), pp. 322-353. New York: Guilford Press. Johnson C, & Connors ME. The Etiology and Treatment of Bulimia Nervosa. New York, Basic Books, 1987. Kaye WH, Strober M, Stein D, & Gendall K (1999). New Directions in Treatment Research of Anorexia and Bulimia Nervosa. Biological Psychiatry, 45, 1285-1292. Kleifield E, Sunday S, Hurt SW, & Halmi KA (1993). Psychometric validation of the Tri-dimensional Personality Questionnaire: Application to subgroups of eating disorders. Comprehensive Psychiatry , 34 (4), 249-253. Kleifield E, Sunday S, Hurt S, & Halmi KA (1994). The tri-dimensional personality questionnaire: An exploration of personality traits in eating disorders. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 28, 413-423. Klump KL, Bulik CM, Pollice C, Halmi KA, Fichter MM, Berrettini WH, Devlin B, Strober M, Kaplan A, Woodside DB, Treasure J, Shabbout M, Lilenfeld LR, & Plotnicov KH (2000). Temperament and character in women with anorexia nervosa. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 188 (9), 559-567. Martin GC, Wertheim EH, Prior M, Smart D, Sanson A, & Oberklaid F (2000). A longitudinal study of the role of childhood temperament in the later development of eating concerns. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 27, 150-162. Mayer LES, & Walsh BT (1998). Pharmacotherapy of eating disorders. In: Neurobiology in the treatment of eating disorders (ed. Hoek, H.W., Treasure, J.L. & Katzman, M.A.) Wiley & Sons. Mizushima H, Ono Y, & Asai M, (1998). TCI temperamental scores in bulimia nervosa patients and normal women with and without diet experiences. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 98, 228-30. Nagata T, Oshima J, Wada A, Yamada H, Iketani T, & Kiriike N (2003). Temperament and Character of Japanese Eating Disorder Patients. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 44, 142-145. Steiger H, & Séguin JR (1999). Eating disorders : anorexia and bulimia nervosa. In: T. Millon, & P.H. Blaney (Eds.), Oxford textbook of psychopathology (pp. 365-389). Toronto: Oxford Univ. Press. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: Hillsdale, New York. Strober M (1991). Disorders of the self in anorexia nervosa: An organismic-developmental paradigm. In Psychodynamic treatment of anorexia nervosa and bulimia (ed. C. Johnson), pp. 354-373. Guilford Press: New York. Strober M, Freeman R, Lampert C, Diamond J, & Kaye W (2000). Controlled family study of anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa: Evidence of shard liability and transmission of partial syndromes. American Journal of Psychiatry, 157, 393-401. Svrakic DM, Whitehead C, Przybeck TR, & Cloninger CR (1993). Differential diagnosis of personality disorders by the seven factor model of temperament and character. Archives of General Psychiatry , 50, 991-999. Van der Ham T, Meulman JJ, Van Strien DC, & Van Engeland H (1997). Empirically based subgrouping of eating disorders in adolescents: a longitudinal perspective. British Journal of Psychiatry, 170, 363-368. Van Strien T.(1986). Eating behaviour, personality traits and body mass. Swets & Zeitlinger B.V.: Lisse. Vitousek KB, & Orimoto L (1993). Cognitive behavioural models of anorexia nervosa, bulimia nervosa, and obesity. In K.S. Dobson, & P.C. Kendall (Eds.), Psychopathology and Cognition (pp.191-243). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Waller DA, Petty F, Hardy BW, Gullion CM, Murdock MV, & Rush AJ (1993). Tridimensional Personality Questionnaire and serotonin in bulimia nervosa. Psychiatry Research, 48, 9-15. Wonderlich SA, Ukestad L, & Perzacki R (1994). Perceptions of non-shared childhood environment in bulimia nervosa. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 33, 740-746. PART III Further study of the psychobiological model of eating disorders 1. Introduction Based on the demonstrated differences in personality dimensions and the related cognitive and behavioural characteristics between healthy volunteers and types of eating disorders, the final aim was to study the association between these psychological findings and neurobiological functioning. The extensive research into the neurobiology of eating disorders has focused on neuropeptide and monoamine (especially serotonin, or 5-HT) systems thought to be central to the physiology of eating and weight regulation. Of the various central and peripheral abnormalities reported, many are likely to be secondary to aberrant eating behaviours and associated weight loss. However, some aspects of 5-HT function remain abnormal after recovery, suggesting that a trait monoamine abnormality might predispose individuals whether or not due to its association with characteristics such as perfectionism. Furthermore, normal dieting in healthy women alters central 5-HT function, providing a potential mechanism by which eating disorders might be precipitated in women who are vulnerable for other reasons (Fairburn, 2003; Kay & Strober, 1999; Kaye, Frank, Meltzer et al., 2001; Frank, Kaye, Meltzer, et al., 2002). The final part of this thesis includes two studies comparing cortical 5-HT2A receptor binding between respectively anorectic and bulimic patients and healthy volunteers. Further research addresses a comparison of 5-HT2A binding between subcategories of anorexia nervosa and the associations between temperament dimensions and the 5-HT2A binding index. 2. Decreased 5-HT2a Receptor Binding in Patients with Anorexia Nervosa9 Abstract Indirect estimations of brain neurotransmitters in patients with anorexia nervosa (AN) and low weight have demonstrated a reduction in brain serotonin (5-HT) turnover in general and led to hypotheses about dysfunction in the 5-HT2a receptor system. It was our aim to investigate the central 5-HT2a receptor binding index using SPET brain imaging. Methods: The 5-HT2a receptor of low weight patients with AN were studied by means of the highly specific radio-iodinated 5-HT2a receptor antagonist 4-amino-N-[1-[3-(4-fluorophenoxy)propyl]-4methyl-4-piperidinyl]-5-iodo-2-methoxybenzamide or 123I-5-I-R91150. Fifteen patients with the clinical diagnosis of AN and 11 age-matched healthy volunteers received intravenous injections of 185 MBq 123I-5-IR91150 and were scanned with high-resolution brain SPECT. Results: Compared to healthy volunteers, patients with AN had a significantly reduced 5-HT2a binding index in the left frontal cortex, in the left and right parietal cortex, and the left and right occipital cortex. A significant left-right asymmetry was noted in the frontal cortex (left < right). Conclusion: These results are in accordance with diminished metabolic and perfusion of frontal and parietal cortices reported in recent neuroimaging studies and imply localized disturbed serotonergic function. The data are discussed in the light of possible confounding factors related to the low-weight AN status. A regional cortical reduction in 5-HT2a binding index is not likely to be caused by a general reduction in serotonergic function due to the possible confounding factors. Suggestions for further research are given. Key words: Anorexia nervosa, serotonin, serotonin-2A receptor, SPECT The essential features of Anorexia Nervosa (AN) are that the individual refuses to maintain a minimally normal body weight and is intensely afraid of gaining weight. Weight loss can be accomplished primarily through excessive physical exercise and through voluntarily reduction in total food intake, sometimes accompanied by purging behaviour (i.e. self-induced vomiting or the misuse of laxatives or diuretics). In addition, a disturbance in the perception of body shape and weight is an essential neuropsychological feature of AN (1). Psychological and biological mechanisms appear to play key roles in the pathogenesis of AN. Neuropsychological investigations have indicated cognitive deficits in frontal cortex and in parietal cortex (2,3). Perceived distortion of body image has been associated specifically with parietal dysfunctions (4). Functional brain imaging with PET and SPET has shown reduced parietal and frontal cortex metabolism (5) and cerebral perfusion (6). 9 Audenaert K, Van Laere K, Dumont F, Vervaet M, Goethals I, Slegers G, Mertens J, van Heeringen C, Dierckx R (2003). Decreased 5-HT2a receptor binding in patients with anorexia nervosa. European Journal of Nuclear Medicine, 44, 163-169. In past decades, the involvement of serotonin as a neurotransmitter in AN was largely indicated through indirect estimations of brain serotonergic function. Impaired serotonin turnover and function were demonstrated through plasma measurements of tryptophan, the precursor of 5-hydroxytryptamine (5-HT) (7) or 5-hydroxyindole acetic acid (5-HIAA, the metabolite of 5-HT), in cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) of low weight AN patients (8). Elevated CSF 5-HIAA concentrations have been reported in longtime-weight-restored AN patients (8) . Another indication of reduced serotonergic function in lowweight AN patients was blunted physiological responses to the administration of selective pharmacological agonists (9-11). Again, these findings normalized after weight restoration (10). The involvement of the 5-HT2a receptor in the pathophysiology of AN was demonstrated indirectly via blood platelet studies, both through an enhanced mobilization of intracellular platelet calcium content, mediated by 5-HT2 receptors (12), and through enhanced platelet serotonin 5-HT2a binding, measured in vitro with 3H-lysergic acid diethylamide (13). Recently, studies with PET and 18Faltanserin as a tracer showed a reduced 5-HT2a binding in the orbitofrontal cortex in recovered bulimic patients (14). Some recent molecular genetic studies in patients with eating disorders have demonstrated an increased frequency of one of the alleles on the promotor region of the 5-HT2a gene (15). Also, twin and family studies suggest a genetic vulnerability to AN, and the hypothesis