!"#$%#&'()%*+$#$%,+'!-%&&'#+.'/#$0*1#$%2#&'3),4&*1'!,&5%+6 78$0,)9:;<'=%+.:#>'7++*'?#)$)* !,8)2*<'@,8)+#&'A,)'B*:*#)20'%+'/#$0*1#$%2:'C.82#$%,+D'E,&F'GHD'I,F'J'9/#>D'HKKL;D'""F'GHMN GGK 384&%:0*.'4><'I#$%,+#&'O,8+2%&',A'?*#20*):',A'/#$0*1#$%2: !$#4&*'PB=<'http://www.jstor.org/stable/749375 722*::*.<'HGQLRQGLHL'HJ<SL Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of JSTOR's Terms and Conditions of Use, available at http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp. JSTOR's Terms and Conditions of Use provides, in part, that unless you have obtained prior permission, you may not download an entire issue of a journal or multiple copies of articles, and you may use content in the JSTOR archive only for your personal, non-commercial use. Please contact the publisher regarding any further use of this work. Publisher contact information may be obtained at http://www.jstor.org/action/showPublisher?publisherCode=nctm. Each copy of any part of a JSTOR transmission must contain the same copyright notice that appears on the screen or printed page of such transmission. JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact [email protected]. National Council of Teachers of Mathematics is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to Journal for Research in Mathematics Education. http://www.jstor.org Journalfor Researchin MathematicsEducation 1990, Vol. 21, No. 3, 216-229 SPATIALORIENTATIONSKILLAND MATHEMATICAL PROBLEMSOLVING LINDSAYANNE TARTRE,CaliforniaState University,Long Beach The purposeof this study was to explore the role of spatialorientationskill in the solution of mathematicsproblems.Fifty-seven tenth-gradestudentswho scored high or low on a spatial orientationtest were askedto solve mathematicsproblemsin individualinterviews.A groupof specific behaviorswas identifiedin geometricsettings,which appearedto be manifestationsof spatialorientationskill. Spatialorientationskill also appearedto be involved in understanding the problemand linking new problemsto previous work in nongeometricsettings. For a long time researchershave attemptedto determine why some students learnmathematicsor areable to solve mathematicsproblemsbetterthanothersdo. Some researchhas focused on the processes used to solve mathematicsproblems. Otherresearchershave tried to identify factors and skills relatedto doing mathematics.Among the skills found to be relatedto mathematicslearningandachievement are spatialskills (Fennema& Sherman,1977; McGee, 1979). The literaturecontainsa greatdeal of discussion aboutthe possible relationship between spatialskills and mathematics.Many studies have found spatialskills to be positively correlatedwith measures of mathematicsperformance(Connor& Serbin,1985;Fennema& Sherman,1977). But whatarespatialskills, andhow and why are those skills relatedto mathematics? SPATIAL SKILLS In general,spatialskills are consideredto be those mentalskills concernedwith understanding,manipulating,reorganizing,or interpretingrelationshipsvisually. In his review of spatialfactors, McGee (1979) distinguishedtwo majortypes of spatial skills: A plethoraof factoranalyticstudiessince the 1930's have providedstrongandconsistentsupport for the existence of at least two distinct spatialabilities-visualization and orientation.(p. 909) A factor analytic study by Connorand Serbin (1980) supportedthe distinction summarizedby McGee. They found that the visual-spatialtests dividedinto two factors....The CubeComparisonTest, PaperFoldingTest, CardRotationsTest, andDAT Space RelationsTest hadhigh loadingon...[onefactor].This factor representswhat ETS has referredto as the "spatialvisualization"subdivision of visual-spatial ability.... [The second spatialfactor]had high loadingson HiddenPatterns,GestaltCompletion, and CardRotations.This factorrepresentswhatETS has referredto as the "closure"subdivision of visual-spatialability. (p. 27) A differentcategorizationwas proposedby LinnandPetersen(1985), who identified threespatialability categories: spatialperception,mentalrotation,and spatial visualization.They defined spatialvisualizationas "those spatialability tasks This articleis based on the author'sdissertationat the Universityof Wisconsin-Madison underthe directionof ElizabethFennema. LindsayAnne Tartre 217 which involve complicatedmulti-stepmanipulationsof spatiallypresentedinformation" (p. 1484). They distinguishedit from the other two categories "by the possibility of multiple solution strategies"(p. 