459755 5Journal of Attention DisordersPillow et al. © 2012 SAGE Publications JAD18310.1177/108705471245975 Reprints and permission: sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav Article Beliefs Regarding Stimulant Medication Effects Among College Students With a History of Past or Current Usage Journal of Attention Disorders 2014, Vol. 18(3) 247–257 © 2012 SAGE Publications Reprints and permissions: sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav DOI: 10.1177/1087054712459755 jad.sagepub.com David R. Pillow1, Lavelda J. Naylor1, and Glenn P. Malone1 Abstract Objective: To examine the beliefs of ADHD college students concerning stimulant medications and to apply the theory of planned behavior toward better understanding the factors instrumental in decisions regarding stimulant use. Method: A cross-sectional, correlational design was used, and students completed a survey under controlled laboratory conditions. Participants were 193 students taking introductory psychology who self-reported receiving a diagnosis of attention deficit disorder or ADHD and a treatment history of using stimulant medications. Results: Beliefs regarding the effects of medication use are represented by four factors ((i.e., improved attention/academics, loss of authentic self, social selfenhancement, and common side effects), where the first three significantly and systematically differentiate between those currently using stimulants and those who are not. Conclusion: To understand decisions regarding stimulant use, it is important to consider how college students perceive the positive and negative effects of the medication with respect to sense of self and social relationships. (J. of Att. Dis. 2014; 18(3) 247-257) Keywords ADHD, stimulants, medication intentions, attitudes, beliefs Properly assessed, beliefs and attitudes predict behavior and comprise the substrate for decision making (Ajzen, 1985, 1991). Previous studies have linked beliefs and attitudes to medical adherence across a wide range of domains (e.g., Bucks et al., 2009; Schuz et al., 2011). That said, not one reasonably sized, systematic investigation of college student beliefs concerning the use of stimulant medication to treat ADHD has been previously reported. This formative study was designed to describe beliefs about stimulants held by college students with a history of ADHD and to examine the link between those beliefs and choices about whether to use stimulant medications. Once viewed as a disorder afflicting individuals only during childhood, ADHD is now recognized as a disorder that can persist throughout the life span (Barkley, 1998). Approximately 3.3% to 5.3% of American adults live with the disorder (Barkley, Murphy, & Fischer, 2008) and between 2% to 4% of college-aged adults are estimated to suffer from ADHD symptoms (for a review, see DuPaul, Weyandt, O’Dell, & Varejao, 2009). Before the 1990s, it was uncommon to diagnose ADHD after childhood, and the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (4th ed.; DSM-IV; American Psychiatric Association, 1994) criteria for a valid ADHD diagnosis currently require symptom onset by 7 years of age. The adequacy of this age-related threshold has been questioned (Barkley et al., 2008), and emerging qualitative studies with small samples indicate that a significant proportion of ADHD college students now seek treatment and begin medical stimulant use in late adolescence (e.g., Loe & Cuttino, 2008). Decisions regarding treatment seeking and adherence for young children reside in the province of the parents, and the research examining parental beliefs and medication decisions for ADHD children is reasonably mature (Hoza, Johnston, Pillow, & Ascough, 2006); however, little is known about medical decision making for individuals who are far less likely to be closely supervised by parents (e.g., older adolescents and college-aged students). Meanwhile, the expression of ADHD symptoms among college students has been linked to a more difficult time adjusting to college life, academically (Frazier, Youngstrom, Glutting, & Watkins, 2007; Norvilitis, Sun, & Zhang, 2010), socially (Shaw-Zirt, Popali-Lehane, Chaplin, & Bergman, 2005), and psychologically (Reaser, Prevatt, Petscher, & 1 University of Texas at San Antonio, USA Corresponding Author: David R. Pillow, Department of Psychology, University of Texas at San Antonio, One UTSA Circle, San Antonio, TX 78249-0652, USA. Email: [email protected] Downloaded from jad.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on May 18, 2016 248 Journal of Attention Disorders 18(3) Proctor, 2007). Furthermore, it is well established that stimulants are highly efficacious for treating a myriad of ADHD symptoms among children, adolescents (Barberesi et al., 2006), and more recently among adults (Berman, Kuczenski, McCracken, & London, 2009; Meszaros et al., 2009). It is thus not surprising that prescriptions of stimulant medications for adults increased at an annual growth rate of 15.3% between 2000 and 2005 (Castle, Aubert, Verbugge, Khalid, & Epstein, 2007). Studies that have investigated stimulant use in college students tend to focus on stimulant misuse (Davis-Berman & Pestello, 2010; DuPont, Coleman, Bucher, & Wilford, 2008; McCabe, Knight, Teter, & Wechsler, 2005; Rabiner et al., 2009a, 2009b) rather than stimulant efficacy. As a result, less is known about the efficacy of stimulants among ADHD college students than among children. Even less is known about attrition rates of prescribed medication routines among ADHD college students. Research on noncollege samples indicates that individuals generally stay in treatment for an average of only 34 months (Barberesi et al., 2006). Toward understanding why ADHD college students stop using stimulants as prescribed, a handful of qualitative studies have been conducted. Meaux, Hester, Smith, and Shoptaw (2006) interviewed 15 college students between the ages of 18 and 21, and found that most of their study participants were diagnosed and began medication treatment during elementary school; however, more than half (54%) stopped using stimulants between seventh and ninth grade. According to Meaux et al., several participants reported moderating their medication intake in an effort to minimize the negative effects of stimulants and maximize the positive benefits. Negative effects included physical side effects (e.g., loss of appetite, tiredness), psychological effects (e.g., not feeling like oneself, loss of creativity), and negative social consequences (e.g., withdrawal from social interaction). Adolescents and college students have also reported experimenting with medication stoppage to determine whether they could be successful without medication (Krueger & Kendall, 2001; Meaux et al., 2006). Along those lines, a qualitative study of 11 adolescents, ages 13 to 19, concluded that adolescents assimilated their ADHD diagnosis as an integral and defining aspect of their identity (Krueger & Kendall, 2001). In addition, Loe and Cuttino (2008) conducted 16 interviews and concluded that college students with ADHD perceived that to achieve