has been put forward that this vulnerability may be expressed in the central serotonergic system (16). Functional imaging techniques, such as PET and SPECT, using specific 5-HT2 receptor ligands, make it possible to evaluate in vivo receptor binding in patients with AN. Preliminary research in healthy subjects has indicated that 4-amino-N-[1-[3-(4-fluorophenoxy)propyl]-4-methyl-4-piperidinyl]-5iodo-2-methoxybenzamide ( 123I-5-I-R91150) is a suitable ligand for imaging 5-HT2a receptors in vivo. It binds reversibly and with high affinity in vitro to 5-HT2a receptors (17). On average, 2% of a bolus dose of 123 I-5-I-R91150 is taken up by the brain (18). Effective blockade of 5-HT2a receptors in vivo was demonstrated in a study of patients with schizophrenia who were treated with risperidone or clozapine (19). The aim of this study was to evaluate the 5-HT2a binding index (BI) in patients with AN. MATERIALS AND METHODS Patients Fifteen patients were included in the study. All were right handed, 16-30 y old, and diagnosed as having AN according to the criteria of the fourth edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV) (1). Patients were recruited from an inpatient eating disorder unit in a university psychiatric hospital. Patients were selected by a senior psychiatrist and a senior psychologist after DSMIV-based structured clinical interviews. To create a group of patients with consistent disease status, only symptom-fixed patients who stayed in the hospital as a result of resistance of the disease to treatment were included. Exclusion criteria were a comorbid psychiatric, major medical, or neurological disorder; antidepressant, neuroleptic or electroconvulsive therapy in the preceding year; substance abuse; pregnancy or lactation, and a Mini-Mental State Examination score < 28. Menstrual status was noted in female patients. The patients were compared with 11 age-matched healthy volunteers, (7 women, 4 men), who were recruited from hospital staff. These individuals had no history of psychiatric treatment or significant medical events. None used psychotropic drugs or other relevant medication, and none abused illegal drugs. All had normal physical examination findings. Ethical approval for the study was granted by the local ethics committee. Both patients and healthy individuals provided written informed consent to participate in this study. Scanning Procedure SPECT scanning was performed using 123 I-5I-R91150 as a radioligand. This tracer was synthesized by electrophilic substitution on the 5-position of the methoxybenzamide group of R91150, followed by purification with high-performance liquid chromatography. The product had a radiochemical purity > 99 % and was negative for bacteria and pyrogen tests. A specific activity of 370 GBq/µmol was obtained. The tracer is a 5-HT2a antagonist with high affinity (Kd = 0.11 nmol) and selectivity for 5-HT2a receptors. The selectivity of the ligand for 5-HT2a receptors is at least 50 times that of other receptors, such as other 5-HT receptors including 5-HT2c, dopamine receptors, adrenoreceptors, and histamine receptors (17). Thyroid blockade was achieved by administration of a single oral dose of 100 mg potassium iodine before injection. All subjects received an intravenous injection of 185 MBq 123 I-5-I-R91150 in normal sitting conditions. SPECT scanning was performed using a triple-headed high-resolution gamma camera GCA-9300 (Toshiba Medical Systems, Oetwil am See, Switzerland) with fanbeam collimation. For 123I, the resulting transaxial image resolution is 9.5 mm in full width at half maximum. Because sequential dynamic SPECT brain scans have shown that the corticocerebellar ratio reaches a plateau between 90 and 110 min, reflecting pseudoequilibrium, and remains stable thereafter for up to 8 h (18), acquisition was started between 110 and 140 minutes after tracer injection, according to previous protocols (20). A transmission scan was acquired before the emission scan, using 3 153Gd rod sources. This scan was used for subsequent image coregistration to stereotactic coordinates. Emission images were acquired during a 40 min-period. The whole-brain volume was acquired within the single scanning session. Images were reconstructed using filtered backprojection and corrected for scatter and nonuniform attenuation (21). Analysis of the scans was performed without knowledge of patient status after automatic image coregistration to stereotactic space using the transmission image (BRASS, Nuclear Diagnostics Ltd., Stockholm, Sweden) (22). The method for coregistration of SPECT receptor data using transmission images has been published previously (22). In short, the sequential acquisition of transmission and emission image can be use to anatomically standardize the emission image using the same linear parameters as those used for the transmission image. The latter is first reoriented to a template in Talairach space, and the same transformation is then given to the emission images. On the Talairach templates, a predefined volumeof-interest (VOI) set can be constructed (originally performed on perfusion SPECT images), which allows a user-independent sampling of the whole-brain volume of different patients without a previously available structural information. A diagram of the VOI map (22,23) is shown in Figure 1 and a description of VOIs is provided in Table 1. TABLE 1 Description of Retained Volumes of Interest with Radioactivity estimates in the cortex were Corresponding Brodmann Areas Volumes of interest Brodmann’s areas assumed to represent total ligand binding (1) Left and (2) right 6, 8, 9, 10, 11, 44, 45, 47 nonspecific binding plus (specific plus frontal cortex (3) Left and (4) right free ligand) (18). sensorimotor Because, very few 5cortex 1, 2, 3, 4 20, 21, 22, 37, 38, the cerebellum HT2a receptors are in (5) Left and (6) right temporal cortex 41, 42 5, 7, 39, 40 compared with the (7) Left and (8) right cortex (24), the parietal cortex 17, 18, 19 cerebellar region can be (9) Left and (10) right considered to represent occipital cortex non-specific activity. (11) Left and (12) right Calculation of relative cerebellum indices of specific BI was performed by VOI normalisation to the activity per volume element in the cerebellum. Under these pseudoequilibrium circumstances, this binding index is directly related to the in vivo receptor density (Bmax) and affinity (KD). BI was defined as: target activity – background activity in brain (background activity) which was operationally estimated as: [(counts/pixel in frontal cortex) – (counts/pixel in cerebellum)] (counts/pixel in cerebellum) Statistical methods The equality of age and body mass index (BMI) distributions between diagnostic categories was evaluated using the Mann Whitney test, and the equality of sex distributions was evaluated using the Fischer exact test. As BIs were normally distributed (Kolmogorov-Smirnov testing), t statistics were used to compare mean levels between categories. Correlation analyses were used to examine any relationships among BI, BMI, and disease duration. RESULTS Demographic and physical variables The 26 individuals in this study were on average 23.8 y old (SD = 4.1), with ages ranging from 16 to 30 y. Mean ages were not significantly different (Mann-Whitney test = 52.5; p= 0.12) between the two study groups, with 22.5 y (SD = 2.5) for patients with AN and 25.6 y (SD = 4.7) for healthy volunteers. A significant difference in sex (Fisher exact test; P = 0.02) existed between the group of healthy volunteers (7 female; 4 male) and patients with AN (15 females). The BMI in patients with AN was 14.9 (SD = 1.6; range, 11.9-16.9) which was significantly different (Mann-Whitney test = 0; P < 0.0001) from the BMI of the healthy volunteers (mean, 22.3; SD = 1.4; range, 20.5-24.5). The number of years of presence of disease was 4.3 (SD = 4.48; range, 1-14 y). All patients were postmenarcheal and amonorrheic. These variables are summarized in Table 2. Binding index Patients with AN had a significantly reduced 5-HT2a binding potential in the left frontal cortex, in the left and right parietal cortex, and the left and right occipital cortex when compared with healthy volunteers (see Table 3). Figure 2 shows an example of a 25-y-old patient compared with the anatomically standardized normal control population used for this study. Additional adjustment for age did not alter either the magnitude or the statistical significance of the observed difference between the groups. Individual values of left frontal and biparietal BI are plotted in Figure 3. Because of the significant difference in sex distribution in the patients and volunteers, individual values of male volunteers are indicated with arrows. All male patients fell in the same range as the female volunteers. Moreover, there is no statistical significance in BI in any of the regions between the male and female healthy volunteers (all P > 0.5). There was a significant difference in the left-to-right ratio in the frontal cortex of patients with eating disorders when compared with healthy volunteers (Table 4). Correlation analyses did not reveal significant relationships between regional cortical BIs, BMI and disease duration. DISCUSSION In this in vivo study of cortical 5-HT2a receptors in patients with AN, a significantly reduced 5-HT2a BI in the left frontal, bilateral parietal, and occipital cortices was demonstrated in comparison with that observed in healthy volunteers. A significant difference also was demonstrated in the left-right BI, (left < right) in the frontal cortex .in patients with AN. When evaluated in the light of indirect studies assessing serotonergic function and 5-HT2a binding status in low weight AN patients, the reduction in regional cortical BI is in keeping with the findings of neuroendocrine challenge tests and blood platelet studies that demonstrated a possible 5-HT2 involvement in