The Impact of Positive Mood on Learning Author(s): Tanis Bryan, Sarup Mathur, Karen Sullivan Source: Learning Disability Quarterly, Vol. 19, No. 3 (Summer, 1996), pp. 153-162 Published by: Council for Learning Disabilities Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/1511058 . Accessed: 01/02/2011 21:24 Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of JSTOR's Terms and Conditions of Use, available at . http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp. JSTOR's Terms and Conditions of Use provides, in part, that unless you have obtained prior permission, you may not download an entire issue of a journal or multiple copies of articles, and you may use content in the JSTOR archive only for your personal, non-commercial use. Please contact the publisher regarding any further use of this work. Publisher contact information may be obtained at . http://www.jstor.org/action/showPublisher?publisherCode=cld. . Each copy of any part of a JSTOR transmission must contain the same copyright notice that appears on the screen or printed page of such transmission. JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact [email protected]. Council for Learning Disabilities is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to Learning Disability Quarterly. http://www.jstor.org THE IMPACTOF POSITIVE MOOD ON LEARNING Tanis Bryan, Sarup Mathur, and Karen Sullivan Abstract. The primary intent of this study was to examine the effects of a brief positive mood induction on a learning task that stimulates beginning reading acquisition. A secondary intent was to examine the durability of this effect across a period of two weeks. Sixty students, half average-achieving and half with learning disabilities, were randomly assigned to either a positive or a neutral mood induction condition. In an effort to control for the effects of prior knowledge, all students received instruction in elementary Hindi language on a series of five tasks. After two weeks, the instruction and tasks were readministered. The results of a MANCOVA indicated that both groups of students in the positive mood condition performed better than those in the neutral condition, although not statistically so. However, gain scores indicated that across a two-week period, students with learning disabilities in the positive condition performed significantly better than students with learning disabilities in the neutral condition. BACKGROUND affect has been one part of triparHistorically, tite models of the mind along with cognition (thinking)and conation (motivation and behavior) (Isen, 1984). This multifaceted perspective representing the tripartite nature of behavior dates back at least as far as the third century B.C., when the Greek physician Erastratosused psychophysiological observations, such as the disruption of a regular heart beat in a young man when his stepmother visited, to isolate the social cause of the individual's malady "lovesickness"(Mesulam& Perry, 1972). However, the empirical discipline of psychology operated until recently on the implicit assumption that cognition, conation, and affect could be adequately studied in separation from each other. In addition, the behavioristand cognitive models that have dominated psychological and educational research for the past several decades share one common characteristic:neither attributesmuch importance to the study of affective processes and reactions (Forgas, 1991). In the past 10 years, however, theorists have proposed a reintegrationof affect into research on cognition and behavior. For example, in an influentialpaper, Zajonc (1980) argued that affective reactions are often primaryto, and may precede, cognitive processing. Zajonc's work stimulatedgreat interest in research on the interface of affect and cognition in various areas of cognitive psychology and social psychology. One of the problems in studyingaffective phenomena has been to arrive at a definition of what emotion, affect, and related terms mean (Frijda,1986). In general, the term "emotion"is used to describe strong feelings that interruptongoing behaviors, result in specific actions, and involve physiological arousal (Isen, 1984). Emotions are intense, short-lived,and usuallyhave a TANIS BRYAN, Ph.D., is Professor Emeritus, University of Illinois at Chicago, and Adjunct Professor,Arizona State University. SARUP MATHUR, Ph.D., is Faculty Research Associate, Arizona State University. KAREN W SULLIVAN, Ph.D., is Faculty Research Associate, Arizona State University. Volume 19, Summer 1996 153 definite cause and clear cognitive content (e.g., annoyance, anger, or fear). In contrast, "mood" is used to describe low intensity and relatively enduring affective states with no immediately salient antecedent cause and little cognitive content (e.g., feeling good or feeling