their academic goals with the aid of stimulant medication, they had to yield aspects of their genuine self. All 16 respondents were concerned about when to stop medication. Some thought they would like to end treatment after college and return to their “authentic selves.” In all, 2 reported that they did not intend to ever again use medication. Understanding the decisions of ADHD college students to initiate or terminate use of stimulant medications requires that we first understand their beliefs regarding the effects of using said medications. The present study was designed to explore the factors underlying the relevant belief structures of college students receiving stimulant treatment for ADHD and to determine whether those beliefs differentiate between those who are currently using stimulants versus those who have discontinued use. This study assessed beliefs, attitudes, and related cognitions using a format specified by the theory of planned behavior (TPB; Ajzen, 1985, 1991). The TPB posits that the intention to perform a behavior is a function of personally held general attitudes regarding the behavior, subjective norms, and perceived control. Attitudes refer to the general positive and/or negative cognitive/affective evaluations of the behavior. Subjective norms refer to the individual’s assessment of what others would want him or her to do, assuming that the individual considers the opinions of the others to be important. Perceived control refers to the individual’s assessment of whether he or she has the capability to perform the behavior. There is abundant research supporting the application of the TPB to healthrelated behaviors (for a review, see Godin & Kok, 1996). Moreover, TPB has been used successfully to predict medication compliance (Conner, Black, & Stratton, 1998), illegal substance use (Conner & McMillan, 1999), and exercise attrition (Yardley & Donovan-Hall, 2007). TPB specifies that general attitudes are a summative function of the beliefs one holds regarding the likely effects of a behavior multiplied by how one evaluates the consequences of those effects. Following the TPB measurement scheme, participants are asked to make separate judgments about (a) their beliefs regarding the likelihood of an outcome and (b) their evaluation of the consequence should that outcome occur. As an example, a participant is asked to judge the likelihood that using stimulants will cause weight loss (i.e., a belief assessed on a 7-point scale centered at 0). A follow-up question is then asked requiring the participant to make a consequence evaluation (CE) of that outcome (via a rating scale centered at 0). For example, assuming that the weight loss occurred, he or she judges whether the resultant weight loss would be bad or good. After multiplying these ratings together and summing across a large set of beliefs/consequences, the resultant summative evaluation is expected to account for general attitudes. The current study involves the assessment of 50 beliefs about stimulant medication. The first objective of the study was to identify the key dimensions underlying these beliefs, which is where we expected to find evidence of factors involving perceived medication effects on the autonomy of the self and participants social relationships. The second objective was to examine how these key belief structures differ across current versus past stimulant users. Consistent with the TPB, we hypothesized that current stimulant users, compared with past stimulant users, would perceive Downloaded from jad.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on May 18, 2016 249 Pillow et al. the positive effects of stimulant medications as more likely to occur, and negative effects as less likely. In addition, we expected that the quantitative analysis conducted here would shed light on the degree to which college students vary (or agree) in their beliefs regarding the benefits and costs of using stimulant medication. Finally, we hypothesized that current stimulant users, compared with past stimulant users, would have more favorable general attitudes, higher subjective norms, and greater perceived control. Method Participants Participants (N = 193) were undergraduates at a university in the southwest who completed the study as partial fulfillment of requirements for an Introductory Psychology course. Based on prescreening questions, participants self-reported having previously used stimulant medication to control symptoms of ADHD for a diagnosed disorder. At the outset of a subsequent data collection session, those who met the initial screening criteria were again asked to reaffirm their diagnosis and treatment history. Participants were asked separate questions regarding whether they had been diagnosed with attention deficit disorder (ADD) or ADHD. Separate questions were asked given that students often fail to recognize that ADHD refers to general disorder that includes an attentiononly subtype. Only those who confirmed their prescreen reports and reported having been diagnosed with ADD or ADHD were included in the analyses. Participants (60% male) ranged in age from 18 to 28 years (M = 19.4, SD = 1.81) and were 71% White, non-Hispanic origin; 22% Hispanic origin; 3% Black; and 4% Asian. These distributions are generally consistent with those represented in the broader ADHD literature (e.g., ADHD is relatively high in males, and samples studied are often pervasively nonHispanic White; Barkley et al., 2008). Self-reported age of diagnosis ranged from 2 to 24 years (M = 11.92, SD = 4.60). The median age of diagnosis was 12 years, and the modal age was 8 years (15.1%). A total of 55% reported receiving a diagnosis during childhood (12 years and younger), 41% during their teen years (13 to 19), and 4% as adults in their 20s. Participants reported having spent an average of approximately 6 years “mostly on” stimulant medication, with responses ranging from 1 to 15 years (M = 6.11, SD = 3.38). Participants reported experience with Adderall (73.6%), Ritalin (44.6%), Concerta (33.2%), Vyvance (12.4%), Focalin (7.8%), and other stimulant medications (each reported by less than 5% of participants). Strattera, a nonstimulant, was used by 6.2% of the sample, but rarely exclusively. The few who reported having used Strattera exclusively were not included in the final sample of 193 participants. Procedure Item generation and pilot study. To aid in the construction of our measures, eight self-reported ADHD undergraduates who had previously ended medication treatment participated in a qualitative pilot study (Naylor & Pillow, 2007). Pilot participants were asked to discuss the benefits and the negative consequences associated with taking medication, and, most importantly, to explain their reasons for ending stimulant treatment. All the interviewed participants noted benefits to taking medication: 100% reported improved concentration, 88% reported lengthened attention span, and 38% reported improved motivation and more emotional continuity. The most commonly reported negative consequences involved social problems and physical side effects (75%). Consistent with Meaux et al. (2006), most reported liking themselves better off the medication (75%), feeling that they lost highly valued social and psychological characteristics when on medication. All participants reported at least one complaint about physical side effects such as sleeplessness (63%), weight loss (38%), and/or irritability (25%). Nonetheless, participants indicated that the physical side effects were less central to their medication intentions than social and psychological consequences. Although, pragmatic factors such as diagnostic problems (38%) and insurance problems (13%) were also reported, those factors were given far less weight in accounting for decisions to end medication. The qualitative data summarized above were combined with previous empirical research and theory to develop rating scales related to the belief and perceived control components of Ajzen’s (1985) TPB. Primary study. Participants completed a computeradministered survey in a controlled laboratory setting that included two rooms, each designed to accommodate six persons per session, where persons sat in cubicles with dividers designed to provide privacy. Informed consent was obtained in writing for each participant prior to each session, and all procedures used were approved by the Institutional Review Board. Participants completed the survey via computer (i.e., SurveyMonkey), and most completed the survey in 30 to 40 min. Data collection was conducted across several semesters spanning from the fall of 2007 to the spring of 2010. After the first two semesters of data collection, a brief diagnostic inventory (i.e., the Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview [MINI]) was added to describe whether those in the selected population (i.e., students who had received a prescription for stimulant medications) met standard criteria for an ADHD diagnosis. The final sample of 193 participants was composed of 98 from the first two cohorts and 95 who completed the additional measures. Downloaded from jad.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on May 18, 2016 250 Journal of Attention Disorders 18(3) Measures Likelihood beliefs and CEs. A total of 50 beliefs regarding the effects of stimulant medications were assessed using a format derived from the TPB (Ajzen, 1985) in which each statement is rated on a 7-point scale from −3 (very unlikely) to 3 (very likely), with a 0 midpoint. A sample belief item would be, “For me, I think it is (unlikely/likely) that taking medication could result in better grades.” In the next section, the 50 beliefs were reevaluated with respect to their yoked consequences; these CE items assume the outcome (e.g., “If taking medication led to better grades, this would be [very bad/very good]”). As with beliefs, CEs were rated on a 7-point scale from −3 (very bad) to 3 (very good) with a 0 midpoint. The 50 beliefs and their yoked CEs were designed to represent the effects of stimulant medication with respect to five general domains: (a) core symptoms of ADHD, (b) academic achievement, (c) sociability/social consequences, (d) self autonomy/psychological consequences, and (e) physical side effects. In all, 10 items were generated for each domain, with 5 items representing positive aspects of each general domain (e.g., reduce anxiety) and 5 items representing negative aspects of each general domain (e.g., increase irritability). Attitudes and intentions. General attitudes were assessed using four items, each rated on a 7-point Likert-type scale. Two of the items were on scales anchored unfavorable to favorable (e.g., “My attitude toward using stimulant medication to treat ADD/ADHD in the next semester is __”). The other two items were anchored from bad to good (e.g., “For me, to NOT take stimulant medication to treat ADD/ ADHD next semester would be ___”). Internal consistency of the scale was strong, Cronbach’s alpha = .95. Intentions were assessed with two items, each rated on a 7-point scale anchored from unlikely to likely (e.g., “I intend to take stimulant medication next semester to treat my ADD/ADHD”), Cronbach’s alpha = .92. Subjective norms and normative beliefs. Following Ajzen (1985), subjective norms were assessed with a single item as follows: “Most people who are important to me think I should use stimulant medication to treat ADD/ADHD next semester.” This item was measured on a 7-point scale anchored, unlikely to likely. Evidence of strong convergent validity was obtained by correlating this single item with a measure of normative beliefs, r = .75, p < .001. Participants provided normative belief ratings for up to 14 persons (e.g., mother, father, sibling, best friend, doctor) by completing each item as follows: “My ___ thinks I (should/should not) stay medicated next semester.” These normative beliefs were weighted by a set of yoked ratings expressing the extent to which participants desire to behave as these persons think they should. Perceived control. Following Ajzen (1985), seven items were constructed to assess perceived control (e.g., “I am confident that I will be able to use stimulants next semester.”) Each of these items were rated on a 7-point Likerttype scale, anchored strongly disagree to strongly agree. One item was dropped given its low interitem correlation, average r = .09, leaving six items judged to be internally consistent, Cronbach’s alpha = .80. Two other subscales were constructed that tap into the construct of perceived control by assessing barriers (both scales were rated on a 7-point Likert-type scale): One scale assessed insurance and financial barriers with four items, Cronbach’s alpha = .78, and the other assessed time barriers with three items, Cronbach’s alpha = .49. ADHD history and diagnostic checks. Background information regarding the participants’ diagnoses and experience with stimulant medications was obtained via a series of questions. Regarding their diagnostic history, participants were asked if they had been diagnosed with either ADD or ADHD and at what age/grade level they were diagnosed. Participants were then asked to check—from a list—all medications used to treat their ADD/ADHD in the past, and if applicable, to check all medications currently being used. Each report was examined to verify that all participants retained in the final data set had clinically relevant experience with stimulant medications. To aid in verifying the self-reported diagnosis, the protocol was modified midway through the study by adding a symptom checklist based on the MINI (Sheehan et al., 1997, 1998). Demographics. Demographic questions gathered typical information about age, gender, marital status, and so on. These items were included to evaluate the characteristics of our sample as a match to the general ADHD population. Results Preliminary Analyses:The Relation of Beliefs and CEs to Attitudes Before exploring the nature of the beliefs assessed, it is important to demonstrate that the set of measured beliefs correlate strongly with general attitudes. The TPB postulates that general attitudes are a function of the sum total of each relevant Belief (B) weighted by its corresponding CE n (i.e., ∑ i =1 Bi ∗ CE i ). Using SPSS, the correlation between the sum total of each CE weighted B with general attitudes was found to equal .61, p < .001. This confirms the relevance of the items selected; however, the multiplicative weighting makes item interpretation extremely difficult, especially when attempting to identify constructs using factor analysis (e.g., double negatives, confounding). Further investigation supported using beliefs alone. Entering all 50 CE weighted beliefs as predictors in a regression equation accounted for 55.8% of the variance in general attitudes, multiple R = .75, p < .001. By comparison, no predictive utility is lost, but is instead gained when entering the Downloaded from jad.