1484). Linn and Petersenfelt that the only commoncharacteristicof spatialvisualizationtaskswas thatthe solutions requiredmore than one step. They included in this class of tasks not only Paper Folding, Form Boards (Ekstrom,French,& Harmon,1976), and the DAT (Space Relations)(Bennett,Seashore,& Wesman,1973) but also the HiddenFiguresTest (Ekstrom,French,& Harmon,1976). McGee (1979) felt that spatial visualization tasks "all involve the ability to mentallymanipulate,rotate,twist, or inverta pictoriallypresentedstimulusobject" (p. 893). Kershand Cook (1979) also suggested thattests of spatialvisualization involve either the rotationor transformationof a mentalobject. This study is based on the categorizationexpressedby McGee (1979), Connor and Serbin (1980), and Kersh and Cook (1979). Spatial visualization is distinguished from spatialorientationtasks by identifying what is to be moved; if the task suggests thatall or partof a representationbe mentallymoved or altered,it is considereda spatialvisualizationtask. By this definition,Form Boards and Card Rotations(Ekstrom,French,& Harmon,1976) and the Space Relationsportionof the DAT (Bennett,Seashore,& Wesman, 1973) are examples of tests of this class of spatial skills. Spatial orientationtasks do not requirementally moving an object. Only the perceptual perspective of the person viewing the object is changed or moved. McGee (1979) statedthat spatialorientationtasks "involve the comprehensionof the arrangementof elements within a visual stimulus patternand the aptitudeto remain unconfusedby the changing orientationin which a spatial configuration may be presented"(p. 909). This means thatspatialorientationitems suggest that the person understanda representationor a change between two representations. There is not uniformagreementamong researchersabouteither the term or the classification of spatial orientationtasks. For example, Linn and Petersen(1985) classified the Rod and FrameTest (Witkin,Dyk, & Faterson, 1962) as a spatial perceptiontest, the CardRotationsTest (Ekstromet al., 1976) as a spatialrotation test, and the Hidden Figures Test (Ekstromet al., 1976) as a spatialvisualization test. McGee (1979) discussed both the Rod and FrameTest and the Hidden Figures Test as spatialorientationtests. ETS originallyclassified the Hidden Figures Test and the GestaltCompletionTest as "closure"tests andthe CardRotationTest as an orientationtest (Ekstromet al., 1976). Connorand Serbin(1980) found the Hidden Figures Test, the Gestalt CompletionTest, and the CardRotationTest to be one factor,which they labeled "closure." In agreementwith McGee (1979) and Connorand Serbin(1980) the label spatial orientationused in this paperto describethose tasks thatrequirethatthe subject mentallyreadjusther or his perspectiveto become consistentwith a representation of an object presentedvisually. Spatial orientationtasks could involve organizing, recognizing, making sense out of a visual representation,reseeing it or seeing it from a differentangle, but not mentallymoving the object. Spatial Orientation 218 By this definition, the Gestalt Completion Test, the Hidden Figures Test (Ekstromet al., 1976), the Rod and FrameTest (Witkin,Dyk, & Faterson,1962), optical illusion, and other hidden object puzzles are examples of tests of spatial orientationskill. (See Tartre,1990, for a more complete treatmentof the classification of spatialskills.) SKILLSANDMATHEMATICS SPATIAL Spatial skills may be intellectually interestingin themselves, but my purpose here is to attemptto understandpartof the natureof the relationship,if any, between spatialskills andmathematics.Do we use individualspatialskills in specific and identifiableways to do mathematics? Do spatialskills serve as general indicators of a way of organizing information that may help solve many types of mathematicsproblems? Althoughmanyhave speculatedabouthow spatialskills andmathematicsmight be relatedand much researchlately has investigatedmathematicsproblem-solving processes, few researchershave attemptedto identify specific processes used to solve mathematicsproblemsthatmightbe relatedto spatialskills. In one longitudinalstudyby FennemaandTartre(1985), middle school studentswith discrepant spatial visualization and verbal skills were asked to draw pictures to solve mathematicsproblems.When asked to tell about the problem before solving it, students with low spatial visualization and high verbal skills tended