bad; Forgas, 1991). Affective states "gentlycolor and redirect ongoing thought and actions, influencing what will happen next but almost without notice and certainlywithout ostensibly changing the context or basic activity"(Isen, 1984, pp. 186-187). It is clear that the mechanisms that mediate between affective states and the cognitive processes involved in learning, performance, and behavior are of criticalimportanceboth in our privateand our working lives (Forgas, 1991). Although learning disabilities have been defined primarilyas deficits in informationprocessing, recent research has found that childrenand adultswith learning disabilitiesexperience significantly more negative and depressed affect than their nondisabled counterparts. Specifically, these individuals report more minor somatic complaints associated with feelings of anxiety and stress (Margalit& Raviv, 1984); greater test anxiety (Bryan,Sonnefeld, & Grabowski,1983); and more depression (Maag& Behrens, 1989). Rourke and colleagues (cf., Ozols & Rourke, 1985) have argued that there is a subtype of children with nonverbal learning disabilitieswho are vulnerableto problems in the affective and social domains. These children are described as being at risk for adolescent and adult depression and suicide (Rourke,Young, & Leenaars, 1989). Recently, Duane (1993) reported on eight subjects in mid- to late adulthood with evidence of right-hemispheredysfunctions. Each subject described a sense of chronic emotional unrest with periodsof depression;however,only one had previously been treated with psychotropic medication. Six of the eight had elevated depression or anxiety scores. Further,Peck (1985) reportedthat 50% of children under age 15 who committed suicide in Los Angeles County over a three-year period had been diagnosed as learningdisabled. It is not known whether findings of increased depression and anxiety among children and adults with learning disabilitiesare "reflectiveof chronic frustration,innate biologic risk related to anomalous CNS development or the genetics of emotion noted in the family history" (Duane, 1993, p. 7). What is known, however, is that 154 Learning Disability Quarterly there are important relationshipsbetween affect and cognitive processes. Thus, the accuracy and efficiency of thinking processes, perceptions, and behavior are significantly influenced by affective states, even mild and transientones. Negative affective states, for example, have been found to produce low-effort processing of informationand the use of less complex semantic processing strategies (Ellis, Thomas, & Rodriguez, 1984) and lower cognitive processing effort (Leight& Ellis, 1981). In contrast, positive affective states have been found to increase memory on various tasks (Potts, Morse, Felleman, & Masters, 1986); mastery of a discriminationtask (Masters,Barden, & Ford, 1979); altruism(Isen& Levine, 1972); and child compliance (Lay, Waters, & Park, 1989). Positive affect has also been shown to facilitate complex cognitive functions that require flexibility, integration,and utilizationof cognitive material (e.g., word association and memory, creativity, and problem-solving).In general, in studies of children, positive affect has shown a facilitating impact on memory, learning and behavior, whereas negative affect has a depressingimpact. The physiological relationshipsbetween negative and positive affect and cognitive processes are largely unknown. However, Schwartz (1993) reported that electroencephalographresearch by Ekmanhas linkedspecific patterns of brainactivity with smiling and positive affect. This pattern of brain activity could be self-inducedby having subjects show a "sincere" smile, defined as a smile that involves contractions of eye and mouth muscles at the same time. It may be possible for an individualto choose when to generate some of the physiologicalchanges that occur duringemotion by making the appropriatefacial expression (Schwartz,1993). A varietyof methods have been used to induce positive and negative moods: having subjects read self-statements (Velten, 1968); distributing cookies (Isen& Levine, 1972); inducinghypnotic states (Bower & Mayer, 1985); spraying a room with an air freshener (Baron, 1990); planting a dime in a phone booth (Isen & Levine, 1972); and giving a small amount of change (Isen,Horn, & Rosenhan, 1973). Most of the studies involving children have used a self-generatedvisualizationmethod to induce moods. Childrenwere asked to close their eyes and think of something that makes them happy or sad for periods ranging from 30 seconds to 2 minutes. Other studies used cartoons, movies, reading stories, or manipulatedwinning and losing scores on a game to induce moods. A number of studies have employed the selfgenerated visualization technique with students with learning