the pathogenesis of AN (11-13). The reduction in BI was not present in all cortical regions but was restricted to the left frontal, bilateral parietal, and occipital cortices. This may be in accordance with functional imaging studies that have assessed cerebral blood flow or metabolism in patients with AN that have shown a regional hypometabolism in frontal and parietal cortices (5,6). The occipital cortex was not evaluated in these reports. Neuropsychological studies have identified a disturbed body image perception in anorectic patients (4), a cognitive function that is attributed to parietal cortex. Others also have reported deficits in attention (3) and problem solving abilities (2,3), which are mediated by the frontal cortex, and deficits in digit symbol test (25), visual-spatial construction (2,26) and mental arithmetic (27), which are related to parietal functions. However, it remains unclear whether this reduction in BI is a cause or a consequence of AN and whether this reduction is trait or state dependent. Many of the biological findings in low-weight AN patients normalized or were even inversely disturbed after long-term weight restoration (8,10,28) and were state dependent and possibly caused by the low weight or neuroendocrine status. A reduction in 5-HT2a BI can be caused by reduced estrogen concentrations. It has been demonstrated that estrogen increases 5-HT2a receptor expression (29). We did not evaluate estrogen concentrations in the patients in our study. However, because all the women were in a postmenarcheal amenorrheic state, it is probable that estrogen concentration was low and, directly or indirectly, could be the driving force behind the reduction in 5-HT2a expression. Another important confounding factor is reduced food intake, especially reduced intake of protein, because the essential amino acid tryptophan is the precursor of serotonin. Alternatively, a third possible confounding factor is that a reduced BI can occur as a compensatory response to chronic overrelease of 5-HT. Physical hyperactivity, which is common among AN patients, may cause lipolysis of intermuscular lipids, resulting in the release of free fatty acids. These fatty acids displace tryptophan from albumin, leading to an increase in free tryptophan and in 5-HT turnover in the brain (30). All the aforementioned variables -estrogen, protein reduction and physical hyperactivity, can be responsible for the reduction in 5-HT2a BI in our study. However, one would then expect a global reduction in 5-HT2a BI, which is contrary to the regional reduction with a clear sparing of the left and right temporal cortex as found in our study. Our findings also can be confounded by mood status, because depression is a common comorbid disorder among AN patients. This possibility cannot be ruled out; however, no functional imaging study of the 5-HT2a receptor in depressed (31,32) or attempted-suicide patients (20) showed a reduction in parietal BI, a finding that was significantly present in our study. Nor was a frontal asymmetry present in any of these studies of depressed individuals. As a potential methodological limitation of our study, the difference in sex distribution may be a possible confounding factor. However, it is unlikely, because sex differences in BI were not reported with this tracer (33), in agrees with our own (unpublished) data, and because male BI values fell in the range of the female healthy volunteers, as is shown in Figure 2 (see original paper). Because of lack of structural matching, another potential confounding factor in this study may be the result of partial volume averaging. It is known that substantial (and partially reversible) brain volume loss may accompany low-weight status in patients with AN. Therefore, neocortical activity per unit brain volume may have been underestimated for the patient group. It would be unlikely, however, that such a process is location specific and could account for the regional differences in the frontal and parietal neocortex only. In this subgroup, no dynamic SPET acquisitions were made. Hence, the assumption that the imaging outcome measure (the BI) is comparable among patient and control subjects cannot not be made. The pseudoequilibrium conditions for the tracer under consideration were defined with regard to the rate of washout from receptor-rich regions (18). Our own measurements in healthy volunteers (20) have shown that the relative activity in neocortical specific regions with regard to aspecific binding in the cerebellum gradually decreases after 90 minutes after injection and thereafter can be approximated by a linear decrease during the first hours, with a stable specific-to-aspecific activity ratio. The time of measurement after injection (start 110-140 min after injection) lies well after the onset of probable pseudoequilibrium. Therefore, although not directly proven in this population, it can be expected that the neocortical equilibrium of the tracer in AN patients would be found approximately at the same time, even with 20-30 % slower kinetics (i.e. reaching of the plateau phase). Moreover, because several neocortical regions (predominantly temporal areas) are normal in patients with AN when compared with those of healthy volunteers, it is unlikely that a very altered kinetic distribution is present within the affected areas that would invalidate the assumption of pseudo-equilibrium at the time the measurements were performed after injection. In addition, without full kinetic modeling, the contribution of altered perfusion and tracer delivery from altered density of binding sites cannot be distinguished. Hence, the possibility can not be excluded that some portion of the demonstrated changes may not involve serotonergic binding deficits, but altered influx to the brain (KI) particularly in light of previous studies in anorexic patients with perfusion deficits in the frontal and parietal neocortex (6,34). Therefore, this study assumes implicitly that the tracer kinetics are in the pseudo-equilibrium state after an hour and are not altered by regional changes in K1 but by specific binding properties. Investigations in a small subset of patients with complete kinetic modeling should be conducted to validate this assumption. It should also be taken into account that, in addition to serotonin, other neurotransmitter system or hormones have altered or disturbed metabolism in patients with AN. At least theoretically, this is possible with neurotransmitter systems (e.g. norepinephrine and dopamine effects on the serotonergic system) or neurohormonal systems (e.g., cortisol and cortisol-releasing factor effects on the serotonergic system). Because the estimation of the 123 I-5I-R91150 BI is based on the assumption of pseudo-equilibrium, the effects of these intrinsic and neuronal interactions could alter tracer kinetics. Another limitation lies in the fact that the specific-to-nonspecific activty ratio is around 2.0 (BI = 1.0) in the age groups studied. The spread in healthy volunteers, which is the combination of intrinsic interindividual physiological differences as well as uncertainties in tracer delivery (kinetics, distribution, Poisson noise etc.), is fairly high (18,33), whereas the uncertainty of the BI of 1.0 is about 0.2-0.3 (or 2030 %). This uncertainty, however, is incorporated in the study because reference to a group of healthy volunteers was included. CONCLUSION A specific regional reduction in BI of the 5-HT2a receptors is demonstrated in low-weight AN patients when compared with healthy volunteers. State-dependent variables of AN may be causal factors in the BI reduction. To address these variables, future work should aim to replicate the findings of this study in the same diagnostic group and in other eating disorder subgroups. A follow-up study of AN patients with a reevaluation of the 5-HT2a BI at long-term weight restoration could help in determining whether this status is state dependent or a trait variable. ACKNOWLEDGMENT The authors gratefully acknowledge logistic support from Nuclear Diagnostics Ltd. References 1. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 4th ed. Washington DC: American Psychiatric Association; 1994: 539-545. 2. Szmukler G, Andrewes D, Kingston K, Chen L, Stargatt R, Stanley R. Neuropsychological impairment in anorexia nervosa: before and after refeeding. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 1992;14:347-352. 3. Lauer C, Gorzewski B, Gerlinghoff M, Backmund H, Zihl J. Neuropsychological assessments before and after treatment in patients with anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa. J Psychiatr Res. 1999;33:129-138. 4. Horne R, Van-Vactor J, Emerson S. Disturbed body image in patients with eating disorders. Am J Psychiatry. 1991;148:211-215. 5. Delvenne V, Goldman S, De Maertelaer V, Lotstra F. Brain glucose metabolism in eating disorders assessed by positron emission tomography. Int J Eating Dis. 1999;25:29-37. 6. Nozoe S, Naruo T, Yonekura R, et al. Comparison of regional cerebral blood flow in patients with eating disorders. Brain Res Bull. 1995;36:251-255. 7. Coppen AJ, Gupta RK, Eccleston EG, Wood KM, Wakeling A. Plasma tryptophan in anorexia nervosa. Lancet. 1976;1(7966): 961. 8. Kaye W, Gwirtsman H, George D, Ebert M. Altered serotonin activity in anorexia nervosa after long term weight restoration: Does elevated cerebrospinal fluid 5-hydroxyindolacetic acied level correlate with rigid and obsessive behaviour? Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1991;48:556-562. 9. Brewerton T, Jimerson D. Studies of serotonin function in anorexia nervosa. Psychiatry Res. 1996;62:31-42. 10. O'Dwyer ALJ, Russell G. Serotonin activity in anorexia nervosa after long-term weight restoration: response to Dfenfluramine challenge. Psychol Med. 1996;26:353-359. 11. Monteleone P, Brambilla F, Bortolotti F, La Rocca A, Maj M. Prolactin response to d-fenfluramine is blunted in people with anorexia nervosa. Br J Psychiatry. 