Filial Piety and Psychosocial Adjustment in Hong Kong Chinese Early Adolescents Journal of Early Adolescence 30(5) 651–667 © The Author(s) 2010 Reprints and permission: sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav DOI: 10.1177/0272431609341046 http://jea.sagepub.com Angel Nga-man Leung1, Stephanie Siu-fong Wong1, Iris Wai-yin Wong1, and Catherine McBride-Chang1 Abstract Is the Confucian concept of filial piety relevant for understanding contemporary Chinese children’s psychological well-being? This study of 231 Hong Kong Chinese fifth and sixth graders demonstrated that parental warmth and two facets of children’s filial piety belief were uniquely associated with life satisfaction, self-esteem, and social competence. Following the dual filial piety model, results distinguished children’s reciprocal and authoritarian filial piety beliefs. In particular, there were significant positive associations of reciprocal filial piety with life satisfaction and social competence and significant negative associations of authoritarian reciprocal filial piety with self-esteem and social competence, even statistically controlling for children’s ages, grade levels, and perceived parental warmth. Results suggested that children’s filial piety belief is a theoretically important aspect of Chinese values and beliefs that is uniquely associated with a variety of psychosocial adjustment variables and should be explored cross-culturally. 1 Chinese University of Hong Kong Corresponding Author: Catherine McBride-Chang, Department of Psychology, Chinese University of Hong Kong, Shatin, N.T., Hong Kong Email: [email protected] Downloaded from jea.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on September 12, 2016 652 Journal of Early Adolescence 30(5) Keywords family, parenting, parent-adolescent relationships, self-esteem/self-worth, social competence Filial piety (xiao) prescribes the way children behave toward their parents (Yeh & Bedford, 2003); it is a central Confucian concept within Chinese family systems (Chao, 1994). However, it has rarely been empirically explored in relation to children’s psychosocial adjustment beyond family functioning itself. Based on the dual filial piety model (Yeh & Bedford, 2004), the present study focused on two focal dimensions of filial piety belief, namely, reciprocal filial piety and authoritarian filial piety, in relation to life satisfaction, self-esteem, and perceived social competence, all of which have been consistently positively linked in previous research to perceived parental warmth. In Chinese cultures, the concept of filial piety is crucial to Chinese family systems because it prescribes how Chinese children should behave toward their parents. Given the overall importance of family relationships to psychological adjustment in children and adolescents, it is both theoretically and practically important to study the relationship between children’s filial piety and their perceived psychosocial adjustment. Filial piety traditionally involves devotion, love, respect, and obedience for one’s parents, including preserving family honor, avoiding family disgrace, continuing the family line, and showing care for them financially, emotionally, and physically (e.g., Ho, 1994). Under the influence of Confucianism, Chinese families have emphasized its importance for many generations. Indeed, it is a pillar of parent-child relationships. There is a Confucian Chinese saying that perhaps best captures the importance of filial piety: “bai shan xiao wei xian.” That is, of a hundred good characteristics a person might possess, filial piety is the most important. Despite the importance of filial piety in Chinese families, however, past research on the association between filial piety beliefs and psychological development has yielded mixed results (Yeh & Bedford, 2003, 2004). Some studies have found that filial piety beliefs are positively related to various family-related variables, including family cohesion (Cheung, Lee, & Chan, 1994), positive intergenerational relationships (Lawrence, Bennett, & Markides, 1992; Sung, 1995), motivation to care for parents (Selig, Thomlinson, & Hickey, 1991), and positive attachment to, support for, and harmony with one’s parents (Sung, 1995). In addition, filial piety beliefs tend to be negatively related to parent-child conflict among adolescents (Yeh & Bedford, 2004) and positively related to happiness in adults (Liu, Ng, Westherall, & Loong, 2000) and life satisfaction in fifth graders Downloaded from jea.