to provide more detailed verbal descriptions of the relevant informationin the problems. However, studentswith high spatialvisualizationskill and low verbal skill translated the probleminto a picturebetterand also had more detailedinformationon the picturefor problemssolved correctly. The issue of whetherspatialskills are generalindicatorsof a particularway of mentallyorganizinginformationthatmightbe helpfulin many areasof mathematics has been discussedby a numberof authors.One hypothesisis thatmathematical reasoning and problem solving are "facilitatedby a 'mental blackboard'on which the activitymay be organizedandthe interrelatednessof components'visualized'"(Anglin, Meyer,& Wheeler,1975,p. 9). Bishop (1980) also theorizedthat spatialtrainingmight help studentsto organizethe situationwith mentalpictures during problem solving in mathematics. He proposed that the structureof the problemmight be understoodthrougha spatial format.The frequentuse of tree diagrams,Venn diagrams,charts,and other figures to organize informationand show relationshipsamongcomponentsof a problemdemonstratesthe plausibility of this hypothesis. Smith (1964) suggested an even more centralrole for spatial skills in problemsolving. He statedthat the conceptionof spatialabilitywhich emergesfromrecentresearch...isso all embracingthatone is led to inquirewhetherthe processof perceivingand assimilating...generalpatternsor configurations(whetherspatialor non-spatial)is not in fact a process of "abstraction"....The process of perceiving and assimilating a gestalt...[is] a process of abstraction(abstractingform or structure).... It is possible thatany process of abstractionmay involve in some degree the perception, retentionin memory,recognitionandperhapsreproductionof a patternor structure.(p. 213-214) Brainhemisphericresearchersbelieve thattests of spatialskill are indicatorsof LindsayAnne Tartre 219 a mode of intellectualfunctioningthat may be tied to global processing of problem situationsor patterns(Davidson, 1979;Wheatley,1977; Galyean, 1981). This body of researchhas establishedthat there are at least two types of logical thinking processes: one type thatis characterizedby step-by-step,deductive,and often verbal processes and one type that suggests more structural,global, relational, intuitive,spatial,and inductiveprocesses.Francoand Sperry(1977) identifiedthe possible roles of these two thoughtprocesses in problem solving by stating that "nonverbalvisuo-spatial apprehensionseems commonly to precede and support the sequentialdeductiveanalysis involved in the solution of geometryproblems" (p. 112). Lorenz (1968) capturedthese ideas best when he said, I hold that Gestalt perception of this type is identical with that mysterious function which is generallycalled "Intuition",and which indubitablyis one of the most importantcognitive faculties of Man [sic]. When the scientist, confrontedwith a multitudeof irregularand apparentlyirreconcilablefacts, suddenly"sees"the generalregularityruling them all, when the explanation of the hithertoinexplicableall at once "jumpsout"at him [or her] with the suddennessof a revelation, the experience of this happeningis fundamentallysimilar to that other when the hidden Gestalt in a puzzle-picturesurprisinglystartsout from the confusing backgroundof irrelevant detail. The Germanexpression:"in die Augen springen"[to springto the eyes], is very descriptive of this process. (p. 176) These descriptionsof the possible relationshipbetween spatialskills and intuitive structuralunderstandinginvolved in problemsolutionsoundmore like the use of spatial orientationskills than spatial visualizationskills. Spatial visualization tasks generally requiremultiple operations, often analytical, and could involve verbal mediation. Spatialorientationtasks are often difficult to analyze. Verbalization of the process or processes is also difficult,if not impossible. Spatialorientationtasks often requirethe subjectto organizeor make sense out of visual information. For example, in the Gestalt Completion Test, the solution is often describedas appearingall at once as a whole figure (see Figure 1 for sample items). It requiresthat the subjects orient themselves to see the ink blots as a whole object. Mentalorganizationof the whole structureof the spatialconfigurationis the importantfeature.Insight is the word often used to describethe solution process. Whetherthis type of mentalorganizationor apprehensionis