1998;172:438-442. 12. Berk M, Kessa K, Szabo CP, Butkow N. The augmented platelet intracellular calcium response to serotonin in anorexia nervosa but not bulimia may be due to subsyndromal depression. Int J Eat Disord. 1997;22:57-63. 13. Spigset O, Andersen T, Hagg S, Mjondal T. Enhanced platelet serotonin 5-HT2A receptor binding in anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 1999;9:469-473. 14. Kaye W, Frank F, Meltzer C, et al. Altered serotonin 2A receptor activity in women who recovered from bulimia nervosa. Am J Psychiatry 2001;158, 1152-1155. 15. Collier D, Arranz M, Li T, Mupita D, Brown N, Treasure J. Association between the 5-HT2A gene promotor polymorphism and anorexia nervosa. Lancet. 1997;350 (9075):412. 16. Treasure J, Campbell I. The case for biology in the aetiology of anorexia nervosa. Psychol Med. 1994;24:3-8. 17. Mertens J, Terriere D, Sipido V, Gommeren W, Janssen P, Leysen J. Radiosynthesis of a new radioiodinated ligand for serotonin-5HT2-receptors, a promising tracer for gamma-emission tomography. J Labelled Compounds Radiopharmaceuticals. 1994;34:795-806. 18. Busatto G, Pilowsky L, Costa D, et al. Initial evaluation of 123I-5-I-R91150, a selective 5-HT2A ligand for single-photon emission tomography, in healthy subjects. Eur J Nucl Med. 1997;24:119-124. 19. Travis M, Busatto G, Pilowsky L, et al. 5-HT2a receptor blockade in patients with schizophrenia treated with risperidone and clozapine. A SPET study using the novel 5-HT2a ligand 123I-5-I-R-91150. Br J Psychiatry. 1998;173:236-241. 20. Audenaert K, Van Laere K, Dumont F, et al. Decreased frontal serotonin 5-HT2a receptor binding index in deliberate self harm patients. Eur J Nucl Med. 2000;27:1800-1808. 21. Van Laere K, Koole M, Kauppinen T, Monsieurs M, Bouwens L, Dierckx R. Non-uniform transmission in brain SPET using 201-Tl, 99m-Tc and 153-Gd static line sources: antropomorphic dosimetry studies and brain quantification. J Nucl Med. 2000;41:2051-2062. 22. Van Laere K, Koole M, D'Asseler Y, et al. Automated stereotactic standardization of brain receptor data using singlephoton transmission images. J Nucl Med. 2001;42:361-375. 23. Van Laere K, Versijpt J, Audenaert K, et al. 99mTc-ECD brain perfusion SPET: variability, asymmetry and effects of age and gender in healthy adults. Eur J Nucl Med. 2001; 28: 873-887. 24. Pazos A, Probst A, Palacios J. Serotonin receptors in the human brain-IV. Autoradiographic mapping of serotonin-2 receptors. Neuroscience. 1987;21:123-139. 25. Palazidou E, Robinson P, Lishman W. Neuroradiological and neuropsychological assessment in anorexia nervosa. Psychol Med. 1990;20:521-527. 26. Mathias J, Kent P. Neuropsychological consequences of extreme weight loss and dietary restriction in patients with anorexia nervosa. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 1998;20:548-564. 27. Hamsher K, Halmi K, Benton A. Prediction of outcome in anorexia nervosa from neuropsychological status. Psychiatry Res. 1981;4:79-88. 28. Delvenne V, Goldman D, De Maertelaer V, Simon Y, Luxen A, Lotstra F. Brain hypometabolism of glucose in anorexia nervosa: normalization after weight gain. Biol Psychiatry. 1996;40:761-768. 29. Connan F, Treasure J. Stress, eating and neurobiology. In: Hoek H, Treasure J, Katzman M, eds. Neurobiology in the treatment of eating disorders. Chicester: John Wiley & Sons; 1998:211-234. 30. Fisher G, Hollman W, De Meirleir K. Exercise changes in plasma tryptophan fraction and relationship with prolactin. Int J Sports Med. 1991;12:487-489. 31. Biver F, Wikler D, Lotstra F, Damhaut P, Goldman S, Mendlewicz J. Serotonin 5-HT2 receptor imaging in major depression: focal changes in orbito-insular cortex. Br J Psychiatry. 1997;171:444-448. 32. Massou JM, Trichard C, Attar-Levy D, et al. Frontal 5-HT2A receptors studied in depressive patients during chronic treatment by selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 1997;133:99-101. 33. Baeken C, D'Haenen H, Flamen P, et al. 123I-5-I-R91150, a new single-photon emission tomography ligand for 5-HT2a receptors: influence of age and gender in healthy subjects. Eur J Nucl Med. 1998;25:1617-1622. 34. Delvenne V, Goldman D, De Maertelaer V, Wikler D, Damhaut P, Lotstra F. Brain glucose metabolism in anorexia nervosa and affective disorders: influence of weight loss or depressive symptomatology. Psychiatry Res. 1997;74:83-92. 3. Comparison of Cortical 5-HT2A Receptor Binding in Bulimia Nervosa Patients and healthy volunteers. 10 ABSTRACT Objective: Bulimia nervosa has been associated with alterations in central serotonergic (5-HT) function. This study investigated iodine-labelled 4-amino-N-[1-[3-(4-fluorophenoxy)propyl]-4-methyl-4-piperidinyl]-5-iodo-2methoxybenzamide (123I-5-I-R91150) binding to the 5-HT2A receptor in the brain by using single photon emission computed tomography (SPECT) in acutely ill bulimia nervosa patients. Method: Cortical 123I-5-IR91150 binding in 10 normal-weight patients with bulimia nervosa, purging type, was compared with that of 11 healthy volunteers. Results: The 5-HT2A binding index of the bulimia nervosa patients, with and without correction for age, was not significantly different from that of the comparison group. Conclusions: As a group, acutely ill BN patients cannot be discriminated from healthy subjects on the basis of cortical 123I-5-I-R91150 binding index. Bulimia nervosa is characterized by binge eating and inappropriate compensatory behaviours to avoid weight gain. The aetiology of bulimia nervosa likely involves interactions among genetic, psychological and biological variables. Of interest is the attention given to neurobiological alterations. Several authors have addressed the role of the serotonin (5-HT) neurotransmitter system in bulimia nervosa. In the majority of these studies, indirect measurements of central 5-HT function were used, and contradictory results were reported (1). Single photon emission computed tomography (SPECT) allows for a more direct exploration of the central 5-HT system. The SPECT radiopharmaceutical, iodine-labelled 4-amino-N-[1-[3-(4-fluorophenoxy)propyl]-4-methyl-4-piperidinyl]-5-iodo-2- methoxybenzamide (123I-5-I-R91150), specifically binds to the 5-HT2A receptor in the brain (2). Recently, we used 123 I-5-I-R91150 and SPECT to compare 5-HT2A receptor availability in anorexia nervosa patients and healthy comparison subjects and found lower 5-HT2A receptor availability in the left frontal, bilateral parietal and occipital regions in the anorexia nervosa patients (3). The present study reports on 123I-5-I-R91150 binding in acutely ill bulimia nervosa patients. 10 Goethals I, Vervaet M, Van De Wiele C, Audenaert K, Slegers G, van Heeringen C, Dierckx R (2004). Binding potential of cortical 5-HT2A receptors is not different in bulimia nervosa patients and healthy volunteers. American Journal of Psychiatry. 161:10, 1916-1918. METHOD We studied 10 normal-weight patients who met the DSM-IV criteria for bulimia nervosa, purging type without a history of anorexia nervosa (nine women, one man). Duration of the eating disorder varied between 1 and 4 years. None of the patients had a concomitant psychiatric disorder. Patients were free of psychotropic medication for at least 1 year. Additional exclusion criteria were a history of major medical or neurological disorder, previous ECT, current pregnancy or lactation, and abnormal menstrual function. Depressive symptoms were rated with the Beck Depression Inventory. Eleven medication-free healthy volunteers (seven women, four men) without a personal or family (first-degree relatives) history of neurological and psychiatric disorder, served as the comparison group. Detailed characteristics of this group were described elsewhere (3). This study was approved by the local ethics committee. Written informed consent was obtained from each subject. All subjects had the same standardized, low-protein breakfast on the morning of the study. Thyroid blockade was achieved by oral administration of 100 mg potassium chloride. Subjects received 185 MBq of 123 I-5-I-R91150 intravenously as a single bolus. Transmission and emission SPECT scans were performed at pseudoequilibrium (2). After automatic image coregistration to stereotactic space, a predefined volume-of-interest analysis was performed. Radioactivity estimates in the cortex represent total ligand binding. Since the cerebellum contains only a small number of 5-HT2A receptors (4), and therefore represents non-specific activity, calculation of binding index was performed by volume-ofinterest normalization to the activity-per-volume element in the cerebellum. As binding index values were normally distributed, between-group comparisons of mean binding index were made by using two-tailed group t tests. Since 5-HT2A binding was shown to decline with age (5), between-group comparisons were recalculated using the general linear model procedure with age as a covariate. Correlation analyses were used to examine relationships between binding index values and psychopathological data. RESULTS The 21 individuals in this study had an average age of 23.6 years (SD=3.5; range= 16.9-29.3). Mean ages were significantly different between the two study groups (patients: mean=21.3 years (SD 3.2); healthy volunteers: mean=25.6 years (SD 2.5)) (U=18, p=0.01). The two groups did not differ significantly in gender (p=0.30, Fisher’s Exact test) or body mass index (U=34.5, p=0.25). None of the patients met criteria for a major depressive episode nor for any other comorbid psychiatric disorder at the moment of the SPECT scanning. The Beck Depression Inventory score was below 10 in all patients. Correlation analysis, in the bulimia nervosa patients as well as in the healthy subjects, revealed a significant (at the 5% level) negative relationship between age and binding index in the prefrontal cortex. In addition, a