Star%ng Strong P-‐3 Ins%tute, Spokane, 2011 This discussion should help you: Unpacking ‘High Quality’: Key Components of Everyday Contexts that Support Early Learning I. II. Communication and Language Pre- and Early-Literacy I-LABS Aug 11, 2011 Starting Strong P-3 Conference, Spokane WA Gina Lebedeva, PhD, SLP Director of Translation, Outreach & Education (TOE) lovetalkplay .org zerotothree .org developingchild .net washington.edu / earlychildhood Tomas, age 7, is on a good track to reading if he can: • Articulate the importance and continuity of how experiences in the first three years of life relate to academic learning • Learn specific research findings that are foundational to children’s language and literacy development • Generate ideas on how research-based principles can enhance your own everyday interactions with children • Identify ways in which social interaction, play, language and literacy are related in young children • RELATIONSHIPS • Early Attachment: warmth, responsiveness, consistency • Social-emotional development & Infant Mental Health • COMMUNICATION • Non-verbal: turn-taking, joint attention, gaze following, gesture, imitation • Self-awareness, self-regulation, thinking reflected through play • LANGUAGE • Vocabulary, grammar, comprehension, narrative skills, pronunciation • Abstract concepts, relating language to experience • PRE-LITERACY • Sound & print awareness; familiarity with books & stories • Home & community literacy practices & routines “Normal” is a Big Range! Large Variability Exists in Every Aspect of Development • Use a sizable vocabulary • Match familiar written and spoken words • Identify segments within words like syllables, onsets, rimes, and phonemes (individual sounds) • Pronounce most of the words he encounters in children’s books How does he get there? Biology + Experience What factors and experiences promote these skills? © UW Ins%tute for Learning & Brain Sciences ilabs.washington.edu Gina Lebedeva, PhD, SLP 1 Star%ng Strong P-‐3 Ins%tute, Spokane, 2011 Back To Basics: Relationships ‘High Quality Input & Interaction’ Language is a form of goal-directed, or intentional behavior. Language is used to have certain effects on listeners. Without healthy relationships, communication suffers. Glen Cooper, Kent Hoffman, Bert Powell, Robert Marvin Infant perception of /ra/-/la/ 100 % Correct 90 American Infants 80 70 Japanese Infants 60 50 0 6-8 months Kuhl, 2004 Sensitivity to Speech at 7.5 months Predicts Language Growth to 2 Years Infant Better Native perception Werker & Tees, 1984; Kuhl et al., 2006 What makes good brain fertilizer? Toddler à Better Non-native perception 10-12 months Why Who Bigger Vocabulary à Smaller Vocabulary Where Differences of up to 400 words Efficient “neural tuning” is associated with more efficient learning Kuhl et al., 2005, 2008 © UW Ins%tute for Learning & Brain Sciences ilabs.washington.edu Gina Lebedeva, PhD, SLP Input + Interaction How How Much When What 2 Star%ng Strong P-‐3 Ins%tute, Spokane, 2011 Who Says It 9-month-old Seattle infants Naturalistic Mandarin Chinese exposure (play & books) 12 sessions, 25 minutes each. Tested on Mandarin sound. Live vs DVD/CD: What we say • Infants track repetitions and patterns of input • 7-month-olds could picked out words more easily when they heard long sentences plus a single word, compared to just a sentence • Redundancy helps ‘See the tiger right there? Tiger!’ vs. ‘See the tiger right there!’ Kuhl, Liu, Tsao, 2003 Only Live Exposure Showed Learning Lew-Williams & Saffran, 2011 How we say it When We Say It • Timing of verbal and non-verbal responses is critical. • When caregivers respond to babble through talk and/or touch, babies babble more than if talk and touch are given at random intervals. • When caregivers label an object when the babble occurs, this facilitates wordobject associations in infants • This association is the foundation for learning word meanings • Label the current object or action itself, not based on how the babble sounds: Interpret ‘do-bee’ as ‘bottle’ not ‘doggie’ at feeding time Liu & Kuhl, 2003 Goldstein et al, 2010 How Much We Say Words Per Hour Heard ‘30 million word gap’ by Kindergarten. *Groups are reversed when it comes to amount of ‘directives’ MOM: There we go, one slipper on… JIM: I see a bird! MOM: A what, love? JIM: See a bird. MOM: (whispering) Is there? Outside? JIM: (pointing, whispering). Yes, see… MOM: Is he eating anything? JIM: No MOM: Where? Oh yes, he’s getting – do you know what he’s doing? JIM: No MOM: He’s going to the paper sack to try to pick out some pieces – Oh, he’s got food! I think he’ll pick out some pieces of thread from the sack to go and make his nest under the roof, Jim. Let’s