disabilitiesand students at risk for referral to special education. The performance of children in self-generated positive moods was contrasted with that of students in a neutral mood generated by having students close their eyes and count to themselves from 1 to 50. Bryan and Bryan (1991a) employed students identifiedby teachers as being at risk for referral because of reading or math problems. The task involved learning five survivalvocabularywords. Childrenin the positive mood condition learned more words than children in the neutral condition. Two studies (Bryan & Bryan, 1991a, 1991b) examined the impact of mood induction on the performance of addition and subtraction problems. In one study, subjectswere childrenat risk for referral;in the other study, subjectswere third- through eleventh-grade students with severe learning disabilities attending a private school for students with learning disabilities.The results of both studies showed that students in the positive mood computed more problems accurately than students in the neutral mood condition. Yasutakeand Bryan (1995) used a short-term memory (digit span) task with first- and secondgrade students with severe learning disabilities. The results showed that students performed better in the positive than in the neutral condition. Similarly, using a listening comprehension task with students classified as high or low readers, Bryan, Yasutake, and Binstock (in preparation) found that good readers benefited much more than poor readers from the positive mood induction. Across these studies, self-generated positive moods have resulted in positive consequences. Not yet tested, however, is whether positive consequences would be found for tasks that require new learning, such as learning to read. The studies conducted to date in this area have used fairlysimple tasks, such as learning five vocabulary words (Bryan & Bryan, 1991a); a short-term memory task (Yasutake & Bryan, 1995); and the WISC-R Coding subtest (Yasutake & Bryan, in press). These tasks do not cap- ture the processing involved in beginning reading acquisition,however. Because these studies have had positive effects, we believe it is important to test whether self-generatedpositive moods facilitateacademic learning; for instance, learning to read. If so, induced positive affect could be employed as a classroom strategy. However, the transferability of this strategy to academic tasks has to be demonstratedbefore its adoption can be recommended. One purpose of the study reported here was to examine the impact of self-induced positive versus neutralaffect on a learning task that stimulated beginning reading acquisition. Second, since the durabilityof positive mood effects has not been examined, we examined the impact of positive moods on a posttest of learning conducted two weeks after the initial instructional session. METHOD Design The study was a 2 (group: LD-learning disabled placement, regular education placement) by 2 (condition: positive, neutral mood) unbalanced factorialdesign (Overall& Spiegel, 1969). Setting The setting was three middle schools in a suburb of Phoenix, Arizona. The school population includedchildrenfrom parents of low to high socioeconomic status. Subjects The total sample population was 66 students. Subjects included 28 students (14 males and 14 females) in 7th grade who had been classifiedby the school districtas having learning disabilities. The males' mean age was 13.07, SD .73; the females' mean age was 12.92, SD .47. In this school districtstudents are classifiedas having learning disabilitiesif they are of average intelligenceand have academic achievement that is at least one grade below grade level in several areas. Further,the discrepancy between intelligence and achievement must not be due primarily to environmental, cultural, behavioral, or speech-language factors, though these may be secondary contributingfactors. The mean Iowa Test of Basic Skills (ITBS) Reading score for males was 63.57, SD 11.06. For females the mean ITBS Reading score was 58.71, SD 13.27. The mean ITBS Language score was Volume 19, Summer 1996 155 53.2, SD 12.81 for males; for females, the mean was 55.14, SD 14.20. The group withoutlearningdisabilitiesincluded 38 7th-grade students, 22 males, 16 females. The mean age of the males was 12.71, SD. 56; the mean age for the girls was 12.56, SD .51. Only students who were not experiencing learning difficulties or were not at risk for learning problems as indicated by teacher observations were includedin the nondisabledgroup. The mean ITBS Reading score for males was 76.68, SD 16.09; for females, the mean was 83.81, SD 13.92. The mean ITBS Language score for males was 74.86, SD 14.36; for females, the mean was 80.00, SD 15.72. Table 1 represents the subjects' age and ITBS Reading and Language scores by