relatedto the organizing processes involved in problem solving and spatialorientationskill was the problemaddressedin this study.The purposeof the studywas to explore the role of spatialorientationskill in the solutionof mathematicsproblemsand to identify possible associated gender differences. The part of the study that investigated genderdifferenceshas been reportedelsewhere (Tartre,1990). This paperfocuses on thatpartof the study thatattemptedto identify ways in which studentsscoring high or low in spatial orientationskill behaved differentlyas they solved mathematics problems. METHOD Sample A representativesample of 97 10th-gradestudentsfrom threehigh schools in a midwesterncity was given a spatialorientationtest and a 10th-grademathematics 220 Spatial Orientation This is a test of yourabilityto see a whole pictureeven thoughit is not completely drawn.You are to use your imaginationto fill in the missing parts. Look at each incompletepictureand try to see what it is. On the line under each picture,write a word or two to describeit. Try the sample picturesbelow: 1. 1. 2. 2. (Picture 1 is a flag and picture2 is a hammerhead.) Fromthe Manualfor Kit of Factor-ReferencedCognitiveTests (p. 27) by R. B. Ekstrom,J. W. French,& H. H. Harmon,1976. Princeton,NJ:EducationalTesting Service. Copyright@ 1962, 1975 by EducationalTesting Service. Reprintedby permission. Figure 1. GestaltCompletionTest. achievementtest (HoughtonMifflin, 1971). Those studentswho scored in the top or bottomthirdof the distributionon the GestaltCompletionTest, which was used to measurespatialorientation,were selected to be interviewed.This interviewsampleconsisted of 57 students(27 high spatialorientationand 30 low spatialorientation). SpatialOrientationMeasure The Gestalt CompletionTest (Ekstromet al., 1976) includes 20 items. Part of the representationof the object is shown, and the subject is to see or mentally organizethe informationin orderto understandwhatthe completedrepresentation would be (see Figure 1). This test was chosen to measurespatialorientationskill because I concludedthatit was the best test to capturethe essence of pure spatial thoughtas describedabove. That is, the tasks would be solved holistically,it appeared unlikely that verbal or analytic processes would contributeto subjects' solutions, and the items directlyrequiredthe structuralorganizationof visual informationin orderto make sense out of the partialpictures.This test has not been used as much as othertests recently in mathematicseducationresearch,possibly because spatialvisualizationhas been studiedmore andbecauseit is so difficultto describethe process or processes used to solve it. However,in this study it is pre- 221 LindsayAnne Tartre cisely that intuitive or insightful spatial organizational process that is under investigation. Problem-SolvingInterview The problem-solvinginterviewconsisted of 10 mathematicsproblems.Each of the problemscould be solved in more thanone way, andthe informationpresented in the problemneededto be organizedin some mannerto solve the problem.Seven problemsconcernedgeometriccontent, and threeproblemsconcernednongeometric content. Five geometric problemswere presentedvisually (using a concrete or pictorialrepresentationcontainingcomplete informationfor the solution) and one nonvisually (with written words). The seventh geometric problem was presented using a geoboard-like,dot grid framework(see Figure 2). One of the nongeometricproblemswas presentedvisually and two were presentednonvisually. Draw a squarethathas areaequal to 2 squareinches using four of the points below as vertices (corners). I--1inch--I Explain how you know thatthe areaof your figure is 2 squareinches. (Hint given verbally if appropriate:You have used horizontaland vertical lines. Is there any other alternative?) Figure 2. Problem6. 