see if… [Bird Flies Away] MOM: Put it up on the stove and leave it there. RAE: Why? MOM: ‘cause. RAE: That’s where it goes? MOM: Yeah. Discourse shapes children’s consciousness, providing a map of how to navigate reality. By 3-4 years, these two children have established different ways of learning, or interacting, of solving problems - different mental dispositions. This is how the parents’ culture is continued. Hasan (2002) Hart & Risley, 1995 © UW Ins%tute for Learning & Brain Sciences ilabs.washington.edu Gina Lebedeva, PhD, SLP 3 Star%ng Strong P-‐3 Ins%tute, Spokane, 2011 Why we say it Children develop richer vocabularies when caregivers… • • • • • • • • Talk A LOT Share more information Ask more open-ended questions Issued fewer commands Offered more choices Were more responsive Engaged children in playful conversation Used more rare or unusual words ‘Talk That Teaches’ • • • • • • • • • • • Responsive to child Imaginative, often silly Open-ended Encouraging but genuine (‘I know what you mean!’) Offers choices Asks and explores questions More complex sentences More adjectives & rhymes Richer vocabulary Engages both partners Past, future, what if ‘Teacher That Talks’ • • • • • • • • • • • Adult-initiated Serious Goal-oriented (‘testing’) Few or ‘fake’ affirmations (‘good talking!’) Directive Statements and commands Short and to the point Prose Simple, concrete vocabulary One-sided ‘conversation’ Here and now Betty Bardgie, Ed.D., A Wealth of Words Gaze points to informational ‘hot spots’ for learning Where We Look • Infants are sensitive to eye-gaze as a signal of what someone is interested in. (by 4 mos) • They follow eye-gaze to share attention on an object or event. • Toddlers use eye-gaze and pointing as signals for what is being referred to. 10-month-olds who follow gaze and look longer at target objects have significantly faster vocabulary growth at 2 years compared to infants who followed gaze less, even after taking maternal education into account. • Joint-attention leads to learning words. Brooks & Meltzoff, 2008 Gaze + Gesture + Vocalizations Gesture is a Stepping Stone to Language Social # Words Produced Long Lookers + Points Short Lookers + Points Long Lookers + No Points “Average” for all kids Short Lookers + No Points Play Cognition Gesture Language Brooks & Meltzoff, 2008 © UW Ins%tute for Learning & Brain Sciences ilabs.washington.edu Gina Lebedeva, PhD, SLP 4 Star%ng Strong P-‐3 Ins%tute, Spokane, 2011 Name the color of the ink Cognitive Control Activate desired response + Inhibit dominant response Prefrontal Cortex: Anterior Cingulate (also empathy) Self Regulation Attention Modulation Self-monitoring, control, restraint Planning for consequences Stroop, 1935 Simon Task © UW Ins%tute for Learning & Brain Sciences ilabs.washington.edu Gina Lebedeva, PhD, SLP Day-Night Task 5 Star%ng Strong P-‐3 Ins%tute, Spokane, 2011 Rachel’s center has… Why Study ‘Cognitive Control’ • Bilinguals are better (adults, kids) • Experts in ‘switching,’ verbal and non-verbal • Helps buffer risk factors (monolingual) • Higher performance = strong resiliency later on • Can early measures of cognitive control predict school readiness? e.g., Meltzoff & Carlson, 2008; Bialystock et al. e.g., Lengua & Trancik, 2008 Changing Classroom Conversations: Narrowing the Gap Between Potential and Reality ‘There is a heightened awareness of the role of language as a driving force of intellectual development. This runs counter to more traditional Piagetian views of the child as a solitary ‘scientist’ who builds concepts of the world through observation and individual experimentation. Many ECE were trained through Piagetian lenses, and many curricula are based on these views.’ Lots of: But still working on: • Books • Group activities • Interest centers with varied themes • Labels • Pretend play materials • Music • Letter-naming activities • Quizzing • Teaching simple concepts • Dialogues during reading • Small group conversations • Integration of experiences + reading + writing • Personally meaningful print • Adults doing pretend play • Word play beyond rhymes • Story-telling • Genuine questioning • Extended exploration and information sharing Four Strategies to Build Interactive Communication 1. Be Responsive • Accept child’s actions or sounds as meaningful • Learn what is meaningful for that child • Respond to what the child is doing at that moment • Respond more to the behaviors you want more of • Translate sounds and actions into words 2. Be Matched • Act & talk in ways that child can • Show the child the next developmental step to try • Expect behaviors that are possible for the child • Join the child’s world first • Use new words about things