conditions and placements. Task A major concern in conducting a study of learning is to ensure that prior knowledge does not make the experimental task easier for some subjectsthan others. To control for students' entry-levelskillsand knowledge, therefore, we constructed a series of tasks in which students learned to sight read a set of Hindi nouns and verbs. Task 1. Students were shown a series of five cards. Each card showed a pictureof a noun and its Hindi transcription (e.g., girl-larki, boy-larka). As the experimenterpresented each card to the subject,she said the noun and the student repeated it. Students were told the English translationof each noun (e.g., "InEnglishKhargosh means rabbit. This is how khargosh is written in Hindi").This process was repeatedtwice. UI Table 1 Subjects' Age and ITBS Reading Placements Boys N Age Reading ITBS Language ITBS LD Neutral 6 Language ITBS 156 by Conditions Non-LD Neutral 10 and Non-LD Positive 12 mean 13.16 mean 13.00 mean 12.60 mean 12.81 0.98 12.00 14.00 68.00 6.29 60.00 75.00 55.00 13.90 43.00 81.99 SD min max mean SD min max mean SD min max 0.53 12.00 14.00 60.25 13.87 40.00 82.00 51.87 12.73 37.00 73.00 SD min max mean SD min max mean SD min max 0.51 12.00 13.00 68.90 14.24 50.00 97.00 71.20 15.98 42.00 96.00 SD min max mean SD min max mean SD min max 0.60 12.00 14.00 83.16 15.08 66.00 110.00 77.91 12.74 63.00 100.00 12.87 0.37 12.00 13.00 51.28 14.20 40.00 80.00 54.71 18.93 32.00 89.00 mean SD 13.00 0.57 12.00 14.00 66.14 7.15 53.00 73.00 55.57 8.84 47.00 72.00 mean SD 12.75 0.46 12.00 13.00 76.75 14.57 53.00 93.00 77.87 20.41 42.00 102.00 mean SD 7 mean SD min max Reading ITBS LD Positive 8 Scores SD min max mean SD min max mean SD min max Girls N Age and Language mean SD min max mean SD min max Learning Disability Quarterly 7 min max mean SD min max mean SD min max 8 min max mean SD min max mean SD min max 8 12.37 0.51 min 12.00 max 13.00 mean 90.87 SD 9.43 min 76.00 max 106.00 mean 82.12 SD 10.13 min 60.00 max 93.00 On the second trial, the student was asked to translatethe Hindiwords into English.Then, with the five cards face up on the table, the experimenter said each word in Hindi and the student pointed to the correspondingcard. Studentswere scored for the number of nouns identified correctly(0 to 5). On this and subsequent tasks, students were given a 10 second wait time to respond. If the student did not respond within 10 seconds, the experimenter moved to the next word or task. Students received no feedback during the tasks. At the end of each task, however, the student was told, "Youdid reallywell. A lot of kids find it hard." Task 2. On this task a set of five verbs were taught using the same procedures as in Task 1. Students were scored for the number of verbs correctlyidentified(0 to 5). Task 3. The five noun and the five verb cards were displayed on the table with the nouns in a row above the verbs. The experimentermodeled the task by saying a two-word, noun-verb sentence, pointingto the noun from the top row and the verb from the bottom row. The experimenter then uttered 10 two-wordsentences and the student pointed to the noun-verbcards represented in each sentence. Students'scores were based on the number of correctly identified nouns and verbsused in the 10 sentences (0 to 20). Task 4. The cards were now removed. The experimenter uttered 10 two-word, noun-verb sentences, which the student translatedinto English. Scores were the number of correctlytranslated words (0 to 20). Task 5. The 10 cards were shuffled and spread out on the table. The experimenter pointed to each card and the student read the Hindi word for each. Scores were the number of words correctlytranslated(0 to 10). Procedures Subjects with and without learning disabilities were randomlyassigned to one of the two mood induction conditions (positive, neutral).Students were individuallybrought to a vacant classroom in the school. To avoid experimenter bias, the three graduate assistants who served as the experimenters were "blind"to the subjects' mood assignment. One of the co-investigatorsserved as the mood inductor for all students. In the positive mood condition, studentswere asked to close their eyes and think of a happy moment in their lives for 45 seconds. In the neutral mood condition, subjects were asked to close their eyes and count silently from 1 to 50 for 45 seconds. The mood inductor then left the room, and one of the graduateassistants entered and taughtthe tasks. To check on the mood induction,the co-investigator returnedat the end of the session to ask students in the positive mood condition, "What did you think." Students were asked to share their thoughts only if they wished to, and their responses were recorded. Students in the neutral mood condition were asked how high they had counted. Follow-up test. Two