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on September 12, 2016 Leung et al. 653 (Wong, 2004). On the other hand, however, filial piety has also been shown to be positively correlated with authoritarian moralism and cognitive conservatism in children (Ho, 1996). Ho (1994) further demonstrated that greater endorsement of filial piety values in children was associated with poorer verbal fluency; a passive, uncritical, and uncreative learning orientation; and fatalistic, superstitious, and stereotypic beliefs. To understand these mixed results in the association between filial piety and psychological outcomes, Yeh (1997, 2003) developed a dual filial piety model to integrate positive and negative influences of filial piety. He identified two focal filial piety factors, reciprocity and authoritarianism. The essence of reciprocal filial piety focuses on children providing emotional, physical, and financial support to parents in gratitude for parents’ devotion in having raised them. Apart from the importance of familial reciprocity, this factor is also based on the Confucian principle of favoring the intimate (Yeh, 2003) as well as the principle of reciprocity. Favoring the intimate refers to individuals’ tendencies to show greater care toward people who are closest within their social networks, according to which children and parents are among the most intimate psychologically (Yeh, 2003). The principle of reciprocity implies that children should repay their parents and ancestors by honoring them, since they owe their parents all the nurturance, comfort, and aid received throughout life. The second factor, authoritarian filial piety, is based on the Confucian principle of respecting the superior (Yeh, 2003). Because parents are at the top level of the family hierarchy, children are morally required to obey them. Therefore, this aspect of filial piety demands children’s repression of their own desires, submission to parental will, spirit of furthering parents’ reputations, and fulfillment of family responsibilities, for example, continuing the family line by giving birth to sons (Yang, Yeh, & Huang, 1989; Yeh, 2003). Thus, theoretically, authoritarian filial piety, emphasizing hierarchy and submission, may be associated with some negative aspects of adjustment in children (Yeh & Bedford, 2003). Reciprocal and authoritarian filial piety, however, are not mutually excusive. They are two different dimensions of filial piety that coexist and are interdependent, but they have different underlying meanings. In a test of the validity of these two dimensions, Yeh and Bedford (2003) found that reciprocal and authoritarian filial piety are positively correlated with one another. Among Chinese high school and college students (Yeh & Bedford, 2003), it was found that reciprocal filial piety was positively related to openness, agreeableness, extroversion, and conscientiousness but negatively related to neuroticism. In contrast, in the same study, authoritarian filial piety was positively related to neuroticism and conscientiousness but negatively related to Downloaded from jea.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on September 12, 2016 654 Journal of Early Adolescence 30(5) openness and extroversion. Yeh and Bedford (2003) also found that reciprocal filial piety was positively related to modernity attitudes, such as egalitarianism, openness, gender quality, and affection-centrism, while authoritarian filial piety was positively related to traditionalist attitudes, such as male dominance and submission to authority. Finally, these researchers demonstrated that reciprocal filial piety beliefs were positively related to empathy, perspective taking, and self-disclosure, while authoritarian filial piety was negatively related to perspective taking. Overall, then, theoretically, this study demonstrated that beliefs in reciprocal filial piety may be positively related to children’s psychosocial adjustment, while authoritarian filial piety may be negatively related to children’s psychosocial adjustment. Apart from filial piety, children’s perceived parental warmth has consistently been shown to have salient beneficial effects for children’s psychological development. Children’s perceived parental warmth reflects children’s perceptions of parents’ psychological acceptance of them (Cournoyer, 2000). High levels of children’s perceived warmth are correlated with children’s abilities to overcome challenges (Peterson & Leigh, 1990) and are related to children’s emotional adjustment (e.g., Gray & Steinberg, 1999), social and academic achievement (Chen, Liu, & Li, 2000), and family harmony (Lau, Lew, Hau, Cheung, & Berndt, 1990). Several studies have found that positive parent-child relationships are consistently strong correlates of life satisfaction of youth in Hong Kong (e.g., Leung & Zhang, 2000; Leung, McBride-Chang, & Lai, 2004). Warm, supportive relationships with parents enhance secure parent-child attachments, which are associated with a child’s social competence and feelings of self-worth (Laible & Thompson, 2000). Perceived parental warmth is also linked to positive adjustment across cultures (Kağitçibaşi, 2007). Despite the important philosophical underpinnings of the concept of filial piety involving both reciprocal and authoritarian processes, there remains little empirical evidence for these. Previous studies of filial piety have focused on parental attitudes, intergenerational relationships, and parent-child conflict and their effects on children’s cognitive development and personality characteristics. However, few studies of filial piety have examined it in relation to early adolescents’ psychosocial adjustment. At the same time, however, many studies have demonstrated the importance of parental warmth to children’s psychosocial development. The present study, thus, aimed to explore how the model of reciprocal and authoritarian filial piety (Yeh & Bedford, 2003) might facilitate understanding of filial piety as a culturally important dimension of values and beliefs in relation to general measures of early adolescents’ adjustment. With increasing calls for the concept of filial piety to be understood within a modern context (e.g., Chao & Tseng, 2002; Mehta & Downloaded from jea.