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on May 18, 2016 251 Pillow et al. unweighted beliefs, multiple R = .78, p < .001. In contrast, entering the consequence evaluations alone results in a multiple R of .61, p < .001. Interestingly, there is almost twice as much variance in the beliefs (MVariance of Beliefs = 3.28) than in the consequence evaluations (MVariance of CEs = 1.67). Factor Analysis of Stimulant Beliefs To identify the key structures that underlie college students’ general attitudes and decisions regarding stimulant use, a factor analysis of the 50 beliefs was conducted using maximum likelihood estimation, followed by an oblique (i.e., Oblimin) rotation of the data structure. The number of factors retained was determined based on examination of the scree plot, the number of items with high loadings per factor, and the simplicity of the resultant solution. Four factors, as shown in Table 1, were identified accounting for 44.37% of the variance in the items. These four factors, computed from factor regression scores in SPSS, predict general attitudes regarding stimulant use with a multiple R of .69 via regression analysis. Comparing multiple solutions across extraction methods, rotations, and criteria for number of factors extracted reveals robust evidence for the organization of items on the first three factors. These three factors each account for unique variance in general attitudes, ps < .01, and entered simultaneously yield a multiple R of .68. The fourth factor accounts for only 0.7% of the variance in general attitudes, p = .11. Competing solutions that break up the fourth factor into smaller factors are possible; however, these factor structures involve highly complex loadings and small numbers of items per factor. Items with loadings below .40 were deleted in constructing scale scores for further analyses. Based on the content of the items within each factor, we labeled the factors as follows: (a) improved attention/academics (IAA), (b) loss of authentic self (LAS), (c) social self-enhancement (SSE), and (d) common side effects (CSE). The fourth factor— CSE—was so named as it included a mix of items long recognized to be stimulant byproducts, where some were direct physical manifestations of stimulants (e.g., loss of appetite, wakefulness) and some were psychological in nature (e.g., increased irritability). Table 1 displays the top factor loadings, and also includes the means for each B and CE. It is interesting to note that the items clustered with respect to CEs even though CEs were not used in the factor analysis; participants perceived the IAA and SSE items to have positive consequences if they occurred, whereas they perceived the consequences of the LAS and CSE items as largely negative. The resulting four scales were found to be internally consistent, with Cronbach’s alphas of .92, .89, .90, and .83 for the IAA, LAS, SSE, and CSE, respectively. IAA correlated significantly with SSE (r = .46) and CSE (r = .25), ps < .01, but was independent of LAS (r = .01). LAS correlated inversely with SSE, r = −.20, p < .01, indicating that there is substantial divergence in the content of these two scales concerning the social self. In addition to correlating positively with IAA, CSE also correlated positively with LAS (r = .41) and SSE (r = .21), ps < .01. The finding that CSE correlated positively with IAA and SSE is consistent with the notion that stimulant use involves trade-offs between positive and negative consequences. Past Versus Current Stimulant Users’ Beliefs and consequence evaluations Regarding Stimulants The mean beliefs and consequence evaluations were examined across the resultant factor domains and between current and past stimulant users. Specifically, we conducted two mixed ANOVAs, one treating beliefs as the dependent variable (DV) and the other treating consequence evaluations as the DV. The first 4 × 2 × 2 ANOVA examined beliefs about the effects of stimulants treating belief domain as a within-participant factor (i.e., IAA, LAS, SSE, and CSE) and current medication status (two levels: current vs. past) and gender as between-participant factors. A second ANOVA treated consequence evaluations as the DV. No gender effects were obtained, and hence, gender was dropped from the analyses. What do ADHD college students believe are the most likely effects of using stimulant medications? This question was answered by probing a main effect that was obtained for the belief domain, F(3, 184) = 303.51, p < .001. Participants reported that IAA performance is the most likely effect (M = 1.65). CSE (M = −0.004) were rated on average as neither likely nor unlikely. SSE (M = −0.66) and LAS (M = −0.81) were perceived to be somewhat unlikely on average. With a Bonferroni correction, pairwise comparisons between means revealed that all differences were statistically significant but one, p < .05 (there was not a reliable difference between SSE and LAS). Do those currently using stimulant medications differ from past users in their beliefs regarding the likelihood of various stimulant medication effects? Yes. A main effect for medication status was obtained (MCurrent users = 0.19, MPast users = −0.10), F(1, 186) = 5.98, p < .05; however, this effect was qualified by a belief domain by medication status interaction effect, F(3, 184) = 5.35, p = .001. As shown in Table 2, those currently using stimulant medications perceived a greater likelihood of IAA and SSE than did past users. In contrast, past users (compared with current users) perceived a greater likelihood of negative effects consistent with loss of one’s spontaneous self (LAS). The two groups did not differ in their overall estimation of whether CSE are likely or not likely. Downloaded from jad.