weeks later, students were individually retested using the same five tasks and like experimental conditions. At this time, there was no mood induction. RESULTS The study was a factorialdesign that included two placements (students with learning disabilities and nonlearning disabled students) by two affect conditions (positive and neutral) across two administrationsof a series of five learning tasks designed to measure students' abilityto recall Hindi language nouns and verbs. The data set consisted of the dependent measures: results of five learning tasks and totals from the initial testing, results of the follow-up retesting two weeks later, and totals - gain scores (the difference between the totals of the initial testing and the follow-up testing) - and specific demographicdata. Initial examination of the data indicated that students with learning disabilitiesin the positive condition outperformed their LD peers in the neutral condition on three of the five subtests during the first testing, on four of the five subtests in the follow-up testing, and on the total scores of both initial and follow-up tests. Nonlearning disabled students in the positive condition outperformed their NLD peers in the neutral condition on all five subtests on initial testing, but failed to outperform them on all but one of the follow-uptests. Total-score performance for NLD students in the positive condition was greater than that of NLD students in the neutral condition. Further, NLD students outperformed students in LD placements on all tasks, both in the initialand follow-up testing sessions. Table 2 presents the Volume 19, Summer 1996 157 UII Table 2 Means and Standard Deviations of Scores on Five Hindi Language Total Scores for Initial and Follow-Up Tests Task Task 1 Mean SD Task 2 Mean SD Task 3 Mean SD Task 4 Mean SD Task 5 Mean SD TOTALMean SD GAINS Mean SD LD-N 2.93 1.03 1.46 1.30 8.26 4.46 6.66 5.55 1.86 1.06 21.20 12.13 Initial Test LD-P NLD-N 2.67 3.76 1.53 1.71 1.66 2.07 1.12 1.50 10.26 11.84 4.53 3.76 8.20 10.92 3.82 4.09 2.93 1.46 1.68 1.89 24.06 30.76 10.32 11.17 means and standarddeviations of LD and NLD students in positive and neutralconditionson initial and follow-uptesting, as well as gain scores. Data analysis was conducted at three levels. The first level of analysis examined the data set with respect to potential effects of the categorical variables of age, gender, experimenter, school, and reading and language ability as reflected by ITBS scores. Table 1 presents the means and standard deviations for Age and ITBS Reading and Language scores. Had the results yielded significantp values for any of these variables, they would have been included in a MANCOVAas covariates, hence controlling for the effects of these categorical variables. However, since none of these factors was found to be significant,they were not used as covariates. As random sample selection was not possible, the total test score was used as a covariate in an effort to control for possible sample bias. Separate Multivariate Analyses of Covariance (MANCOVAs)were then used to test the effects of condition and placement on the dependent measures of the initialand follow-uptests. The results of the MANCOVA of the initial testing revealed no significant main effects for 158 Learning Disability Quarterly NLD-P 3.85 0.94 2.64 1.83 13.21 3.19 12.35 2.62 3.92 1.68 35.92 6.34 LD-N 3.64 1.33 2.57 1.33 11.07 4.32 8.42 5.24 3.14 2.47 28.64 12.25 6.92 6.30 Tasks and Follow-Up LD-P NLD-N 4.20 4.76 1.08 0.59 2.06 3.92 1.11 1.25 12.20 16.69 4.52 3.32 11.93 16.61 5.00 4.68 4.13 7.23 1.90 2.16 34.67 49.23 12.12 10.78 10.60 18.46 11.83 6.73 NLD-P 4.57 0.76 3.50 1.09 16.87 2.21 16.21 2.42 6.37 1.98 47.50 5.41 11.57 6.48 placement or condition and no interaction between condition and placement. Even though initial review of the data indicated that students in the positive condition as well as students in regulareducation placement achieved higher total scores, these differences were not found to be statisticallydifferent. MANCOVAresults of the follow-up tests indicated a highly significant main effect for placement, Wilks'Lambda= 0.729, F(5,47) = 3.502, p < .0009. UnivariateF tests for the five individual tasks are presented in Table 3. As shown, students in regular education settings outperformed students in LD placements on four out of the five tasks. No main effect was noted for condition. However, a significantinteractionwas indicated between placement and condition, Wilks' Lambda= 0.788, F(5,47) = 2.526, p < .042. UnivariateF tests for the five individualtasks are presented in Table 4. Three out of the five tasks showed significantor marginallysignificant interactionsbetween placement and condition. The third level of analysis involved examining the gain score, or difference between the total scores on