Book reading and prosody: comparing literal and sarcastic remarks Debra L. Burnett, Ph.D. Kansas State University Abstract Mothers and children, ages 6-8 years, participated in shared book reading of books with and without sarcastic remarks. Prosody was analyzed using measures of mean fundamental frequency, frequency range, intensity, and duration. Preliminary findings indicated differences in prosody between sarcastic remarks, humorous nonsarcastic remarks, and sincere remarks. Introduction Results The analyzed sample consisted of 327 recorded words measured for pitch (fundamental frequency - F0), intensity (dB), and duration (milliseconds) across the three stories (see Table 1). An analysis of variance (ANOVA) with repeated measures was used to compare prosody for the sarcastic, sincere, and humorous target words (see Figures 1 and 2 for boxplots). Analysis revealed a main effect of story for mean F0 (F(23,303) = 5.01, p=.0072, ηp2=0.31), minimum dB (F(23,303) = 54.01, p<.0001, ηp2=0.29), range dB (F(23,303) = 5.97, p=.0029, ηp2=0.04), and mean dB (F(23,303) = 61.98, p<.0001, ηp2=0.27). Sarcasm is a remark where what is said is not what is meant. In sarcasm, the speaker’s intended meaning is emphasized over the stated meaning. Little is known about prosody and sarcasm during shared book reading. Reading books aloud at home is a common practice: 94% of young children were read to at least once per week with over half engaging in shared book reading daily (Kuo, Franke, Regalado, & Halfon, 2004). Kuo et al. (2004) described parents’ use of prosody that strengthens social-emotional bonds while it directs the child’s attention to the importance of stress and intonation. Mothers may use a higher pitch and slower tempo to highlight language for the child (Martin, 1999). In conversation, sarcasm is often marked by lower pitch and slower tempo (Rockwell, 2000). Therefore, the current study explored the prosody used during book reading when books contained sarcastic remarks, nonsarcastic humorous remarks, and literal remarks. Post hoc analyses were performed using the Ryan-Einot-GabrielWelsch range test (R-E-G-W). For mean F0, the sincere “terrific” was significantly lower in pitch than the sarcastic “terrific” and humorous “nothing,” which were not significantly different from each other. For mean intensity (dB), the sarcastic “terrific” was higher than the humorous “nothing” which was higher than the sincere “terrific.” Duration (ms) was analyzed for the target word “terrific” in two of the stories. Analysis revealed a main effect for story (F(15,159) = 5.98, p<.0001, ηp2=0.05). Method For duration, the sarcastic “terrific” was longer than the sincere “terrific.” Other prosodic measures did not reach significance. Figure 1. Boxplot of Mean F0 Figure 2. Boxplot of Mean dB Terrificsincere Nothinghumor Terrificsarcastic Vocal prosody during sarcastic remarks differed from both literal remarks and remarks that convey humor for measures of intensity. Vocal prosody during sarcastic remarks and nonsarcastic humorous remarks differed from literal remarks for measures of mean fundamental frequency. Given that mean fundamental frequency did not differ between sarcastic and nonsarcastic humor, it may be that a lower pitch serves to highlight more than one type of figurative language during book reading. While prior literature has found the use of a higher pitch to highlight important language, there may be differences between pitch used to emphasize literal and nonliteral language. Certainly, the sarcastic intent of the characters in books may be conveyed using prosodic cues. In the current study, the books did not contain any explicit cues to sarcasm (e.g., he said sarcastically) so the mothers’ pitch and intensity served to highlight the sarcastic remarks of the character. In addition, the target word analyzed for the sarcastic and sincere contexts was identical (i.e., “terrific”) and the duration was longer for the sarcastic contexts. Prosody observed for sarcastic remarks during book reading was similar to prosody observed for sarcastic remarks during conversation. Future research will explore the use of prosody by fathers and older siblings. In addition, children as readers and authors will be explored. References Boersma, P. and Weenink, D. (2011). Praat: doing phonetics by computer [Computer program]. Version 5.3.02, retrieved from http://www.praat.org/. Kuo, A. A., Franke, T.M., Regalado, M., and Halfon, N. (2004). Parent report of reading to young children. Pediatrics, 113, 1944-1951. Martin, L. (1999). Mothers' prosodic features: Strategies to guide young children's understanding of book language. Reading Horizons, 40, 127-146. Rockwell, P. (2000). Lower, slower, louder: Vocal cues of sarcasm. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 29, 483-495. Acknowledgements Mean dB Mean F0 8 mother-child dyads participated in the study. Children ranged in age from 6;0 to 8;9 years (M=86.87 months, SD=11.76 months). Three books were selected, each containing a target word: Nothing by Jon Agee (target word = “nothing” used as nonsarcastic humor) I’m Terrific by Marjorie Weinman Sharmat (target word = “terrific” used sincerely) Terrific by Jon Agee (target word = “terrific” used sarcastically) Mothers read all three books aloud in one of two orders: I’m Terrific, Nothing, Terrific Terrific, Nothing, I’m Terrific The dyads were then interviewed about book reading practices. Sessions were video recorded with a Canon Vixia HFS10-HD mounted on a tripod; mothers wore a lavalier microphone. Target words were identified, trimmed, and analyzed using Praat software (Boersma & Weenink, 2011) Discussion Terrificsincere Nothinghumor Terrificsarcastic The author would like to thank the following people: The families who participated in the study have my deepest thanks for their time and willingness to be video recorded during book reading. Much appreciation to the students who volunteer each week in the language and literacy laboratory at K-State. To all the authors who write such wonderful stories for parents and children to share together and to these authors in particular, thank you! Contact information: [email protected]
© Copyright 2025 Paperzz