222 Spatial Orientation For one geometricproblempresentedvisually studentswere given a black and white photographencasedin glass of approximatelyfifty blocks stackedunevenly. They were asked to estimate and then determinehow many blocks there were. They were also asked to determinehow many blocks were completely hidden in the picture.Problem3 (Figure3) is anotherexample of a visually presentedgeometricproblem.The geometricnonvisuallypresentedproblemdescribedthe relationshipsand distancesamong threetowns on a road and askedaboutthe distance from one of the three towns to a fourthtown. For the nongeometricvisually presentedproblemstudentswere given six cubes with 2, 3, 5, 6, 7, and 9 on the upper faces. They were askedto use the six numbersto make two 3-digit numbersso that when one is subtractedfrom the otherthe smallestpositive differenceresults.One nongeometricnonvisually presentedproblemasked the studentto determinethe numberof groupingsof fruitthatcould be made using one or more of five different types of fruit. Withoutcalculating,what do you thinkis the area of the shadedfigure? I I<-1 inch-- shaded ofthe area What isthe figure What is the areaof the shadedfigure? Figure 3. Problem3. LindsayAnneTartre 223 InterviewProcedures The studentsin the sample were asked to solve the problemsand explain their solutions in individualinterviews.The interviewerwas not awareof the student's spatialorientationskill level duringthe interviewor duringcoding. The problems were randomlyorderedfor each student. Studentswere asked not to discuss the problemswith other students. Duringthe interviewthe studentswere encouragedto talk aboutwhat they were doing as they attemptedto solve each problem.Questionswere asked duringand afterproblem solution, when appropriate,to help understandthe student'sthinking processes more fully. In addition, on completion of each problem, students were asked to look back at what they had done and if possible, to describe their motivationfor the organizationused to solve the problem.Generally,a balancewas sought between the need to make the solution process as naturalas possible and the interviewer'sdesire to obtainthe maximumamountof informationaboutthe processes used by each student.Each interview was about 1 hour in length. The interviews were audiotaped,and record-keepingsheets were used duringthe interview for a written account of the student's overt problem-solving activities. Interviewswere coded using a coding system developed for this study. Coding Categories Some coding categories were chosen because it was hypothesized that they manifestedthe use of spatialorientationskill in specific ways. A shortrationaleis includedwith the definitionfor the categorieshypothesizedto be relatedto spatial orientationskill. In addition, some of the categories were identified from overt behaviorsobservedduringthe interviews. Categoriesapplicableto all problems.The following categorieswere applicable to all problems: Correctanswer indicatedwhetheror not the studentfoundthe correctanswerto the problemwithouta hint.A hint was only possible for Problem6 (see Figure2). Done like described whether or not the student indicated having previously encountereda similarproblem.This statementmight have occurredin responseto a question asked by the interviewer, or the student could have volunteered the informationin a comment aboutrememberingor forgettinghow to do it, or having just done one like it in class. Failure to breakset indicatedwhetheror not the studentdemonstratedan inability to breakthe problemor pictureapartor did not change a mind set thatwould provide an incompleteor inaccuratesolution.For example, for Problem6 (Figure 2), the student might have drawn shapes involving only horizontal and vertical lines and then indicatedthat the problemcould not be solved. That studentdemonstrateda failure to break set. It was hypothesizedthat because spatial orienta- 224 SpatialOrientation tion skill requiresreseeing or looking at somethingin a new way, inflexibility in breakinga mental set could indicatea lack of spatialorientationskill use. Mental movementindicatedany evidence that the studentmentally moved objects in the problem.For example, if a picturewas presentthe studentmight have said "I picked this partup and moved it over here,"indicatingmental movement of partof the picture. Misunderstoodproblem indicated whether or not the student demonstrateda misunderstandingor confusion aboutthe problem.It could have been a misunderstandingthatthe studentcarriedto the end of the solution or a misperceptionthat was rectifiedbefore the end of the solution process. Categories applicable to specific problems.The following categories only applied to certainproblems: Added marksindicatedthe adding of marksor lines to a pictureincluded as a partof a problem. Drewpictureindicatedthe drawingof a picture,diagram,chart,or any otheraid that could provide visual or spatialrelationshipsor organizationfor the solution process. Drew relation indicated a markingor drawing with which the student was attemptingto show a