the initialand follow-up tests. Table 2 presents the means and standard deviations of the gain scores by placement and condition. A one-way Analysis of Covariance (ANCOVA)was performed. The initialtest total of five tasks was used as a covariateto attempt to control for bias as in the previouslyreported MANCOVA.Table 5 is a source table for the results of this ANCOVA. A significant main effect was found for placement as well as a significantinteraction between placement and condition. In order to ascertain the nature of this interaction, a Tukey's HSD post-hoc test of significance was conducted. Results indicatedthat students in LD placements in the positive condition significantly outperformed students with LD in the neutralcondition, p < .000. Verification of moods found that all but two studentsin the positive condition reportedhaving happy thoughts. Specifically,they reportedthinking about passing school, music, dance, going out with friends, "my cat Oliver,"and so forth. One student said he could not think of anything, and one said he didn't know what his thoughts were. With the exception of these students, it appears that the inductionmethod used was effective in inducingpositive thoughts by students. DISCUSSION The most interesting and importantfinding of this study is the impact of positive mood on the performance of the students with learning dis- abilities on the follow-up test conducted two weeks after the original learning experience. Thus, examination of group means on the initial and follow-up tests revealed that LD students in the positive conditions performed better than their LD peers in the neutral condition. The differences on the initial testing were not statistically different, but on the posttest students with learning disabilities significantly outperformed their LD peers in the neutralcondition. Hence, it appears that the positive mood induction, carried out two weeks prior to the posttest, may have increased the differences between the students with learning disabilities. This interaction effect may be considered a sleeper effect, in which the results of instruction are not immediatelydemonstratedbut appear at some later time. A sleeper effect was also found by Bay, Stavers, Bryan, and Hale (1992), who compared immediate learning and generalization of science learning among students with learning disabilitiesand average-achievingstudents under two instructionalstrategies. No group differences were found on the immediate test, but the students with learning disabilitiesperformed significantly better on the posttest of generalization under one of the teaching strategies. In this study, as in Bay et al. (1992), the effects of the teaching strategies would not have been found if posttest resultshad not been examined. Table 3 Follow-Up Test - Univariate F Tests of Placement Univariate F Tests Variable Task1 Error Task2 Error Task3 Error Task4 Error Task5 Error SS 1.403 43.646 12.754 67.674 78.019 420.286 118.838 622.169 35.759 151.281 DF 1 51 1 51 1 51 1 51 1 51 Effect for Five Tasks MS 1.403 0.856 12.754 1.327 78.019 8.241 118.838 12.199 35.759 2.966 F 1.640 P 0.206 9.611 0.003** 9.467 0.003** 9.741 0.003** 12.055 0.001** **p< .01. Volume 19, Summer 1996 159 I Table 4 Follow-Up Test - Univariate F Tests of Placement Five Tasks Univariate F Tests Variable Task1 Error Task2 Error Task3 Error Task4 Error Task5 Error SS 2.568 43.646 0.209 67.674 9.186 420.286 76.550 622.169 17.210 151.281 DF 1 51 1 51 1 51 1 51 1 51 by Condition Interaction for MS 2.568 0.856 0.209 1.327 9.186 8.241 76.550 12.199 17.210 2.966 F 3.001 P 0.089 0.157 0.693 1.115 0.296 6.275 0.015* 5.802 0.020* *p < .05. L Table 5 Source Table: Analysis of Covariance for Gain Scores Analysis of Variance Source Condition Placement Condition *Placement Pretotal Error I SS 4.092 925.722 DF 1 1 MS 4.092 925.722 F 0.071 16.044 P 0.791 0.000** 331.612 2847.658 2942.698 1 1 51 331.612 2847.658 57.700 5.747 49.353 0.020* 0.000 *p < .05. **p< .01. II How do we explain this "sleeper"effect? Isen (1987) suggested that it is easier to recall and utilize material in memory under the influence of a positive mood state; that is, that positive affect results in a more efficient utilization of cognitive material than neutral or negative moods. Because of evidence showing that positive affect facilitates complex cognitive functions requiringflexibility,integration, and utilization of cognitive material such as memory, categorization, creative problem-solving, deci160 Learning Disability Quarterly sion-making and learning, it is reasonable to assume that it also affects underlyingcognitive organization (Isen, 1987). Positive affect may influence cognitive organization such that cognitive materialis more integratedand related than might occur without the influence of positive affect. Although