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on May 18, 2016 252 Journal of Attention Disorders 18(3) Table 1. Summary of Exploratory Factor Analysis for ADHD Beliefs About Stimulant Medication Use Using Maximum Likelihood Factor Analysis (N = 193). Factor loadings IAA Improve my grades Improve my concentration and focus Help me stay on task Help me achieve my academic goals Help me keep my school-related priorities balanced Help me to listen when spoken to directly Improve my organization LAS Make me less expressive in artistic pursuits Make me feel like I am not myself Take away important parts of who I am Decrease my ability to laugh and joke around with others Lead me to isolate myself from others Keep me from being creative Keep from being successful at things other than academics Keep me from pursuing multiple interests SSE Help me get along well with others Improve my general mood Improve my social interactions Allow my true personality to shine Make me approachable Enable me to foster friendship Enable me to get others to see me as I see myself CSE Decrease my ability to get a good night of sleep Cause me to talk excessively Make me more impulsive Help me stay awake Make me restless and fidgety Improve my energy level Cause me to lose my appetite MB MCE .872 .840 .829 .795 .694 .651 .632 2.07 2.26 2.05 1.98 1.61 1.70 1.45 2.70 2.63 2.48 2.58 2.31 2.18 2.33 .797 .788 .734 .650 .648 .647 .640 .626 −0.81 −0.01 −0.58 −0.71 −0.31 −1.17 −1.15 −0.89 −2.18 −2.61 −2.62 −2.38 −2.36 −2.45 −2.12 −1.90 .786 .750 .702 .690 .690 .664 .635 −0.18 −0.39 −0.41 −1.28 −0.43 −0.90 −1.16 2.03 2.09 1.99 1.95 1.52 1.49 1.34 −.622 −.615 −.610 −.599 −.555 −.522 −.447 0.067 −0.053 −0.076 1.52 0.23 0.23 1.42 −2.21 −1.06 −0.68 1.14 −2.11 2.04 −1.36 Note: IAA = improved attention/academics; LAS = loss of authentic self; SSE = social self-enhancement; CSE = common side effects. To conserve space, the seven items with the highest loadings on each factor are shown above. The full list of items is available from the authors on request. The means for each belief (MB) and consequence evaluation (MCE) are provided for the reader’s information. The higher the MB, the more likely participants perceived that the effect would occur; the higher the MCE, the more positive the consequence of having the associated effect occur. Do college students currently using stimulant medication differ from past users in their evaluation of the consequences of stimulant effects? No. In a second mixed ANOVA using consequence evaluations as the DV, there was neither a main effect for medication status nor a significant CE type by medication status interaction. There was, however, a main effect for CE domain, F(3, 184) = 807.63, p < .001. Participants perceived IAA very positively (M = 2.37), followed by a favorable evaluation of positive social enhancement (M = 1.65). Participants perceived CSE as having somewhat negative consequences on average (M = −.89) and perceived LAS as having very negative consequences (M = −2.29). Each of these pairwise comparisons, conducted with a Bonferroni correction, was significant, p < .05. Differences Between Past and Current Users on the TPB Constructs A multivariate ANOVA was conducted to examine differences between past and current stimulant users on the core constructs of the TBP (i.e., general attitudes, subjective norms, perceived control, and future medication intentions) Downloaded from jad.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on May 18, 2016 253 Pillow et al. Table 2. Mean Differences Between Past and Current Users of Stimulant Medications on Beliefs About Medication Effects and Core Constructs in the Theory of Planned Behavior. Past users (n = 117) M (SD) Current users (n = 76) X>0 Beliefs Improved attention/academics 1.20 (1.20) 89.5% Loss of authentic self −0.55 (1.43) 38.2% Social self–enhancement −0.92 (1.21) 21.1% CSE −0.01 (1.28) 48.7% Positively valenced items from the CSE factor Improves energy −0.23 (1.89) Helps me stay awake 1.11 (1.89) Theory of planned behavior constructs and auxiliary measures of barriers General attitudes about stimulants 3.50 (1.84) Subjective norms favoring use 2.57 (1.81) 4.20 (1.47) Perceived control over use 2.88 (1.98) Future intentions to use stimulants Financial barriers to use 2.26 (1.38) Time barriers to use 3.49 (1.24) M (SD) X>0 rpb 2.01 (0.75)** −0.94 (1.28)* −0.37 (1.29)* 0.11 (1.17) 97.4% 22.2% 38.5% 51.3% .38 −.14 .21 .05 0.53 (1.90)** 1.79 (1.56)** .19 .19 5.86 (1.17)** 5.88 (1.43)** 6.09 (0.98)** 5.88 (1.43)** 1.97 (1.10) 3.03 (1.20)* .62 .58 .61 .66 −.12 −.18 Note: CSE = common side effects. Percentages are provided to describe how many had scale scores (i.e., “X”) of 0 or above. Point biserial correlations (rpb) are provided as estimates of effect size. *p < .05. **p < .01. as well as two ancillary measures of barriers considered relevant to using stimulant medications. A significant multivariate effect was obtained for medication status, F(6, 185) = 28.11, p < .001, partial η2= .48. Subsequent univariate tests indicated significant differences between past and current stimulant users. Compared with past stimulant users, current stimulant users had more favorable attitudes, higher subjective norms, greater perceived control, and fewer perceived barriers involving time demands, ps < .01; differences on financial barriers approached significance, p = .08 (see Table 2 for means, standard deviations, and effect sizes). Notably, the effect sizes for measures of time- and financial-related barriers were small compared with effects of the core TPB constructs. Supplemental Analyses Individual differences in beliefs about stimulant medications. Examining the averages alone, current and past stimulant users evaluated the likelihood of LAS as relatively unlikely, denoted by means obtained on the negative end of the scale. That said, not all participants had scale scores in the negative range on LAS, and indeed, there was substantial evidence of individual differences. We calculated the proportion of persons who evaluated the loss of one’s authentic self as more likely than not, defined by scale scores greater than 0. (We similarly examined individual differences in the other belief domains by using the same cut point.) Doing so, we found that 38.2% of past users perceive some LAS as more likely than not (compared with 22.2% of current users). In contrast, 38.5% of current users perceive positive SSE effects as at least somewhat likely (compared with 21.1% of past users). These percentage breakdowns are provided for descriptive purposes; note that any implied proportional differences between groups were found to be redundant with the corresponding inferential tests of mean differences shown in Table 2. Although approximately 48% to 51% of the participants perceived CSE as more likely than not (i.e., scores greater than 0), the likelihood of such effects did not differ significantly between current and past stimulant users overall. However, it is noteworthy that only two of the items in this cluster were perceived as generally positively valenced via participant reports of consequence evaluations. On examining these two items in isolation, differences between current and past users were observed. Specifically, current users were more likely than past users to report that stimulant medication would help them stay awake and improve energy levels. The relevant means and differential proportions are shown in Table 2, where the items are displayed as breakouts from the general CSE scale score. Interestingly, current users did not differ from past users in their evaluation of the favorability of these effects. Is it possible that the items on the CSE scale would better predict general attitudes and differentiate between past and present users if not averaged to form a single scale score? Downloaded from jad.