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on September 12, 2016 Leung et al. 655 Ko, 2004), a distinction between reciprocal and authoritarian aspects of filial piety was expected to help elucidate its importance for specific psychosocial outcomes, beyond parental warmth. These psychosocial outcomes included perceived life satisfaction, selfesteem, and social competence, all markers of positive youth development, in the present study. Parental warmth was expected to be positively related to each of these, as shown in previous research (e.g., Gray & Steinberg, 1999; Kağitçibaşi, 2007; Leung et al., 2004). Life satisfaction is a multifaceted construct referring to an individual’s overall evaluation of life domains, such as health, finances, and interpersonal relationships (Michalos, 1991), whereas self-esteem encompasses one’s sense of personal worth, self-approval, and self-respect. Previous work on Chinese parenting values and beliefs has demonstrated that guan, or training (e.g., Chao, 1994; Stewart et al., 1998), an important facet of filial piety, was positively associated with both life satisfaction and self-esteem in Hong Kong Chinese adolescent girls. In contrast, these researchers (Stewart et al., 1998) found that restrictive control was negatively associated with both self-esteem and perceptions of well-being. Thus, adopting the dual filial piety model, we hypothesized that, beyond warmth, children’s reciprocal filial belief would positively explain self-esteem and life satisfaction, while authoritarian filial piety would be negatively associated with each. Social competence primarily reflects one’s peer acceptance, for example, having lots of friends, being easy to like, and doing things with other children (Harter, 1982). To the best of our knowledge, previous research has not explored the role of filial piety for social competence with peers per se. However, drawing from previous work both on general psychosocial adjustment (e.g., Stewart et al., 1998) and interpersonal relationships within the family (e.g., Lawrence et al., 1992; Sung, 1995; Yeh & Bedford, 2004), which demonstrated that general filial piety tends to be positively associated with social relationships, we hypothesized that reciprocal and authoritarian filial piety would have different associations with social competence as well. Specifically, reciprocal filial piety, with its focus on mutual respect within a family relationship, was expected to be associated with general social competence because the spirit of mutual respect in a relationship might logically extend from within the parent-child relationship to other relationships as well. In contrast, authoritarian filial piety was hypothesized to be negatively associated with social competence because of its overall emphasis on submitting to another’s will and obeying. These ideas, perhaps natural in parent-child relationships, might not fit well within a peer relationship context, in which status should presumably be considered similar across peer dyads. Downloaded from jea.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on September 12, 2016 656 Journal of Early Adolescence 30(5) Method Participants A total of 231 Hong Kong Chinese students in Grades 5 and 6 (125 boys; 104 girls) participated in the present study. Their ages ranged from 9 to 13, with an average age of 10.68 (SD = .75) years. These grade levels were chosen because previous studies have suggested that children around this developmental level tend to describe their parents as the most important people in their lives (e.g. Furman & Buhrmester, 1992). They depend on their parents for affection, advice, enhancement of self-worth, and assistance with daily problems. Thus, different facets of parent-child relationships were considered to be particularly salient for understanding the psychosocial development of children in this early adolescent age range. Measures Participants completed all questionnaires in Chinese. All questionnaires apart from the Filial Piety Scale, already in Chinese, were back translated from English to Chinese. As shown in Table 1, the internal consistency reliabilities of all the scales were satisfactory. Scales administered included the following: Filial Piety Scale. This scale (Yeh & Bedford, 2003) consists of 16 items, 8 measuring reciprocal filial piety and the other 8 measuring authoritarian filial piety. The scale was developed in Taiwan, and it was used in a college population in Taiwan (Yeh & Bedford, 2003) and a junior and high school students sample in Taiwan (Yeh & Bedford, 2004), all with satisfactory reliabilities and validity. Participants responded to each item of this scale on a 5-point Likert scale from strongly disagree (1) to strongly agree (5). Sample items from the