nonsignificantly negative correlation between age and binding index was found in the temporal , parietal, and occipital regions. Further, there were no significant relationships between binding index values and gender, body mass index, or disease duration. The mean 5-HT2A binding index in the bulimia nervosa patients, with and without correction for age, was not significantly different in any predefined cortical brain region from the mean binding index values in the healthy comparison subjects (Table 1). DISCUSSION In this study, no changes in cortical 5-HT2A receptor binding, assessed by 123 I-5-I-R91150 and SPECT, were observed in acutely ill bulimia nervosa patients, relative to healthy comparison subjects. In addition, a significant negative relationship between binding index and age in the prefrontal cortex indicated that in bulimia nervosa patients, as well as in healthy subjects, an age-related decline in 5-HT2A receptor availability was preserved. These results agree with those of the literature (5). In a previous (indirect) study, no significantly difference in CSF levels of 5-hydroxyindoleacetate acid (5-HIAA) was found between bulimia nervosa patients and healthy comparison subjects (6). This finding could indicate that central 5-HT turnover is not disturbed in acutely ill bulimia nervosa patients. Yet, in another study using 123 I- -CIT and SPECT, a reduction of available binding sites in 5-HT transporter- rich areas was documented in 10 medication-free female patients with bulimia, compared with 10 agematched healthy female subjects (7). Although decreased 123 I- -CIT binding may reflect central 5-HT dysfunction, secondary changes may involve the postsynaptic 5-HT2A receptor as well. In a positron emission tomography study using 18F-altanserin, a ligand for the postsynaptic 5-HT2A receptor, reduced binding in the orbital frontal cortex was found in patients who recovered from bulimia nervosa (8). Based on the observation that recovered bulimia nervosa patients have elevated CSF 5-HIAA concentrations (9) and in agreement with in vitro studies (10), the authors postulated that 5-HT2A receptors were down-regulated. Based on our findings, acutely ill bulimia nervosa patients are not characterized by altered cortical 5-HT2A receptor binding in specific (prefrontal) brain regions. These results clearly differ from those in acutely ill AN patients (3), suggesting that different biological mechanisms are involved in patients with different types of eating disorders. In reporting our findings we are aware of several limitations. A major limitation of this study was the small number of subjects. Consequently, we did not correct for a large number of potential confounding factors, such as psychopathological variables. References The authors thank Koen Van Laere, M.D., Ph.D., Dr.Sc. for comments on the manuscript and Erik Nolf, M.Sc. for technical advise. 1. Walsh BT, Devlin MJ: Eating disorders: progress and problems. Science 1998; 280:1387-1390 2. Busatto GF, Pilowsky LS, Costa DC, Mertens J, Terriere D, Ell PJ, Mulligan R, Travis MJ, Leysen JE, Lui D, Gacinovic S, Waddington W, Lingford-Hughes A, Kerwin RW: Initial evaluation of 123I-5-I-R91150, a selective 5-HT2A ligand for singlephoton emission tomography, in healthy human subjects. Eur J Nucl Med 1997; 24:119-124 3. Audenaert K, Van Laere K, Dumont F, Vervaet M, Goethals I, Slegers G, Mertens J, van Heeringen C, Dierckx RA: Decreased 5-HT2a receptor binding in patients with anorexia nervosa. J Nucl Med 2003; 44:163-169 4. Pazos A, Probst A, Palacios JM: Serotonin receptors in the human brain--IV. Autoradiographic mapping of serotonin-2 receptors. Neuroscience 1987; 21:123-139 5. Baeken C, D'haenen H, Flamen P, Mertens J, Terriere D, Chavatte K, Boumon R, Bossuyt A: 123I-5-I-R91150, a new single-photon emission tomography ligand for 5- HT2A receptors: influence of age and gender in healthy subjects. Eur J Nucl Med 1998; 25:1617-1622 6. Kaye WH, Ballenger JC, Lydiard RB, Stuart GW, Laraia MT, O'Neil P, Fossey MD, Stevens V, Lesser S, Hsu G: CSF monoamine levels in normal-weight bulimia: evidence for abnormal noradrenergic activity. Am J Psychiatry 1990; 147:225229 7. Tauscher J, Pirker W, de Zwaan M, Asenbaum S, Brucke T, Kasper S: In vivo visualization of serotonin transporters in the human brain during fluoxetine treatment. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 1999; 9:177-179 8. Kaye WH, Frank GK, Meltzer CC, Price JC, McConaha CW, Crossan PJ, Klump KL, Rhodes L: Altered serotonin 2A receptor activity in women who have recovered from bulimia nervosa. Am J Psychiatry 2001; 158:1152-1155 9. Kaye WH, Greeno CG, Moss H, Fernstrom J, Fernstrom M, Lilenfeld LR, Weltzin TE, Mann JJ: Alterations in serotonin activity and psychiatric symptoms after recovery from bulimia nervosa. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1998; 55:927-935 10. Barnes NM, Sharp T: A review of central 5-HT receptors and their function. Neuropharmacology 1999; 38:1083-1152 General conclusions Eating disorders have emerged as clinical problems of growing relevance due to their increasing prevalence, substantial somatic and psychiatric co-morbidity, increased mortality, frequent relapse and common failure of treatment. Eating disorders occur predominantly during adolescence or early adulthood, and the ‘core psychopathology’ can be described as an over-concern about body-shape and body-weight resulting in eating-disordered thinking and behaviour. This over-evaluation of body-shape and body-weight is probably based on an extreme need for control, expressed as self-starvation in anorectic patients, or on a fear of rejection, which motivates bulimic patients to strive for ideal body shapes. During the past decades, aetiological models have to a varying extent, emphasized the role of a balance between nature and nurture but increasing insight, have not yet clarified the association between body dissatisfaction and self-esteem on the one hand and unsuccessful dieting on the other hand. The first three papers in Part 1 of this thesis examine factors, which may place individuals at risk of ‘weight concerns’ and ‘dieting’, and describe the relation between these factors and the development of eating disorders. The risk of disturbed eating behaviour associated with a focus on dieting was studied five years after treatment for obesity, combining cognitive-behavioural treatment with a very low calorie diet (VLCD) or a low calorie diet (LCD). Significantly more patients in the VLCD-group than in the LCD-group reported bingeing. More than half of the successfully treated women (BMI ≤ 30) reported binge eating, and restrained eating was as common in successfully treated as in unsuccessfully treated women. Better treatment outcome was associated with less external and emotional eating, and with a smaller quantity of food intake in the case of bingeing. The second study clearly showed a dissatisfaction with body shape among female adolescents, as desired body weights were clearly lower than current BMI’s and the medical standard. Consequently, it is not surprising that female adolescents commonly reported restrained eating and, to a lesser extent, vomiting, laxative abuse, counting of calories, and skipping meals in order to lose weight. This restrained eating style was frequently associated with emotional eating. On the contrary, boys reported a higher desired than current BMI. Although restrained eating and vomiting was also found in boys, the occurrence of these characteristics decreased with age, which was not the case in girls. A preoccupation with body weight in school-girls was confirmed in the third study, in which their attitudes towards eating were compared to those among fashion models and eating disordered patients. Vomiting, the use of anorectic drugs and laxatives, eating alone, counting calories and the experience of a loss of control were reported by a substantial proportion of adolescent girls. Fashion models tended to have an eating-style that was comparable to that of patients with eating disorders. The main differences were a more outspoken intensity of disturbed eating patterns and the more commonly reported premorbid overweight in ED patients. Probably, this overweight is a risk factor for developing eating-related psychopathology in a Western society with a slimming culture. Without denying the importance of contextual variables, further research targeted the study of a vulnerability, which may place individuals at an increased risk of developing an eating disorder. In addition, it was studied whether there were differences in vulnerabilities between and within the eating disorder categories. Evidence of an overlap of symptoms in binge-eating disordered (BED) and bulimia nervosa non-purging (BN-NP) patients was indeed found. In our study population, BED patients were older and had a longer duration of illness, a larger weight cycling, a higher current and previous BMI, and a lower score on the temperament dimension ‘Persistence’ than BN-NP patients. After correcting for age, the differences for weight variables remained, including comparatively higher BMIs and larger weight fluctuations among the BED patients. These differences may well be due to a genetically determined propensity to be overweight and to the temperamental characteristic of Persistence. Due to personality characteristics these patients are apparently indeed not able to restrain, which clearly differentiates them from BN-NP patients. Also within the group of anorectic patients, understanding of this restrained eating style is not straight forward. Indeed, an intense fear of increasing weight, even when underweight, is one of the core features of AN patients. A low or absent drive for thinness in more than one-quarter of our AN patients, particularly among those of the restricting type, is therefore, at the least, remarkable. A low drive for thinness was associated