clearly hypothetical, we suggest that somehow, perhaps through rehearsal, positive moods induced students with learning disabilities to organize the material in memory for better recall. Earlierstudies have reported main effects for induced positive moods on the learning and performance of students with learning disabilities (e.g., Bryan & Bryan, 1991a, 1991b; Yasutake & Bryan, in press). Two exceptions have been reported. For example, Yasutake and Bryan (in press) found that students in the neutral mood condition did better than students in the positive mood condition on the Woodcock-Johnson (1978) math computation subtest. Bryan, Yasutake, and Binstock (in preparation) found that good readers outperformed poor readers on a listening comprehension task. We believe that the math computationsubtest, the listening comprehension, and the Hindi reading tasks used in the present study were more difficultfor students than the tasks in the other affect studies. On the Hindi tasks, there were neither floor nor ceiling effects. This suggests that research is needed to establish such parameters as which tasks, materials, and even instructionalmethods are affected by and interact with induced mood states. To the best of our knowledge, this study represents the first attempt to examine the durability of induced positive moods on learning. Isen, Clark, and Schwartz (1976) studied the duration of mood states in field studies of altruism.Positive moods were induced by presenting adults with a free sample of stationery, or no sample for the neutral mood. At designated time intervals, from 3 minutes up to 20 minutes, participants were called on the telephone and asked to contact someone else by phone. The results indicated that induced positive moods did not last past 20 minutes. Also, a negative linear trend suggested that as the time intervalincreased, the effect on altruismdecreased. The results of the present study, on the other hand, suggest that induced moods may have a far more robust influence on children's learning than they did on adults'altruism.For example, it is remarkablethat 45 seconds of positive thinking had a significant impact on children'slearning across a two-week time span. Whether these differences are reliable, and which factor(s) account for the differences, calls for furtherstudy. Thus, the results of this study suggest that there is a need for furtherresearch on the relationship between affect and task difficulty. Inductionof positive affect in the cognitive domain appears to be a promising area of research and interventionwith students with learning disabilities. However, the effects of positive mood are complex. For example, there may be significant interactions between mood, frequency of inductions, age, child factors, and cognitive tasks. Because studies have found students with learning disabilitiesto experience greater depression and anxiety than nonlearning disabled students (Maag & Behrens, 1989), an additional question relates to whether prior moods, such as general depressive states, influence efforts to induce positive moods. It has been suggested that positive and negative affect have differenteffects on cognitive processing (Forgas, 1991). It is not clear whether they are two separate or relativelyindependent systems. Many aspects of these areas need to be studied, but it is clear that futureresearch on the interrelationships between affect and cognitive processing is likely to have implications for understandingthe nature of learning disabilities,as well as contemporarymodes of assessing the effects of intervention. REFERENCES inducedposiBaron,R.A. (1990). Environmentally tive affect: Its impact on self-efficacy,task performance,negotiation,and conflict.Journalof Applied Social Psychology, 20, 368-384. Bay, M., Stavers,J., Bryan,T., & Hale,G. (1992). for the mildlyhandicapped: Direct Scienceinstruction versusdiscoveryteaching.Journalof Reinstruction search on Science Teaching, 46, 78-90. Bower, G.H., & Mayer,J.D. (1985). Failureto replicatemood-dependentretrieval.Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 23, 39-42. F. (1981). Bryan,J.H., Sonnefeld,J., & Grabowski, Children'sand parents'views aboutingratiationtactics. Learning Disability Quarterly, 4, 170-179. Bryan,T., & Bryan,J. (1991a).Positivemoodand math performance. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 24, 490-494. Bryan,T., & Bryan,J. (1991b). Positive mood and vocabulary learning. Unpublished manuscript, of Illinoisat Chicago. University Bryan,T., Yasutake,D., & Binstock,E. (inprepara- tion). The effect of induced positive mood on high and low readers' comprehension. Unpublished of Illinoisat Chicago. manuscript, University Duane, D.D. (1993). Later-life affective, cognitive and neurological manifestations of developmental right-hemisphere syndromes. Paper presented at the AnnualMeetingof the International Academyfor Researchin LearningDisabilities, Boston. Ellis, H.C., Thomas, R.L., & Rodrigues, I.A. (1984). Emotionalmoodstatesand memory:ElaboraVolume 19, Summer 1996 161 tive encoding, semantic processing, and cognitive effort. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 10, 470-482. Forgas, J.P. (1991). Affect and social judgments:An introductoryreview. In J.P. Forgas (Ed.),Emotion and social judgments (pp. 3-30). New York:Pergamon. Frijda, N.H. (1986). The emotions. Cambridge: CambridgeUniversityPress. Isen, A.M. (1984). Toward understandingthe role of affect in cognition. In R.S. Wyer, Jr., & T.K. Srull (Eds.), Handbook of motivation and cognition (Vol. 3, pp. 179-236). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Isen, A.M. (1987). The influence of positive and negative affect on cognitive organization. In N.L. Stein, E. Leventhal,& T. Trabasso(Eds.),Psychological and biological approaches to emotion (pp. 7594). Hillsdale,NJ: LawrenceErlbaum. Isen, A.M., Clark, M., & Schwartz, M.F. (1976). Duration of the effect of good mood on helping: "Footprintson the sands of time." Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 34, 385-393. Isen, A.M., Horn, N., & Rosenhan, D.L. (1973). Effects of success and failureon children'sgenerosity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 27, 239-247. Isen, A.M., & Levine, P.F. (1972). Effect of feeling good on helping: Cookies and kindness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 21, 384-388. Lay, K., Waters, E., & Park, K.A. (1989). Maternal responsiveness and child compliance: The role of mood as a mediator.Child Development, 60, 14051411. Leight, K.A., & Ellis,H.C. (1981). Emotionalmood states, strategies, and state-dependency in memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 20, 251-266. Maag, J.W., & Behrens, J.T. (1989). Depression and cognitive self-statementsof learning disabled and seriously emotionally disturbedadolescents. Journal of Special Education, 23, 17-27. Margalit,M., & Raviv, A. (1984). LDs' expression of anxiety in terms of minor somatic complaints. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 17, 226-228. Masters, J.C., Barden, C., & Ford, M.E. (1979). Affective states, expressive behavior, and learning in children.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 380-390. I 0-rT111j71= M-J MNOVEMBER "LITERACY: Mesulam, M., & Perry, J. (1972). The diagnosis of lovesickness: Experimentalpsychophysiology without the polygraph.Psychophysiology, 9, 546-551. Overall,J., & Spiegel, D. (1969). Concerning least squares analysis of experimental data. Psychological Bulletin, 72, 311-322. Ozols, E.J., & Rourke, B.P. (1985). Dimensions of social sensitivityin two types of learning disabledchildren. In B.P. Rourke (Ed.), Neuropsychology of learning disabilities: Essentials of subtype analysis (pp. 281-301). New York:Guilford. Peck, M. (1985). Crisis interventiontreatment with chronically and acutely suicidal adolescents. In M. Peck, N.L. Farberow, & R.E. Litman (Eds.), Youth suicide (pp. 112-122). New York:Springer. Potts, R., Morse, M., Felleman, E., & Master,J.C. (1986). Children'semotions and memory for affective narrativecontent.Motivationand Emotion, 10, 39-57. Rourke, R., Morse, M., Felleman, E., & Master, J.C. (1986). Children'semotions and memory for affective narrative content. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 22, 169-175. Schwartz, J. (1993). Sincere smile delivers good vibes. Arizona Republic, December 5. Velten, E. (1968). A laboratorytask for inductionof mood states. Behavior Research and Therapy, 6, 473-482. Wechsler, D. (1974). Manual for the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Revised. New York: PsychologicalCorporation. Woodcock, R. (1978). Woodcock-Johnson PsychoEducational Battery. Boston: TeachingResources. Yasutake,D., & Bryan, T. (1995). The influence of induced positive affect on middle school childrenwith and without learning disabilities.Learning Disabilities Research and Practice, 10, 38-45. Yasutake,D., & Bryan, T. (in press). The influence of affect on achievement and behavior of students with learning disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities. Zajonc, R.B. (1980). Feeling and thinking: Preferences need no inferences. American Psychologist, 35, 151-175. Requests for reprintsshould be addressedto: Dr. Tanis Bryan, Special EducationProgram, College of Education, Arizona State University, Box 872011, Tempe, AZ 85287-2011. _ THE TRAIL 6-9, 1996 TO FREEDOM" P L A C E: Park Plaza Hotel, Boston, MA C O N TA C T: The Orton Dyslexia Society ? 8600 LaSalle Rd., 382 Chester Bldg. Baltimore, MD 21286-2044 (410) 296-0232 * FAX (410) 321-5069 E-mail:[email protected] 162 Learning Disability Quarterly
© Copyright 2025 Paperzz