the child talks about Handbook of Child Development & Early Education: Research to Practice (2009) 3. Be Balanced and Share Control • Do as much as the child during play • Let the child lead half the time • Extend turn-taking exchanges for one more turn • Make the interaction more playful than task-oriented • Make commands and questions each <25% of your talk • Do or say one thing and wait for the child’s response 4. Be Emotionally Playful • Be more interesting than the distractions • Ensure the child’s success • Find what make the child belly-laugh • Laugh and smile authentically yourself • Do the unexpected • Avoid making talking a test for the child © UW Ins%tute for Learning & Brain Sciences ilabs.washington.edu Gina Lebedeva, PhD, SLP J. MacDonald, Communicating Partners Early Learning as an Investment • ‘Intervention’ model • Costly Basic Learning Mechanisms Informed Practices • Earlier identification of risk factors • ‘Prevention’ model • Efficient Level Playing Field 6 Star%ng Strong P-‐3 Ins%tute, Spokane, 2011 ‘Interaction Rules’ to Foster Language • • • • • • • • • National Early Literacy Panel 2009 Report (Available Online) OWL: Observe. Wait. Listen. Match: Do what she does Balance: Take only ONE turn at a time Respond: Follow child’s lead Expand: child’s stage +1; Model next step Comment: 3 for every 1 ‘directive’ or ‘wh’ Q Get down: Eye-to-eye Reflect: Wonder what your effect is Have fun: For real Michael Tomasello, Dorothy Bishop, James Macdonald, Stanly Greenspan, Marc Bornstein… To have strong learners, we need to provide environments where P-3ers are most likely to: Have healthy relationships: Follow conversation ‘rules:’ Use safe & secure base to explore and learn from the world Stay on topic Introduce and provide adequate information for listeners Strong attachment to important people in their life Self-regulate social-emotional responses Take appropriate conversation or play turns To have strong learners, we need to provide an environment where P-3ers are most likely to: Use a large vocabulary: In interactions directed at them Hear a wide diversity of words Learn words they can imitate Engage Be Engaged Can use play to represent experiences: Connect different formats: Gestured, pictured, written and experienced concepts Effectively use many functions: Follow social cues: Multi-step sequences in play Sensitive to eye gaze, gesture and body language of others Pretend & symbolic play Understand intentions of others Lead and be followed: Explore with positive consequence Use many forms of play To request, refuse, comment, inquire, explain, get needs met, get attention, express emotion CONTINUUM OF LANGUAGE AND LITERACY SKILL DEVELOPMENT Oral Language (receptive and expressive) Vocabulary Concepts of Print Comprehension (read aloud) Alphabet Knowledge Phonological Awareness Phonemic Awareness Phonics Comprehension (independent reading) Fluency Writing Content Knowledge / Life Experience Scarborough, H.S., in Neuman & Dickinson (2001) Handbook of Early Literacy Research © UW Ins%tute for Learning & Brain Sciences ilabs.washington.edu Gina Lebedeva, PhD, SLP 3 4 5 6 Age (years) 7 8 Wasik & Newman, 2009 7 Star%ng Strong P-‐3 Ins%tute, Spokane, 2011 Predicting the Predictors Infant Toddler Phonetic Perception • Knowledge of and skills in reading and writing obtained prior to conventional literacy • Provides a foundation for higher-level literacy Preschooler Early Vocabulary & Grammar Receptive & Expressive Language Phonological Awareness Emergent Literacy is Part of a Continuum Early Literacy Emergent Literacy Emergent/Early Reading Skill Conventional Literacy • Learning to read: Decoding • Learning about print and sound • Reading to learn: Comprehension Lebedeva, 2010 4 years à 4th Grade* Stronger vocabulary, decoding and reading comprehension in 4th grade was related to these characteristics at age 4 years: – Conversations with teachers included more sophisticated words Around 4-5 years for typical children reared in print-rich homes From Birth à K transition Timing established by biology + experience (‘nature + nurture’) – Teachers’ use of sophisticated words resulted in children’s use of more sophisticated words Emergent Literacy Period – During free play conversations, teachers extended topics by asking children to clarify or explain their thoughts – During group times, teachers kept children involved by non-punitive efforts to focus (e.g., by calling their names) – During book reading, teachers engaged children in discussion to analyze the story, relate it to their own experience, and to discuss the meaning of words Current Emphasis: Many Indicators, K-Readiness Preventing academic & reading difficulties well-established