reciprocal filial piety subscale are “Be grateful to your parents for raising you,” and “Take care of your parents when they are old.” Sample items from the authoritarian filial piety scale include “Live with your parents even after you grow up and get married” and “Listen to parents’ advice on decisions about a future career.” Perceived parental warmth. Greenberger and Chen’s (1996) Perceived Parental Warmth Scale was used. It probed participants’ perceived relationships with their parents on a 6-point Likert scale from strongly disagree (1) to strong agree (6). A sample item is “My parent enjoys spending time with me.” Perceived life satisfaction. The Multi-dimensional Students’ Life Satisfaction Scale (MSLSS) measured participants’ subjective well-being (Huebner, 1994) specifically in children. It consists of 32 items measuring children’s satisfaction in four domains: family (9 items), friends (8 items), school (8 items), Downloaded from jea.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on September 12, 2016 657 Leung et al. Table 1. Means, Standard Deviations, and Internal Consistencies of All Measured Variables and Correlations Among Filial Piety, Warmth, Self-Esteem, Life Satisfaction, Social Competence, Gender, Grade, and Age — α 1. Reciprocal filial piety 2. Authoritarian filial piety 3. Self-esteem 4. Life satisfaction 5. Social competence 6. Warmth 7. Gender 8. Grade 9. Age .86 4.34 (0.62) — .74 3.38 (0.67) .57** — .81 2.74 (0.55) .92 2.93 (0.48) .32** .52** .16** .37** — .63** — .68 2.93 (0.54) .28** .09 .45** X (SD) 1 2 3 4 .53** 5 6 7 8 9 — .79 4.18 (1.06) .60** .51** .46** .74** .35** — — — .15* -.03 .05 .01 .04 .03 — — — -.17** -.27** -.13* -.30** -.10 -.25** -.09 — — 10.68 (0.75) -.09 -.17* -.08 -.23** .02 -.18** -.02 .68** — **p < .01.*p < .05. and self (7 items). A 4-point scale was used to measure the frequency with which various experiences are perceived to occur, from 1 indicating never to 4 indicating always. For example, a sample item on the family domain subscale is “I enjoy being at home with my family,” and a sample item from the friend domain is “My friends treat me very well.” A sample item from the school subscale is “I look forward to going to school,” and a sample item from the self domain is “I like myself.” The four subscales are significantly and moderately to strongly correlated with one another. In the present study, for example, the family subscale was significantly correlated with the friend (r = .42, p < .01), school (r = .46, p < .01), and self subscales (r = .61, p < .01). The friend subscale was also positively related to school (r = .23, p < .01) and self (r = .51). The school subscale was relatively strongly related to self as well (r = .51, p < .01). In view of the moderate to strong associations among subscales, a composite score of the four was used across all analyses to represent students’ multidimensional life satisfaction. Self-esteem. The 10-item Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (Rosenberg, 1965) measured participants’ global personal self-esteem on a 4-point scale from 1, strongly disagree to 4 strongly agree. A sample item is “On the whole, I am satisfied with myself.” Perceived social competence. The seven-item Social Competence subscale of the Perceived Competence Scale for Children (Harter, 1982) assessed participants’ self-perceived social competence. Participants chose the Downloaded from jea.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on September 12, 2016 658 Journal of Early Adolescence 30(5) description of the type of child he or she found to be most like him/her and then judged whether he or she was “sort of like” or “really like” that type of child. Items were thus scored on a 4-point scale from not very competent (1) to very competent (4). Examples of the items are “Some children find it hard to make friends, but for others it is pretty easy,” and “Some children have a lot of friends.” This scale has satisfactory psychometric properties with samples of Chinese children (Stigler, Smith, & Mao, 1985). Results Table 1 shows the means, standard deviations, and correlations of the measured variables. A significant difference between fifth and sixth graders for both reciprocal filial piety (t = 2.62, p < .01) and authoritarian filial piety scores (t = 4.18, p < .01) was indicated by t test analyses, with sixth graders scoring significantly lower on both. In addition, girls had higher reciprocal filial piety than boys did (t = –2.26, p < .05); no other gender differences were found. From the first-order correlational analysis, it was found that there was a stronger correlation between the outcome variables and reciprocal filial piety (RFP) than for those between the outcome variables and authoritarian filial piety (AFP). Both AFP and RFP were significantly positively associated with one another, with warmth, and with all psychosocial variables included. Nevertheless, our main interest was to examine the extent to which filial piety would be uniquely associated with children’s psychological adjustment beyond warmth. To begin with, we hypothesized that children’s reciprocal filial belief would positively explain self-esteem, life