mathematicalrelationshipvisuallyratherthanjust to keep track of a countingprocess. Estimate error was used for four of the problems, in which the studentswere askedto estimatethe answerbefore computingit. It was thoughtthatspatialorientationskill might be used in getting some sense of the size of the answer.The estimate errorindicatedthe proportionof differencebetween the correctanswerand the estimate given by the student.This category was groupedinto two clusters: analytic,in which given elementsneededto be analyzedand recombined,andperceptual, in which visual apprehensionof the approximatemagnitudeof a shown figure was required.For applicable problems the estimate errorwas calculated using the following ratio: 1- studentresponse correctanswer After all the interviews were completed, they were coded on the basis of both the audio and writtenrecords.With one exception (estimateerror),all categories were coded dichotomously(behaviorpresentor absent)for each applicableprob- Lindsay Anne Tartre 225 lem for each student.After the coding was completed, a randomsample of four interviewswas recodedandintracoderagreementof at least 80%was obtainedfor each category. RESULTS A spatialorientationskill groupby sex analysis of covariance(ANCOVA)was performed,using mathematicsachievement as the covariate, for each behavior category by problemtype. Table 1 shows the means and standarddeviations for the categoriesfor geometricand nongeometricproblemtypes. No significantdifference between spatialorientationskill groupswas found for the numberof correctanswersfor eitherthe geometricor nongeometricproblems.A significantmain effect for spatial orientationskill level was obtainedfor three categories: failure to breakset, misunderstoodproblem,and done like. The mean for the low spatial orientationgroupwas greaterthanthe mean for the high spatialorientationgroup for failure to breakset for geometric problems,F (1, 52) = 6.48, p < .05, and for misunderstoodproblemfor nongeometricproblems,F (1, 52) = 4.78, p < .05. A significant difference favoring the high spatial orientationgroup was found for done like for nongeometricproblems,F (1, 52) = 12.82, p < .01. Table 1 Means (StandardDeviations) by Problem Typesfor CategoriesApplicable to All Problems Geometric problems Spatialorientationlevel Nongeometricproblems Spatialorientationlevel Category Low High Low High Correctanswer 1.97 (1.79) 0.37 (0.72) 3.37 (1.47) 0.57 (0.86) 0.87 (0.94) 2.96 (1.91) 0.37 (0.74) 2.07* (1.57) 0.93 (0.73) 0 74 (0.81) 1.00 (0.83) 0.27 (0.52) 1.17 (0.91) 0.03 (0.18) 1.60 (0.72) 1.33 (0.68) 0.96 (0.76: 1.00 (0.68: 0.07 (0.27: 1.04 (0.85: Done like Failureto breakset Mentalmovement Misunderstoodproblem Note.n = 30 forlowspatialorientation group.Meansarebasedon 7 group;n = 27 forhighspatialorientation and3 nongeometric geometric problems. *p < .05. **p < .01. Table 2 shows the means and standarddeviations for the categories that were only applicableto specific problems.A significantmain effect for spatialorientation skill level was obtainedfor two categories: estimate error(perceptual)and drew relation.The mean for the low spatialorientationgroupwas greaterthanthe mean for the high spatial orientationgroup for estimate error(perceptual),F (1, 52) = 4.87, p < .05. A significant difference favoring the high spatial orientation group was found for drew relation,F (1, 52) = 9.23, p < .01. 226 Spatial Orientation Table 2 Means (StandardDeviation) by Categories Applicableto Specific Problems Category Total numiber of problems Spatialorientationlevel Low High 2.63 (1.38) 1.20 2.48 (1.48) 1.56 Added marks 5 Drew picture 2 (0.71) (0.58) Drew relation 2 1.23 (0.77) 1.81** (0.40) Estimateerror Analytic 2 Perceptual 2 1.15 (0.49) 1.56 (2.43) 1.32 (0.97) 0.52* (0.25) Note:n = 30 forlow spatialorientation group. group;n = 27 forhighspatialorientation *p< .05.