evidence base *Even after taking into account language skill at age 3, family education and income, and amount of literacy support in preschool (Dickinson & Porche, 2011, Child Development) What are some indicators of emergent literacy development? • • • • • • • • • • • • • • Listening to stories Knowing which part of the page has printed words Knowing which way to hold the book and turn pages Producing fictional narratives, Acting out stories Recognizing which things are shapes, which are letters Recognizing and writing one’s name Pretending to write Pretend reading from favorite books Identifying major elements of a book (characters, author, title) Naming words in the pictures Reciting all the letters Knowing some letter-sound correspondences Detecting rhymes, knowing nursery rhymes Identifying alliteration (when words start with the same sound) © UW Ins%tute for Learning & Brain Sciences ilabs.washington.edu Gina Lebedeva, PhD, SLP Generally grouped into 5 key areas that strongly predict later conventional literacy skills Oral language • Listening to stories, making narratives • Vocabulary, grammar, social use Emergent Writing • Writing one’s own name • Pretend writing (‘meaningful scribble’) Print Knowledge • Pretend reading from favorite books • Identifying which are letters vs shapes Alphabet Knowledge Phonological Awareness • Knowing names of letters • Knowing some sound-letter correspondence • Detecting rhymes • Detecting sub-parts of words 8 Star%ng Strong P-‐3 Ins%tute, Spokane, 2011 Phonological Awareness • Ability to abstractly manipulate the sounds within speech, independent of meaning: Sound Play • Larger Segments: – Words as units of phrases and sentences – Syllables as units of multi-syllable words • Smaller Segments: – Sounds within a syllable – Phonemes: initial, middle & final sounds – Also called phonemic awareness More words = more sounds = easier to be aware of sounds happy neat frog might clean opposites loose see market sight rat seen Phonological Awareness • Word Awareness: Clap for each word with me • Syllable Awareness: Clap with me for each part of the word /wa ter/ • Rhyming: What word rhymes with Cat? • Blending Syllables and sounds: What word is d – o – g? • Syllable splitting: ‘cup’ starts with /k/ and ends with /up/ • Phoneme segmentation: What are the sounds in ‘sit’ • Oddity: What word doesn’t belong with the others – cat, mat, bat, run • Phoneme deletion: What is ‘cat’ without the /k/ sound? • Phoneme manipulation: What would ‘cat’ be if you changed the /t/ to /n/? 5 yr Phonological Awareness Tasks Level Measure Sample Task Basic Rhyming Do ‘man’ and ‘pan’ rhyme? What rhymes with ‘tickle?’ Moderate Sentence Segmentation Clap one time for each word in ‘I like pizza’ Syllable Segmentation Clap one time for each syllable in ‘watermelon’ Elision Say ‘spider’ without ‘der’ ‘milk’ without /k/ Blending ‘ham-mer’ makes? ‘p-e-n-c-i-l’ makes? Matching Which one starts/ends with the same sound as ‘pan?’ Non-word Repetition Repeat ‘ver-bug-lee-mush’ fat make container computer bank throw bark Jan lark Jump, jumped Advanced Phon. Working Mem. Home Literacy Environment Book Sharing Helps Level the Playing Field Strong Link to Reading Readiness More than 3 “less favorable” home literacy factors Lower pre-literacy score (-1SD) Less than daily book sharing with child Child’s infrequent interest in books Rare trips to libraries Regardless of phonetic sensitivity or vocabulary Few appropriate books at home Rare observing parents reading for pleasure Lebedeva, Bhagat, Raizada & Kuhl, 2009; Hoff-Ginsburg; Huttenlocher © UW Ins%tute for Learning & Brain Sciences ilabs.washington.edu Gina Lebedeva, PhD, SLP 9 Star%ng Strong P-‐3 Ins%tute, Spokane, 2011 How many verbs in your sentences? Sentence complexity of parent input is related child’s brain development in language regions (5-year-olds) Reading to children exposes them to particularly rich language. “It could be that children’s processing efficiency is facilitated by more dense or more complex talk because those experiences provide extensive practice with interpreting language in challenging contexts. Or perhaps children benefit from richer talk because it provides more robust and reliable links between language and meaningful aspects of the world. These experiences could lead to accelerated growth in vocabulary and grammar as well as to further advances in processing efficiency and working memory capacity.” Lebedeva, Bhagat, Raizada & Kuhl (2010) (Marchman & Fernald, 2008, pg. F15) Broca’s Area: Speech, Syntax, Reading Zero to Three Dot Org www.zerotothree.org/BrainWonders www.zerotothree.org/BrainWonders Model Reading and Writing • • • • as something adults do for work and