with a higher age of onset of the eating disorder, less eating-related pathology (lower frequency of purging and less restrained eating) and less severe psychopathology (lower harm avoidance and higher self-directedness). Weight concerns may thus have other motivations than a culture-bound drive for thinness. Historical descriptions of cases of self-starvation without weight concerns in cultures, in which there was no emphasis on slimness, and cross-cultural comparison have suggested that AN does not necessarily follow the accepted Western format of thinness. It could well be that reinforcing effects of food restriction per se play a crucial role. Indeed starvation in AN patients could serve to reduce serotonergic neuronal activity in response to their trait-related (i.e. premorbid and persisting after recovery) increase in 5-HT neuronal transmission. In support of this hypothesis, we demonstrated an association between a low drive for thinness and restricting rather than binging/purging behaviour. Thus, this mechanism appears to be relatively effective in RAN patients, in view of the comparatively less severe eating disorder-related symptoms associated with a comparatively lower harm avoidance and a higher self-directedness. From a therapeutic point of view, the use of food restriction to judge self-worth, in order to cope with anxiety, requires cognitive restructuring procedures, perhaps facilitated by a pharmacologic (i.e. serotonergic) reduction of trait-dependent anxiety. The current findings suggest also a role of serotonergic agonists in the prevention of attempted suicide among eating disordered patients. Attempted suicide and eating disorders share a more common prevalence among females, while their onset often occurs during adolescence. Both behaviours can be used as a means of taking control over one’s own body. Previous studies have demonstrated that suicidal behaviour is relatively common among eating disordered patients and that its occurrence may be related to co-morbid axis-I disorders, to core ED-symptoms and to personality characteristics. In our study, a history of suicide attempt was more common among BN patients than among AN patients or EDNOS patients. The diathesis among AN patients appears to differ from that in BN and EDNOS patients. First, and in keeping with the results of previous studies in AN and BN patients, co-morbidity with depression appears to be a specific risk of attempted suicide in BN, but not in AN. Secondly, an increased risk of attempted suicide is associated with a combination of specific dimensions of personality, i.e. high harm avoidance, moderate to high novelty seeking and low self-directedness. In case of AN, commonly characterized by a lower novelty seeking compared to the two other diagnostic groups, a higher scores on novelty seeking was indeed associated with a higher frequency of attempted suicide. The current findings indicate that the combination of moderate to high novelty seeking, increased harm avoidance and low self-directedness among eating disordered patients may become manifest through cognitive and behavioural problems including increased drive for thinness, bulimia, body dissatisfaction, ineffectiveness, perfectionism, interpersonal distrust, interoceptive awareness, maturity fears and external and emotional eating. While no causal interpretation of this association can be inferred from the data, the results suggest that these characteristics can be regarded as cognitive coping strategies reflecting a shift from the interpersonal to the personal domain and from a psychological to a somatic focus in an attempt to increase perceived control. Taking into account the stress-diathesis model of suicidal behaviour, the current findings suggest that the anxiety and low selfdirectedness involved in the development of eating disorders, in absence of the rigidity due to a low novelty seeking, increases the risk of a break down of the inhibition, and thus to more disinhibition or impulsive behaviour. Unfortunately, intent to die was not assessed in the current study, but previous studies report that attempted suicide in AN differs from that in bulimia nervosa by a relatively more marked intent to die. Moreover, as both attempted suicide and completed suicide appear to be, at least in part, under genetic control, further study is needed with regard to genetic influences on the occurrence of suicidal behaviour in eating-disordered patients. The last part of this thesis described the extent to which personality dimensions are associated with specific eating-disordered cognitive and behavioural characteristics, using data on the total study population (n = 800). The findings were in keeping with those as reported in papers 7 and 8, which were based on a smaller population. When compared to healthy volunteers, ED patients were cautious, nervous, timid, doubtful, discouraged, insecure, passive, and negativistic, even in situations that do not worry other people, which reflects their high HA. This important predisposing factor was combined with difficulties in pursuing self-chosen goals and values, and by a tendency to consider other people and situations responsible for frustrations due to a reduced self-directedness. ED patients showed a greater tendency to intolerance, criticism, and opportunism than controls. Due to their clinical perfectionism, they lack the needed flexibility and showed maladaptive perseveration. When differences between the ED categories RAN, BPAN, BP were studied, BP patients were less organized, methodical and systematic, with higher frustration in uncertain situations and less satisfaction with what they have when compared to AN patients. Differences in traits were most marked between the RAN and the BP group, as the previously described differences were more outspoken, with an additional by a higher persistence (P) in the RAN group. These differences were not due to age, duration of illness or age at onset of the eating disorder. The findings confirmed the hypothesized association between Novelty Seeking and BMI, HBMI, LBMI, bulimia, external and emotional eating and vomiting and/or laxative abuse. The negative correlation between drive for thinness and NS, and the lack of a correlation between body dissatisfaction and NS were unexpected. The hypothesized negative correlation between Persistence and BMI, HBMI, LBMI, external and emotional eating, and bulimia was also confirmed. The positive relation between Persistence and restrained eating (p<0.01) and the absence of a (negative) correlation with drive for thinness was unexpected. Of interest was also the positive correlation between excessive exercise (mostly seen in the RAN group) and Persistence. With regard to the three key features of eating disorders i.e. body dissatisfaction, perfectionism and interpersonal distrust, following correlations were found. A correlation between body dissatisfaction and HA (+) and SD (-) was found in all three diagnostic groups (RAN, BPAN and BN). Perfectionism correlated especially positively with HA and P, and negatively with NS and SD. Interpersonal distrust correlated positively with HA and negatively with NS, RD and SD. Body dissatisfaction thus appears to play a central role in the basic spectrum of all three ED’s, whereas perfectionism seems to play a more important role in the RAN type and interpersonal distrust correlated with all personality dimensions in the BP group. The BPAN group has features of the RAN and the BP group. Only positive correlations between the eating pathology-related cognitive and behavioural characteristics were found. Levels of Body dissatisfaction correlated with scores on all eating-disordered behaviours, except for excessive exercise, and both ‘Perfectionism’ and ‘Interpersonal Distrust’ correlated with restrained eating and laxative abuse. The outspoken emotion-driven rigidity in RAN patients may interfere with successful cognitive behaviour therapy, and these patients may thus benefit from psychopharmacological treatment. Although the mechanism of neurotransmitter action is not yet clear, a genetically determined vulnerability may be expressed via the central serotonergic system. Functional brain imaging with PET and SPECT has shown diminished metabolic activity and perfusion in frontal and parietal cortices, and demonstrated disturbed central serotonergic function in AN patients. Also in our study, patients with AN, when compared to healthy volunteers, had a significantly reduced 5-HT2a receptor binding index (BI) in the left frontal cortex, bilateral parietal and occipital cortex. A significant left-right asymmetry was noted in the frontal cortex (left < right). Neuropsychological studies in AN patients found cognitive deficits reflecting frontal and parietal dysfunctions. The disturbed body image perception and deficits in digit symbol test, visual-spatial construction and mental arithmetic can be attributed to changes in parietal cortical functioning. Deficits in attention and problem-solving abilities can be related to diminished frontal cortical activity. Confounding factors as estrogen and protein reduction and physical hyperactivity could explain the described reduction in 5-HT2a BI, but the clear sparing of the left and right temporal cortex does not support this explanation. Functional imaging study of the 5HT2a receptor in depressed or attempted-suicide patients showed no reduction in parietal BI, nor a frontal asymmetry. Although the effect of serotonergic pharmacological therapy was demonstrated in depressed patients, similar research in eating disorders yielded contradictory results. A comparison of the cortical 5-HT2a receptor binding between 10 normal-weight BP patients with 11 healthy volunteers demonstrated no significant differences. These biological findings could support our psychological results, suggesting that divergent mechanisms are involved in patients