**p < .01. OFRESULTS DISCUSSION Spatialorientationskill appearedto be indicatedin several ways wheregeometric contentwas involved. The low spatialorientationgrouphad highermeans for failureto breakset for geometricproblems.This meant thatthe low spatialorientation group demonstratedless flexibility in changing a formed perceptualmind set for those geometric problems.This was particularlyapparentfor Problem 6 (Figure 2). Approximately10%of the low spatialorientationstudentswere able to get the correctanswerto this problembefore the hint was given, whereas41% of the high spatial orientationstudents found the correct answer on their own. However,47% of the low spatialorientationand 56% of the high spatialorientation studentswere able to find the solution after the hint was available.The low spatial orientationstudentsappearedto need and use the hint more than those in the high spatialorientationgroupto help thembreakthe mind set of horizontaland verticallines providedby the grid. Failureto breakset may be relatedto drew relation.The low spatialorientation groupalso had a lower mean thanthe high spatialorientationgroupfor drew relation. Failureto break set for Problem3 (Figure 3) might have been indicatedby the student'sinabilityto breakthe shapeinto partsto calculateits area.Studentsin the high spatialorientationgroupdemonstratedthat they could see a way to analyze the problemmore often than studentsin the low spatialorientationgroupby addingmarksto divide the figure into geometric shapes that they could measure (drewrelation). There was some indicationthat the mental movement category was related to estimateerrorfor the perceptualproblems.The differencefoundfor estimateerror indicated that the high spatial orientationgroup was better able to estimate the approximatemagnitude of the answer for the problems in this cluster. Mental LindsayAnne Tartre 227 movementfor Problem3 (Figure3) could have been indicatedby the studentwho, in an attemptto count completed rectangularshapes, said something like "this piece looks like it fits over here."It is possible thatthat statementmay be describing either one of two very differentmental processes. It may be arguedthat for some studentsthat statementindicatedmovement of a mental image, which is a spatialvisualizationskill. However,for otherstudentsit may have representedan informalmentalassessmentof the size and shapeof a particularpartof the figure, which is like the task for estimate error(perceptual),a spatialorientationskill. It is possible that the group of behavior categories just discussed-failure to breakset, drew relation,estimate error,and mentalmovement-may be different manifestationsof the same organizationalprocess, particularlyas they apply to geometriccontentproblems.For geometricproblems,the high spatialorientation group estimatedthe approximatemagnitudeof figures more accuratelythan the low spatialorientationgroup (estimateerrorand perhapsmentalmovement),was less likely to get stuck in an unproductivemind set (failureto breakset), and was more likely to add marksto show mathematicalrelationships(drewrelation). The two significantspatialorientationgroupdifferencesfound for nongeometric problems(done like andmisunderstoodproblem)supportthe thesis thatspatial orientation skill is manifested more broadly than just in geometric or visually presentedcontexts. It is an importantgoal of problem-solvinginstructionfor students to be able to identify the structureof a new problemand therebyrelate it to others, alreadysolved, that are structurallylike the new problem.Studentsin the high spatialorientationgroupindicatedmore often thanstudentsin the low spatial orientationgroup that they had done a problemlike it before and less often that they misunderstoodthe problem in nongeometric settings. The combinationof these two significantdifferencessuggested thatthe high spatialorientationgroup was betterable to build and link mental structuresby understandingthe problem and by linking the new problemto previous work. CONCLUSIONS The results from this study suggest that spatial orientationskill appearsto be used in specific and identifiable ways in the solution of mathematicsproblems. These ways include accuratelyestimatingthe approximatemagnitudeof a figure, demonstratingthe flexibility to change an unproductivemind set, addingmarksto show mathematicalrelationships,mentallymoving or assessing the size and shape of part of a figure, and getting the correct answer without help to a problem in which a visual frameworkwas provided. Supportwas also given to the idea thatspatialskill may be a more generalindicatorof a particularway of organizingthoughtin which new informationis linked to previousknowledge structuresto help make sense of the new material. Any relationshipbetweenmathematicslearningand spatialskill is dependenton the specific spatial skill or test employed. The test of spatialorientationthat was used for this studyhas not been used often for researchin mathematicsand spatial skills. And yet, the findings based on thattest are intriguing. 