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on May 18, 2016 254 Journal of Attention Disorders 18(3) Before concluding that CSE items have little, if any, predictive value, we considered the possibility that these items—especially the negatively valenced items—might have greater predictive utility if treated individually. This possibility was examined by repeating an earlier presented regression analyses in which the first three factor scores representing IAA, LAS, and SSE were entered in Step 1, accounting for 46.8% of the variance in general attitudes about stimulant medications, multiple R = .684, F(3, 189) = 55.43, p < .001, where the standardized betas were .48, −.38, and .15, respectively, ps < .01. However, instead of adding the CSE factor as before, we subsequently entered all the items that loaded on the CSE factor in Step 2. This set of items resulted in a marginally significant R2 change of .058, ΔF(13, 176) = 1.65, p = .08. Further inspection revealed that three items were related to general attitudes: (a) concerns about losing “more weight than I need to,” β = −.16, t = −2.45, p < .05; (b) reduced “desire to join others at meal time,” β = −.14, t = −1.97, p = .05; and (c) “improve my energy level,” β = −.17, t = −2.20, p < .05. However, the two negative side effects concerning loss of appetite did not differentiate between past and current stimulant users, ps > .20. As noted earlier, current stimulant users were more likely to report improved energy levels. Are the effects the same for those meeting diagnostic criteria for ADHD? To answer this question, a series of ANOVAs were conducted to compare effects across participants who (a) completed the MINI and qualified for either a childhood diagnosis or an adult diagnosis of ADHD (n = 59) and (b) those who were not administered the MINI (n = 98) or did not qualify for a diagnosis based on the MINI (n = 36). The two mixed ANOVAs and the MANOVA conducted as the primary analyses above were each repeated entering diagnostic status as a factor, but no statistically significant interactions involving diagnostic status were obtained. Discussion The present research advances the literature by delineating the nature of the beliefs underlying the attitudes of ADHD college students toward stimulant use, thereby providing a framework for better understanding their decisions to initiate or terminate the use of stimulants. A few qualitative studies have previously explored the reasons college students with ADHD give for using or avoiding stimulant medication (e.g., Meaux et al., 2006), therein alluding to possible beliefs about stimulants (e.g., the loss of sense of self). There are also a few studies that have explored reasons for stimulant use among the general population (e.g., Rabiner et al., 2009b). However, the present study is the first to combine (a) a comprehensive assessment of beliefs about the effects of stimulant medications, (b) a theory guided and quantitative measurement/analytical approach, and (c) a reasonably sized sample of individuals self- reporting a diagnosis of ADHD with a history of using prescribed stimulants. It has been argued that decisions regarding stimulant use involve trade-offs between benefits and costs (Meaux et al., 2006). This study found evidence that the benefits can generally be described with respect to two factors summarized as IAA and SSE. Not surprisingly, improvement in core ADHD symptoms and academic performance was found to have the largest effect in differentiating between current and past stimulant users. However, given previous reports of individuals feeling “not themselves” on stimulants (Loe & Cuttino, 2008), it was surprising that approximately 38.5% of the current stimulant users in our study perceived that stimulant use improved their social self (e.g., improves mood, “allows (their) true personality to shine,” and enables them to foster friendships). Although it is well established that ADHD children behave more appropriately in social interactions as a result of using stimulant medication (Hinshaw, 1991), research exploring the related social benefits of medication in adults with ADHD is lacking. On the other side of the equation, the costs of using stimulant medication can also be summarized with respect to two factors: LAS and CSE. As the label implies, “common” side effects involving issues such as appetite, sleeplessness, and irritability are likely to be readily recognized by physicians and patients, and regularly discussed as issues involved in adjusting medications. It is unclear whether physicians are likely to talk about issues involving identity adjustment, personality changes, and psychological autonomy; however, our findings make clear that these issues are important to a substantial minority of patients. That is, issues involving one’s social identity distinguish between those who have decided to continue using stimulants and those who have decided to suspend or terminate medication, whereas negative CSE items do not. In this respect, our findings are consistent with reports that the physical side effects of stimulants used for treating those with ADHD are marginal and manageable (Connor, 2002; Efron, Jarman, & Barker, 1997). Interestingly, our analysis of consequence evaluations indicates that ADHD college students largely agree that LAS, if experienced, is more aversive than CSE consequences (e.g., increased irritability, weight loss, etc.). The weight given to LAS is understandable given the developmental concerns of those transitioning between late adolescence and young adulthood, an age when constructing one’s identity and developing intimate relationships are paramount (Erikson, 1956). Moreover, these quantitative findings complement previous qualitative studies detailing the personal stories of students who express concerns regarding losing one’s true self and/or one’s natural capacity for social fluidity on stimulants (Loe & Cuttino, 2008). Although current users did not differ from past users in their beliefs about the likelihood of negatively evaluated CSE items, current users believed stimulants would help them Downloaded from jad.