with different types of eating disorders. Enhancing self-directedness and treating the comorbidity in terms of comorbid depression by serotonergic drugs, may nevertheless enhance the effective treatment of bulimic patients. The implications for treatment and prevention are an important impetus for the study of personality characteristics in eating disordered patients. According to Cloninger’s psychobiological model, and thus assuming that character and temperament involve concept-based and percept-driven memory, stable personality change requires habit modification by conceptual insights, perhaps facilitated by combined pharmacotherapy. The normalization of eating patterns, and an increase in self-esteem and in social skills by promoting conflict-handling and problemsolving capacities, are the most important therapeutic goals. This means that the cognitive behavioural treatment of eating disorders has to include perceptual (i.e. temperamental) and conceptual (i.e. character-based) learning processes in which classical and operant conditioning procedures have their place. Cue exposure to binge food, systematic desensitisation with forbidden food, and approach behaviour to reinforcing social situations are effective components by increasing predictability and efficacy. Combined pharmacotherapy could well have a facilitating effect on such learning procedures since anxiety and rigidity induce and maintain avoidance and escape behaviour. The role of drugs targeting the dopaminergic system in the treatment of eating disorders, and particularly their effect on cognitive and behavioural characteristics as identified in this study is yet to be established. The role of the low score on Novelty Seeking in anorectic patients and the link with the dopaminergic system will be the target of further research. In addition, neuropsychological investigations are needed to explore deficits in cognitive functioning in the ED groups and to possibly differentiate between ED and other psychiatric categories. Nederlandse samenvatting Eetstoornissen zijn complexe psychiatrische aandoeningen die vooral voorkomen bij meisjes in de adolescentie. De incidentie van typische eetstoornissen neemt de laatste jaren niet echt toe, maar deze vaak chronische stoornissen kunnen bijzonder ernstige gevolgen hebben op het vlak van morbiditeit en mortaliteit. Er is evidentie voor de effectiviteit van de cognitief-gedragstherapeutische behandeling bij bulimia nervosa, maar zeker in het geval van anorexia nervosa, ontbreekt duidelijkheid over de causaliteit van eetstoornissen. Gangbare etiologische modellen situeren zich op het continuüm nature-nurture. Het stress-diathese model vertrekt van de hypothese dat gebeurtenissen bij een kwetsbaar (genetisch bepaald) persoon op zo’n manier gepercipiëerd, geïnterpreteerd en onthouden worden dat ze aanleiding kunnen geven tot ziektegedrag dat resulteert in een ziektetoestand. Afhankelijk van de mate (zowel in verscheidenheid als intensiteit) van de kwetsbaarheid kunnen meerdere toestandsbeelden zich voordoen of kan de ziekte evolueren naar een chronische toestand. Dit doctoraatsproefschrift handelt over persoonlijkheidstrekken bij patiënten met eetstoornissen (ED’s) en hun relatie met de aan de ziekte gebonden cognitieve karaktertistieken en gedragsvariabelen. Hierbij wordt gebruik gemaakt van een psychobiologisch persoonlijkheidsmodel dat zich het inzicht in het neurobiologisch functioneren van de informatieverwerking tot doel stelt. Bij ernstige eetstoornissen kan op basis van een valide biopsychologisch model worden verondersteld dat een combinatie van psychologische interventies met farmacologische ondersteuning de effectiviteit en de efficiëntie van de behandeling verbeteren. Op vlak van de individuele patiënt wordt beoogd dat het dagelijks (sociaal) functioneren meer gecontroleerd en voorspelbaar wordt gemaakt met een groter welbevinden als doel. In enkele voorbereidende studies (Part I) werd bij een aantal kwetsbare groepen (obese vrouwen, adolescente schoolmeisjes en mode modellen) het risico op het ontwikkelen van gestoord eetgedrag bestudeerd. In het eerste artikel wordt aangetoond dat obese vrouwen vijf jaar na een multidisciplinaire behandeling voor hun overgewicht meer eetbuien vertonen als zij in hun therapie een strenger dieet volgden (‘very low calorie diet’ versus ‘low calorie diet’). Eetbuien kwamen ook voor bij vrouwen bij wie de behandeling succesvol was (BMI ≤ 30), maar ze waren minder frequent en beperkter qua hoeveelheid. Uit de tweede en derde studie bleek dat veel meisjes, ongeacht hun gewicht, willen vermageren met het oog op een ideaal gewicht dat onder de medische standaard ligt. Dit geeft aanleiding tot een wijdverspreid lijngedrag en, in mindere mate, braken, gebruiken van laxeermiddelen, tellen van calorieën en overslaan van maaltijden. Dit lijngedrag komt vaak samen voor met eetbuien. Gestoord eetgedrag werd nog vaker gerapporteerd door modellen wiens eetstijl veel gelijkenissen vertoont met die van meisjes met eetstoornissen, zij het in minder extreme mate. Bij meisjes met eetstoornissen wordt echter vaker een premorbied overgewicht vastgesteld. Jongens, daarentegen, willen bijkomen in gewicht en het voorkomen van gestoord eetgedrag nam bij hen af met de leeftijd. Het tweede deel van dit proefschrift (Part II) bestaat uit vijf artikels, waarin een specifiek persoonlijkheidsprofiel bij vrouwen met eetstoornissen wordt beschreven. Bovendien blijken nuanceringen in dit profiel al naargelang de diagnostische categorieën, vooral met betrekking tot restrictieve anorectische vrouwen (RAN) en purgerende bulemische vrouwen (BP). Om dit te onderzoeken werden ED’s vergeleken met een vrouwelijke controlepopulatie (CW) en werden diagnostische categorieën onderling vergeleken. In de studiepopulatie (n = 800 ED’s en n = 326 CW’s) werden ED patiënten gekenmerkt door een relatief hoge ‘harm avoidance’ (HA) en lage ‘selfdirectedness’ (SD), gecombineerd met een hogere score voor ‘persistentie’ (P) en een lagere score voor ‘reward dependence’ (RD) en ‘cooperativeness’ (C) in vergelijking met gezonde vrouwen. Dat betekent dat ED patiënten relatief angstig zijn, moeite hebben om zich te hechten, perfectionistisch zijn in combinatie met verminderde capaciteiten om hun emoties te sturen of om coöperatief te zijn. Bij de onderlinge vergelijking werden enkel typische eetstoornissen (anorexia en bulimia nervosa) betrokken. De niet-purgerende bulemische groep (B-NP) werd apart bestudeerd wegens de grote gelijkenis met een subcategorie van de aspecifieke eetstoornissen, met name de eetbuienstoornis (BED). Naast het feit dat patiënten met BED eetbuien niet compenseren en ouder zijn, hebben zij, zelfs na een correctie voor leeftijd, een hoger premorbid gewicht en een lagere persistentie. Vermoedelijk maakt dit lijnen voor hen extra moeilijk. Bij de vergelijking van de andere typische eetstoornissen wordt bij de restrictieve anorectische groep een lagere NS, hogere P en hogere SD gevonden in vergelijking met de purgerende bulemische groep. Dit kan worden vertaald in een emotioneel doorgedreven rigiditeit, die ook klinisch zo herkenbaar is bij deze RAN groep. Deze verschillen konden niet toegeschreven worden aan leeftijd, ziekteduur of aanvangsleeftijd van de ziekte. Bulemische patiënten tonen zich sneller gefrustreerd en ontevreden en leggen de verantwoordelijkheid eerder buiten zichzelf. Naast de categoriale verschillen in persoonlijkheidsdimensies, kon deze differentiatie geassociëerd worden met de diagnostisch beschreven specificiteit in cognitieve aspecten en gedragskenmerken. De gemeten persoonlijkheidsverschillen tussen de RAN en de PB group correleren effectief met meetbaar gedrag en waargenomen cognitieve factoren. Op die manier kon de hypothese bevestigd worden dat, in combinatie met een lage SD, hoe hoger de NS was, hoe hoger de ‘body mass index’ (BMI), de hoogste BMI ooit gehad (HBMI), extern en emotioneel eten, zelf-opgewekt braken en/of misbruik van laxeermiddelen en ‘bulimia’ was. Dit zijn eigenschappen die, zoals diagnostisch verondersteld en in deze studie gemeten, meer voorkomen bij de BP groep. In tegenstelling tot onze veronderstelling, konden we geen negatieve correlatie tussen ‘drive for thinnes’ en NS weerhouden noch een positieve correlatie tussen ‘body dissatisfaction’ en NS. Opnieuw zoals verwacht werd een negatieve correlatie gevonden tussen ‘Persistence’, BMI, HBMI, laagste BMI na de groeispurt, ‘External’ en ‘Emotional Eating’en ‘bulimia’. Deze eigenschappen scoren ook effectief het laagst bij de RAN groep. Bovendien werd een positieve correlatie gevonden tussen Persistence en ‘excessieve beweging’; die klinisch duidelijk meest aanwezig is bij de RAN groep. De positieve correlatie tussen Persistence (P) en ‘restrained eating’ en de afwezigheid van een negatieve correlatie tussen Persistence en ‘drive for thinness’ waren dan weer niet verwacht. In recente toetsbare etiologische modellen zoals deze van Serpell, Vohs, en Fairburn, wordt de aan het negatieve ‘self-esteem’ onlosmakelijk verbonden ‘body dissatisfaction’ als centraal kenmerk van eetstoornissen weerhouden. Deze uitgesproken preoccupatie met lichaamsvorm en uiterlijk kan beschouwd worden als vemijdingsgedrag in confrontatie met een gepercipiëerde te bedreigende realiteit en zou uitgelokt en sterk onderhouden worden door perfectionisme (vooral ‘Self-Oriented Perfectionism’). Mogelijks zou dit vermijdingsgedrag sterk bekrachtigd worden omdat patiënten met eetstoornissen zich onveilig voelen in sociale situaties (‘interpersonal distrust’). Deze sociale overgevoeligheid kan voortvloeien uit het persoonlijkheidskenmerk perfectionisme (vooral ‘Social Prescribed Perfectionism’), maar kan eveneens het gevolg zijn van een persoonlijkheidsdimensie (te lage ‘Reward Dependence’). Deze drie basiskenmerken werden dan ook gemeten bij de drie typische diagnostische categorieën (RAN, BP en BPAN = binge/purger