228 Spatial Orientation Attemptingto understandand discuss something like spatial orientationskill, which is by definition intuitive and nonverbal,is like trying to grab smoke: The very act of reachingout to take hold of it dispersesit. It could be arguedthat any attemptto verbalizethe processes involved in spatialthinkingceases to be spatial thinking.Spatial skill use is mental activity.Any evidence about how it is manifested must be indirect,since we cannotget into people's heads and see whatthey see in theirmind's eye. Often, the processes involved are not even understoodby the people experiencingthem.The resultingindirectnessof the researchin this area does set limits on it but shouldnot curtailit. If spatialskills areimportantto mathematics, then researchersmust find ways to identify and describethe specific roles that spatialskills play in doing mathematics. REFERENCES Anglin, L., Meyer,R., & Wheeler,J. (1975). Therelationshipbetweenspatial ability and mathematics achievementat thefourth- and sixth-gradelevels (WorkingPaperNo. 115). Madison:Universityof Wisconsin Researchand DevelopmentCenterfor Cognitive Learning. Bennett,G. K., Seashore,H. G., & Wesman,A. G. (1973). Differentialaptitudetests: Administrator's handbook.New York:The Psychological Corporation. Bishop, A. J. (1980). Spatial abilities and mathematicseducation-a review. EducationalStudies in Mathematics,11, 257-269. Connor,J. M., & Serbin,L. A. (1980). Mathematics,visual-spatialability, and sex roles. (Final Report).Washington,DC: National Instituteof Education(DHEW). (ERICDocument Reproduction Services No. ED 205 305) Connor,J. M., & Serbin,L. A. (1985). Visual-spatialskill: Is it importantfor mathematics?Can it be taught?In S. F. Chipman,L. R. Brush,& D. M. Wilson (Eds.), Womenand mathematics:Balancing the equation.(pp. 151-174). Hillsdale, NJ: LaurenceErlbaumAssociates. Davidson, P. S. (1979, May). Neurologicalresearchand its implicationson mathematicseducation. Journal of the VirginiaCouncil of Teachersof Mathematics,pp. 1-8. Ekstrom,R. B., French,J. W., & Harmon,H. H. (1976). Manualfor kit offactor-referencedcognitive tests. Princeton,NJ: EducationalTesting Service. Fennema,E., & Sherman,J. (1977). Sex-relateddifferencesin mathematicsachievement,spatialvisualizationand affective factors.AmericanEducationResearchJournal, 14(1), 51-71. Fennema,E., & Tartre,L. A. (1985). The use of spatialvisualizationin mathematicsby girls and boys. Journalfor Research in MathematicsEducation,16(3), 184-206. Franco,L., & Sperry,R. W. (1977). Hemispherelateralizationfor cognitive processing of geometry. Neuropsychologia,15(1), 107-113. Galyean,B. C. (1981). The brain,intelligence,andeducation:Implicationsfor gifted programs.Roeper Review,4(1), 6-9. HoughtonMifflin Company.(1971). Testsof academicprogress (FormS, mathematics,grades9-12). Boston: Author. Kersh,M. E., & Cook, K. H. (1979, August). Improvingmathematicsability and attitude,a manual. Seattle:MathematicsLearningInstitute,Universityof Washington. Linn, M. C., & Petersen,A. C. (1985). Emergenceandcharacterizationof genderdifferencesin spatial ability:A meta-analysis.ChildDevelopment,56, 1479-1498. Lorenz, K. Z. (1968). The role of gestalt perceptionin animal and humanbehaviour.In L. L. Whyte (Ed.).Aspectsofform: A symposiumonform in natureand art (pp. 157-178). New York:American Elsevier PublishingCompany. McGee, M. G. (1979). Human spatial abilities: Psychometric studies and environmental, genetic, hormonal,and neurologicalinfluences. Psychological Bulletin,86 (5), 889-918. Smith, I. M. (1964). Spatial ability: Its educational and social significance. London: University of LondonPress. LindsayAnne Tartre 229 Tartre,L. A. (1984). The role of spatialorientationskill in the solution of mathematicsproblemsand associated sex-related differences. (The University of Wisconsin-Madison). Dissertations Abstracts International46A: 94-95; July 1985. Tartre,L. A. (1990). Spatial skills, gender& mathematics.In E. Fennema& G. Leder(Eds.), Mathematics and gender: Influenceson teachers and students.New York:Teachers'College Press. Wheatley, G. H. (1977). The right hemisphere'srole in problemsolving. ArithmeticTeacher,25(2), 36-39. Witkin, H. A., Dyk, R. B., & Faterson,H. F. (1962). Psychological differentiation.New York: Wiley. AUTHOR LINDSAY ANNE TARTRE,Associate Professor,Departmentof Mathematics,CaliforniaState University-Long Beach, 1250 Bellflower Blvd., Long Beach, CA 90840
© Copyright 2025 Paperzz