pleasure in many ways, for many purposes for children to imitate in pretend play www.zerotothree.org/BrainWonders Building Literacy With Love – B. B © UW Ins%tute for Learning & Brain Sciences ilabs.washington.edu Gina Lebedeva, PhD, SLP 10 Star%ng Strong P-‐3 Ins%tute, Spokane, 2011 Engage Children Use Silly Songs and Movement Games Follow the child’s lead. Comment – and wait up to 5 seconds. Ask a question – and wait up to 5 seconds. Respond by adding information – and wait up to 5 seconds. • • • • to accompany and enhance routines to encourage wordplay and creativity to teach concepts such as opposites to extend vocabulary Bump, jump, jiggly jump. Bump -ety, Bump, jump, Bump -ety, Tune into Sounds • • • • listen to sounds in the environment highlight key words and word parts encourage wordplay play listening and memory games Pitter, patter Pitter, patter Pitter, patter Pit The rain that pounds My window pane Puts polka dots on It. What We Know § Hamsters have tails and fur. § Hamsters eat seeds, bugs, and hamster food. § Hamsters can run fast. § Hamsters are mammals. § Hamsters hide food in their cages and in their cheek pouches. Pitter, patter Pitter, patter Pitter, patter Pat The rain taps My umbrella drum Just like That What We Want to Know § Where do wild hamsters live? § What do wild hamsters eat? § Can pet hamsters get sick? § What do baby hamsters look like? § Why do hamsters bury food? jump-ety, bump, bump. jump-ety, jump Share Informative Talk • • • • • talk about the past and the future create fantasy worlds together use specific, descriptive language introduce unusual words ask and encourage open-ended questions • explore children’s interests in depth Get Involved in Pretend Play • set up the environment • take on a role • expand ideas • help children participate Wait, watch, and wonder – then find a way to enter. © UW Ins%tute for Learning & Brain Sciences ilabs.washington.edu Gina Lebedeva, PhD, SLP 11 Star%ng Strong P-‐3 Ins%tute, Spokane, 2011 Encourage Children’s Conversations • in home language and English • pair children for support and modeling • set the stage • provide interesting things to talk about Create a Print-Rich Environment • convey messages • with signs, logos, words, and icons • on walls, toys, storage bins • let children play with many forms of print Use Words to Solve Problems • help children “use their words” • talk through steps • encourage collaboration and negotiation • scaffold children’s reasoning • discuss steps and rules within an activity (see ‘cognitive flexibility’) Partner with Parents • Support home language • Bring family stories and funds of knowledge into the classroom • Build a community that celebrates reading Unit Planning Exercise Share Family Stories • “what’s happening” at home and school • photo displays and scrapbooks • special songs and rhymes • artifacts that reflect cultural heritage • visits, field trips, and special events • pretend play environments and imaginary friends © UW Ins%tute for Learning & Brain Sciences ilabs.washington.edu Gina Lebedeva, PhD, SLP q How will you share the book with children? q What do you think will intrigue the children? What opportunities might you seize for making connections, asking cognitively challenging questions, and extending vocabulary? q How will the classroom change, especially the pretend play and block areas? q What projects will children engage in? How will they incorporate reading, writing, drawing, maps, or graphs? q How will you involve parents? 12 Star%ng Strong P-‐3 Ins%tute, Spokane, 2011 Every Child Ready To Read: www.everychildreadytoread.org/ Literacy Information & Communication System (LINCS) http://lincs.ed.gov/publications/publications.html Preschool Language & Literacy Research: Dr. Laura Justice: http://preschoollab.osu.edu/people/ First Words Project: http://firstwords.fsu.edu/ Build a Lending Library for Your Families! Resources for Inspiration & Vast Information National Early Childhood Transition Center http://www.hdi.uky.edu/SF/NECTC/Home.aspx Edutopia: http://www.edutopia.org/ Children of the Code: http://www.childrenofthecode.org/library/refs/index.htm I-LABS and Thrive will have featured roles, representing Early Learning and WA. Tune In! Gratefully acknowledging our partners, collaborators, colleagues, and supporters • NSF Science of Learning Center grant to the University of Washington’s LIFE Center • The National Institutes of Health (NIH) • The Hsin-Yi Foundation • The McDonnell Foundation • The Human Frontiers Science Program • Cure Autism Now © UW Ins%tute for Learning & Brain Sciences ilabs.washington.edu Gina Lebedeva, PhD, SLP Tax-Deductable Contributions: ilabs.washington.edu 13
© Copyright 2025 Paperzz