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on May 18, 2016 255 Pillow et al. stay awake and provide extra energy more than past users. This is in keeping with studies reporting that students with and without ADHD have taken stimulants to aid with study demands (Davis-Berman & Pestello, 2010; DuPont et al., 2008; Rabiner et al., 2009a, 2009b). Consistent with this interpretation, “stay awake” correlated .32 with the IAA factor and was perceived to have largely positive consequences; however, stay awake correlated even more strongly with the item assessing decreased “ability to get a good night of sleep,” r = .52, ps < .01. This suggests that the benefit was tightly coupled with adverse effects, thereby accounting for its loading on the CSE factor. Although this study focused largely on beliefs as the basis for general attitudes, the variables most strongly differentiating current from past stimulant users were general attitudes, subjective norms, perceived control, and future intentions. The measures of general attitudes and perceived control have in common with our assessment of specific beliefs that they focus on the individual’s intrapersonal cognitions and affective states. Subjective norms, however, highlight concerns for how others (e.g., parents, friends, relatives, the individual’s physician, etc.) feel about using stimulant medication, and may suggest, consistent with the TPB, that interpersonal processes play an important role in the medication decisions made by college students. These subjective norms may contribute independently of beliefs and general attitudes to influence medication decisions, but the fact that subjective norms correlate strongly with general attitudes (r = .66) suggests that individuals also develop or change their beliefs and attitudes about stimulants based on their associations with others. Simply knowing others who use stimulant medications may influence decisions, where the positive experiences of others likely increase medication compliance; if friends and relatives have negative experiences with stimulants, individuals may be prone to formulate negative beliefs and cease medication. Of course, it should also be noted that the direction of causality may be reversed such that either beliefs or the behavioral decisions influence one’s perceptions of the subjective norms. Although the opinions of close others do have bearing, clearly more research is needed to sort out the role that subjective norms play in the medication decisions of college students. The primary purpose of the present study was to examine the nature of beliefs accounting for general attitudes about stimulant use. Our findings highlight that these beliefs can be organized around four important dimensions to be considered in further research, with the three factors representing improvements in academics and attention, LAS, and SSE accounting for most of the variance in general attitudes. CSE were not found here to account for substantial variance in general attitudes, and negative side effects did not differentiate between past and current users, whether CSE were averaged across items or examined as single predictors. Furthermore, this research indicates that there are substantial individual differences in general attitudes about stimulant use, the opinions of others regarding whether to use stimulants, and perceptions of control. The mean differences between current and past stimulant users on core TBP constructs were very large, suggesting that these measures may have predictive and clinical utility. The current research provides an important assessment of the beliefs that differentiate current stimulant users from past users, but clearly, future research is needed to specify the temporal or causal sequence relating those beliefs to behavioral decisions. This will require a longitudinal design to determine whether those beliefs have predictive utility or emerge as post hoc rationalizations. The dimensions identified here also have import for exploring interaction effects consistent with the view that decisions regarding stimulant medications involve trade-offs (e.g., LAS beliefs may moderate the predictive utility of IAA beliefs). A limitation of the study concerns whether our participants had a valid diagnosis of ADHD. Only a small percentage of the sample fully qualified for a diagnosis of ADD or ADHD based on testing using the MINI (Sheehan et al., 1997). Similar findings have been noted by Barkley et al. (2008) who argued that criteria for diagnosing adult ADHD may be overly stringent and that symptom count cutoffs are, to some extent, arbitrary. That said, no substantial differences in the pattern of results obtained when we examined the data for only those who, based on a retrospective assessment, met criteria on the MINI either as children or as adults. Although additional research will be necessary to answer the questions raised above, the present study sheds light on important individual differences to be considered in how selfreported ADHD college students understand the effects of stimulant medication. This study identifies important tradeoffs that self-reported ADHD college students perceive: On one hand, there are benefits with respect to symptom control, academic progress, and—in some cases—facilitation of a positive social image, and on the other hand, students must weigh the possible loss of their autonomous and natural sense of self, along with commonly experienced side effects. Acknowledgments The authors would like to thank Tamara Leyva and Cristina Cadena for their help in collecting this data and background research. They also thank Michael Baumann, Robert Fuhrman, Michelle Little, and Jonathan Pillow for comments and suggestions on previous drafts of this manuscript. Declaration of Conflicting Interests The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. Downloaded from jad.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on May 18, 2016 256 Journal of Attention Disorders 18(3) Funding The author(s) received no financial support for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. References Ajzen, I. (1985). From intentions to actions: A theory of planned behavior. Heidelberg, Germany: Springer. Ajzen, I. (1991). The theory of planned behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 50, 179-211. American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author. Barberesi, W. J., Katusic, S. K., Colligan, R. C., Weaver, A. L., Leibson, C. L., & Jacobson, S. J. (2006). Long-term stimulant medication treatment of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Results from a population-based study. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 27, 1-10. Barkley, R. A. (1998). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: A handbook for diagnosis and treatment (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Guilford. Barkley, R. A., Murphy, K. R., & Fischer, M. (2008). ADHD in adults: What the science says. New York, NY: Guilford. Berman, S. M., Kuczenski, R., McCracken, J. T., & London, E. D. (2009). Potential adverse effects of amphetamine treatment on brain and behavior: A review. Molecular Psychiatry, 14, 123-142. doi:mp200890 [pii] 10.1038/mp.2008.90 Bucks, R. S., Hawkins, K., Skinner, T. C., Horn, S., Seddon, P., & Horne, R. (2009). Adherence to treatment in adolescents with cystic fibrosis: The role of illness perceptions and treatment beliefs. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 34, 893-902. Castle, L., Aubert, R. E., Verbugge, R. R., Khalid, M., & Epstein, R. S. (2007). Trends in medication treatment for ADHD. Journal of Attention Disorders, 10, 335-342. Conner, M., Black, K., & Stratton, P. (1998). Understanding drug compliance in a psychiatric population: An application of the theory of planned behaviour. Psychology, Health & Medicine, 3, 337-344. Conner, M., & McMillan, B. (1999). Interaction effects in the theory of planned behavior: Studying cannabis use. British Journal of Social Psychology, 38, 195-222. Connor, D. (2002). Preschool attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: A review of prevalence, diagnosis, neurobiology, and stimulant treatment. Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 23, S1-S9. Davis-Berman, J. L., & Pestello, F. G. (2010). Medicating for ADD/ADHD: Personal and social issues. International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction, 8, 482-492. DuPaul, G. J., Weyandt, L. L., O’Dell, S. M., & Varejao, M. (2009). College students with ADHD: Current status and future directions. Journal of Attention Disorders, 13, 234-250. DuPont, R. L., Coleman, J. J., Bucher, R. H., & Wilford, B. B. (2008). Characteristics and motives of college students who engage in nonmedical use of methylphenidate. American Journal on Addictions, 17, 167-171. Efron, D., Jarman, F., & Barker, M. (1997). Methylphenidate versus dexamphetamine in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: A double-blind crossover trial. Pediatrics, 100, E6. Erikson, E. H. (1956). The problem of ego identity. Journal of American Psychoanalytic Association, 4, 56-121. Frazier, T. W., Youngstrom, E. A., Glutting, J. J., & Watkins, M. W. (2007). ADHD and achievement: Meta-analysis of the child, adolescent, and adult literatures and a concomitant study with college students. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 40, 49-65. Godin, G., & Kok, G. (1996). The theory of planned behavior: A review of its applications to health-related behaviors. American Journal of Health Promotion, 11, 87-98. Hinshaw, S. P. (1991). Stimulant medication and the treatment of aggression in children with attentional deficits. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 20, 301-312. Hoza, B., Johnston, C., Pillow, D. R., & Ascough, J. C. (2006). Predicting treatment response for childhood attention-deficit/ hyperactivity disorder: Introduction of heuristic model to guide research. Applied & Preventative Psychology, 11, 215-229. Krueger, M., & Kendall, J. (2001). Descriptions of self: An exploratory study of adolescents with ADHD. Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Nursing, 14, 61-72. Loe, M., & Cuttino, L. (2008). Grappling with the medicated self: The case of ADHD college students. Symbolic Interaction, 31, 303-323. McCabe, S. E., Knight, J. R., Teter, C. J., & Wechsler, H. (2005). Non-medical use of prescription stimulants among US college students: Prevalence and correlates from a national survey. Addiction, 99, 96-106. Meaux, J. B., Hester, C., Smith, B., & Shoptaw, A. (2006). Stimulant medications: A trade-off? The lived experience of adolescents with ADHD. Journal for Specialists in Pediatric Nursing, 11, 214-226. Meszaros, A., Czobor, P., Balint, S., Komlosi, S., Simon, V., & Bitter, I. (2009). Pharmacotherapy of adult attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): A meta-analysis. International Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology, 12, 1137-1147. Naylor, L. J., & Pillow, D. R. (2007). College student reports of medication trade-offs and end-points. University of Texas at San Antonio, San Antonio, Texas Unpublished data. Norvilitis, J. M., Sun, L., & Zhang, J. (2010). ADHD symptomatology and adjustment to college in China and the United States. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 43, 86-94. Rabiner, D. L., Anastopoulos, A. D., Costello, E. J., Hoyle, R. H., McCabe, S. E., & Swartzwelder, H. S. (2009a). The misuse and diversion of prescribed ADHD medications by college students. Journal of Attention Disorders, 13, 144-153. Rabiner, D. L., Anastopoulos, A. D., Costello, E. J., Hoyle, R. H., McCabe, S. E., & Swartzwelder, H. S. (2009b). Motives and perceived consequences of nonmedical ADHD medication use by college students. Journal of Attention Disorders, 13, 259-270. Downloaded from jad.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on May 18, 2016 257 Pillow et al. Reaser, A., Prevatt, F., Petscher, Y., & Proctor, B. (2007). The learning and study strategies of college students with ADHD. Psychology in the Schools, 44, 627-638. Schuz, B., Marx, C., Wurm, S., Warner, L. M., Ziegelmann, J. P., Schwarzer, R., & Tesch-Romer, C. (2011). Medication beliefs predict medication adherence in older adults with multiple illnesses. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 70, 179-187. Shaw-Zirt, B., Popali-Lehane, L., Chaplin, W., & Bergman, A. (2005). Adjustment, social skills, and self-esteem in college students with symptoms of ADHD. Journal of Attention Disorders, 8, 109-120. Sheehan, D. V., Lecrubier, Y., Harnett-Sheehan, K., Janavs, J., Weiller, E., Keskiner, A., & Dunbar, G. C. (1997). The validity of the Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview (MINI) according to the SCID-P and its reliability. European Psychiatry, 12, 232-241. Sheehan, D. V., Lecrubier, Y., Sheehan, K. H., Amorim, P., Janavs, J., Weiller, E., & Dunbar, G. C. (1998). The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I.): The development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 59, 22-33. Yardley, L., & Donovan-Hall, M. (2007). Predicting adherence to exercise-based therapy in rehabilitation. Rehabilitation Psychology, 52, 56-64. Author Biographies David R. Pillow is an associate professor at the University of Texas at San Antonio. He trained as a social/quantitative psychologist at Arizona State University and subsequently completed postdoctoral work on the self-perceptions of ADHD children at the Western Psychiatric Institute and Clinic. He conducts research in the interface of social and clinical psychology, focusing broadly on issues of self-evaluation and identity negotiation. One line of his current work focuses on the self-perceptions of college students with ADHD; a second line focuses on belongingness/rejection. He teaches psychological statistics at the undergraduate and graduate levels. He enjoys traveling with his wife and children, visiting wineries, and long weekend walks with his dog. Lavelda J. Naylor graduated summa cum laude from the University of Texas at San Antonio and has a BA in psychology. Her honor’s thesis generated part of the data reported in this article under the direction of David Pillow. She is pursuing a master’s of arts in marriage and family therapy at St. Mary’s University in San Antonio and enjoys spending time with her family. For contact information, publications, and current research interests, visit laveldanaylor.wordpress.com Glenn P. Malone recently completed his master’s degree in psychology at the University of Texas at San Antonio and has primary interests in positive psychology. His thesis included the construction of a general belongingness scale and testing the belongingness hypothesis. In addition to his master’s degree, he has a bachelor of science in education from Texas State University. He is currently pursuing a career in research. He enjoys sports and vacationing. Downloaded from jad.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on May 18, 2016
© Copyright 2025 Paperzz