type van anorexia nervosa). De veronderstelde correlaties met de persoonlijkheidsdimensies, afhankelijk van het type eetstoornis, werden bevestigd. Zo werd een duidelijke positieve correlatie tussen HA en ‘body dissatisfaction’ en een negatieve correlatie tussen SD en ‘body dissatisfaction’ gevonden bij de drie diagnostische categorieën. Perfectionisme correleerde positief met HA en P en negatief met NS en SD. ‘Interpersonal distrust’ correleerde positief met HA en negatief met NS, RD en SD. Er werd vastgesteld dat perfectionisme vooral een belangrijke rol speelt bij de RAN groep, terwijl ‘interpersonal distrust’ met alle persoonlijkheidsdimensies correleerde in de BP groep. De BPAN deelde kenmerken met de RAN én de BP groep. De meer uitgesproken temperamentseigenschappen van de RAN groep (lage NS, hoge HA, lage RD, hoge P) in vergelijking met de BP groep en dus ook in vergelijking met de normaal populatie (de scores van de BP groep liggen tussen die van de RAN groep en de gezonde controles), sturen en onderhouden de observeerbare emotioneel gedreven rigiditeit van deze meisjes. De resulterende uithongering, al dan niet gecombineerd met excessieve beweging, kan door verschillende motieven geactiveerd worden. In overeenstemming met de resultaten van historisch en cross-cultureel onderzoek wees één van de onderzoeksbevindingen er immers op dat niet alle restrictieve anorectische meisjes een hoge ‘drive for thinness’ hebben. Voor de meisjes zelf heeft de voedselrestrictie vaak de betekenis van herwonnen controle, puurheid, sublimatie, het zich verheffen boven het materiële, impulsieve en lichamelijke. Biologisch gezien zou de ‘starvation’ echter vooral kunnen geïnitiëerd worden door de gedragsinhibitie ten gevolge van de hoge HA (serotonerg) en lage NS (dopaminerg) met als bekrachtigend effect een reductie van de serotonerge neuronale activiteit wat mogelijks angstreducerend werkt. Dit zou een verklaring kunnen bieden voor het egosyntone karakter van het ziektebeeld en de daaruit volgende ziekte-ontkenning en therapieresistentie. De resultaten uit de vergelijking tussen ED’s met en deze zonder suïcidepoging wijzen eveneens in de richting van een specifieke diathese bij AN patiënten. Vooreerst waren er meer BN patiënten (23%) met een voorgeschiedenis van zelfmoordgedrag in vergelijking met de AN (13,5%) en atypische (EDNOS, 7%) patiënten. Ten tweede waren er meer suïcides in de anorectische groep (n=3; BN: n=0). Daarbij komt dat comorbiditeit met depressie een hoger risico vormt voor zelfmoordgedrag bij BN patiënten, terwijl dit niet het geval is bij AN patiënten. Tenslotte stijgt het risico op een zelfmoordpoging, wanneer een hoge HA gecombineerd is met een matige tot hoge NS en lage SD. Zoals we weten komt deze combinatie van persoonlijkheidsdimensies meestal voor bij BN patiënten. Echter ook bij AN patiënten zien we het risico op een suïcidepoging verhogen, wanneer de NS niet laag is. In gedragstermen zou dit kunnen betekenen dat het door de hoge HA geïnitieerde ‘avoidancebehaviour’ (inhibitie) doorbroken wordt door een matige tot hoge NS (desinhibitie), waarop ‘escapebehaviour’ (eetbuien, zelf-opgewekt braken, zelfmoordpoging) volgt. De doelstelling van dit proefschrift was bij eetstoornissen de diagnostisch beschreven cognitieve factoren en gedragskenmerken te relateren aan een biopsychologisch persoonlijkheidsmodel teneinde de psychologische concepten neurobiologisch te kunnen meten. Hoewel meerdere studies in voorbereiding zijn, werden twee reeds gepubliceerde papers in deze thesis opgenomen. Door middel van functionele beeldvorming vonden we evidentie voor functionele verschillen in cerebrale activiteit tussen AN en BN patiënten die kunnen correleren met persoonlijkheidskenmerken. In een vergelijking met gezonde vrijwilligers, werd door middel van SPECT-onderzoek een significant gereduceerde 5-HT2a receptor binding index (BI) gevonden bij AN patiënten in de links frontale, bilateraal parietale en occipitale cortex. Deze resultaten kunnen een verklaring bieden voor de disfunctionele informatieverwerking, meer specifiek voor de gevonden aandachtsstoornissen, vooral met betrekking tot ‘body image’ (parietaal), en de verminderde problem solving capaciteiten (frontaal). In vergelijking met andere psychiatrische populaties (depressieve patiënten en suïcidepogers) is de frontale assymetrie en de verminderde parietale BI wel heel specifiek voor deze groep. Meer nog, wanneer we dezelfde vergelijking maken maar met BP patiënten, vinden we geen verschillen in 5-HT2a receptor binding index tussen de patiëntengroep en gezonde vrijwilligers. De bekende diagnostische differentiatie tussen anorectische en bulemische meisjes blijkt op die manier gerelateerd te zijn aan persoonlijkheidsverschillen, die neurobiologisch meetbaar zijn. Uiteraard heeft dit therapeutische implicaties. Perceptueel gedreven (temperamentsgebonden en impliciete) en conceptueel gebaseerde (karakteriëel en expliciete) informatieverwerking zal optimaal kunnen beïnvloed worden wanneer men de psychologische interventies baseert op de verschillende leerparadigma’s (klassieke en operante conditionering). Deze psychologische interventies dienen gefaciliteerd te worden door farmacologische ondersteuning indien de temperamentsconstellatie en/of karakterformatie leren niet of onvoldoende toelaat. Strategieën dienen gericht te worden op objectievere informatieverwerking met een shift van de lichaamsgerelateerde focus naar sociale interacties en van ‘vermijdingsgedrag’ naar ‘toenaderingsgedrag’ met uitbreiding van sociale competentie. Op basis van deze resultaten dient, zeker bij de restrictieve anorectische groep, de nadruk gelegd te worden op het ‘Self-oriented’ en ‘Social Prescribed’ perfectionisme, dat het vermijdingsgedrag onderhoudt en dat de patiënt doet “kiezen” voor rigiditeit. Bij BN patiënten zou de therapie kunnen gericht worden op het opnemen van meer eigen verantwoordelijkheid en zelfsturing om een einde te stellen aan het voortdurend ‘ontsnappen’ aan conflictueuze gevoelens en/of situaties. De rol van psychofarmaca, die het serotonerge systeem kunnen reguleren, blijkt belangrijk bij de behandeling van eetstoornissen, maar, zoals uit deze studie kan worden afgeleid, moet de interactie met het dopaminerge en wellicht ook met het noradrenerge (lage RD) systeem verhelderd worden. Eetstoornissen zijn complexe psychiatrische aandoeningen, niet alleen omwille van de consequenties voor het individu, haar familie en omgeving, haar hulpverlener en de gezondheidszorg, maar eveneens omwille van de veelheid van de betrokken persoonlijkheidsdimensies. Dit proefschrift had dan ook als voornaamste doel het leveren van een bijdrage aan het leren begrijpen en respecteren van deze complexiteit. BIJLAGE: GEBRUIKTE AFKORTINGEN 5-HIAA: 5-hydroxyindole acetic acid: the major metabolite of serotonin 5-HT: 5-hydroxytryptamine or serotonin APA: American Psychiatric Association AS: attempted suicide BDI-II-NL: Beck Depression Inventory (Beck et al., Swets & Zeitlinger) BI: binding index BMI: body mass index (G/L2) CSF: cerebrospinal fluid CW: control women DEBQ: Dutch Eating Behaviour Questionnaire (Van Strien, 1986) RE: restrained eating EE: external eating, eating elicited by external stimuli EME: emotional eating: eating elicited by emotional stimuli, DSM-IV: 4th Revised editon of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental disorders As I :Clinical disorders (and other conditions that may be a focus of clinical attention As II : Personality disorders (and mental retardation) Cluster A: paranoïd, schizoïd, schizotypical PD Cluster B : antisocial, borderline, histrionic, narcissistic PD Cluster C: avoidant, dependent, obsessive-compulsive PD As III: General medical conditions As IV: Psychosocial and environmental problems As V: Global assessment of functioning ED: eating disorders AN: anorexia nervosa RAN anorexia nervosa of the restricting type BPAN anorexia nervosa of the binge-purging type. BN: bulimia nervosa BP: purging type BNP: non-purging type of bulimia nervosa EDNOS: eating disorders not otherwise specified (USA) or atypical eating disorders (UK) BED: binge eating disorder EDE: Eating Disorder Examination (Fairburn & Cooper, 1993) EDI: Eating Disorder Inventory (Garner, Olmstead & Polivy, 1983) DT: drive for thinness (subscale) FE: Fischer exact test HBMI: highest lifetime BMI (after 16 years) ICD-10: international classification of diseases (WHO, 1992) LBMI: lowest lifetime BMI LCD: low calorie diet MWU: Mann-Whitney test N: number OCD: obsessive-compulsive disorder OCPD: obsessive-compulsive personality disorder PET: positron emission tomography PSED: partial syndrome of eating disorders RE: restrained eating, the tendency to eat to lose weight SCAN: Schedules for Clinical Assessment in Neuropsychiatry SD: Standard deviation SMR: standardized mortality ratio SPECT : single photon emission computerized tomography TCI: Temperament and Character Inventory; Cloninger et al., 1993 NS: novelty seeking HA: harm avoidance RD: reward dependence P: persistence SD: self-directedness C: cooperativeness ST: self-transcendence TRP: tryptophan, the precursor of 5-HT VLCD: very low calorie diet VOI: volume of interest Eating disorder (ED): syndrome, in which disturbed eating behaviour is the central and meaningful characteristic associated with an extreme worry about or preoccupation with body size and body weight Disturbed eating behaviour: fasting, dieting and/or binge eating, and/or inappropriate compensating behaviour, including self-induced vomiting or misuse of laxatives, diuretics, or enemas, and excessive exercise. Binge eating: eating, in a limited period of time (e.g., within a two-hour period), an amount of food that is definitely larger than most people would eat during a similar period of time and under similar circumstances, combined with a sense of lack of control over this eating behaviour.
© Copyright 2025 Paperzz