satisfaction, and social competence, while authoritarian filial piety would be negatively associated with each. Therefore, to examine the unique contribution of both AFP and RFP beyond children’s perceived parental warmth to all three psychosocial variables, hierarchical regressions were carried out. Students’ grade level and age were included in the first step of the hierarchical regressions, while perceived parental warmth was included in the second step. Such an order of entry was done based both on logic and theory. Grade and age were correlated with the dependent variables. They were entered first in order to control these general demographic effects, allowing a clearer examination of main effects (Cohen & Cohen, 1983). Warmth was then included in the second step, followed by the two measures of filial piety, so as to demonstrate the unique contributions of the filial piety variables beyond warmth, a parenting variable that is well established in the literature cross-culturally for its importance for children’s psychosocial adjustment (Kağitçibaşi, 2007; Laible & Thompson, 2000). Downloaded from jea.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on September 12, 2016 Leung et al. 659 To examine the relative importance and directions of AFP and RFP (i.e., the positive and negative associations of AFP and RFP on psychosocial outcomes), the regression analyses were run twice, once with AFP included in Step 3 and RFP included in Step 4 and again when the entry of these two variables into the equation was reversed. This procedure was carried out so that the unique variance explained by each dimension of filial piety could be established, given that previous studies had not explicitly compared the relative strength of each dimension for outcome measures. Cohen, Cohen, West, and Alken (2003) suggested that in instances in which two sets of variables may have different predictive values for the dependent variables but there is a lack of theory on which set should be entered first into a hierarchical regression, then both sets can be entered in different sequences in order to report them both; thus, our entries of AFP and RFP were done following this advice. The results of these regression equations are shown in Table 2. In these analyses, as expected, parental warmth explained significant variance in selfesteem, life satisfaction, and social competence. Regardless of the order in which it was entered in the equation, reciprocal filial piety explained life satisfaction and social competence positively in children. In contrast, authoritarian filial piety was significantly negatively associated with self-esteem and social competence with all other variables statistically controlled. With grade, age, warmth, and AFP controlled, RFP uniquely and positively explained 2% unique variance in life satisfaction and 3% of unique variance in social competence, while it was positively but not significantly associated with selfesteem. In contrast, with grade, age, warmth, and RFP controlled, AFP uniquely and negatively explained 2% of the variance each in self-esteem and social competence, respectively, while it was negatively but not significantly associated with life satisfaction in the equation. By testing the associations of RFP and AFP to psychosocial variables in Steps 3 and 4, respectively, we demonstrated the unique importance of RFP for life satisfaction and social competence and the unique importance of AFP for self-esteem and social competence, independent of the other measure of filial piety. At the same time, despite the fact that in the first-order correlations as presented in Table 1, AFP was positively (albeit relatively weakly) associated with self-esteem, life satisfaction, and social competence, this measure was negatively associated with each in the regression model once all other variables were statistically controlled. To understand this seemingly contradictory result, and to understand the mechanism by which these variables interacted, we then used partial correlations to specifically examine correlations of AFP with each of the psychosocial outcome measures with either Downloaded from jea.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on September 12, 2016 660 Downloaded from jea.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on September 12, 2016 Self-Esteem Grade .02 .02 Age Warmth .20 .18** AFP .21 .01 RFP .22 .01 RFP .21 .00 AFP .22 .02* -.09 (–1.02) .10 .10** .04 (0.52) .04 (5.48**) .55 .45** -.17 (-2.10*) .55 .00 .13 (1.58) .57 .02** .13 (1.58) .56 .01** -.17 (-2.10*) .57 .00 Beta (t Value) R2 R2 Change Beta (t Value) R2 R2 Change Beta (t Value) -.15 (-1.63) .19 (2.16*) .29 (3.43**) -.17 (-2.06*) .22 (2.43*) .22 (2.43*) -.17 (-2.01*) Social Competence -.10 (-1.54) .03 .03 -.05 (-0.86) .63 (10.18**) .14 .11** -.09 (-1.41) .15 .01 .19 (2.93**) .17 .03* .19 (2.93**) .15 .01 -.09 (-1.41) .17 .02* Life Satisfaction Measures of Psychological Well-Being Note: Standardized beta weights and t values in the final regression model are shown above. AFP = authoritarian filial piety; RFP = reciprocal filial piety. **p < .01. *p < .05. Step 1 Step 2 Step 3 Step 4 Step 3 Step 4 R2 R2 Change Table 2. Hierarchical Regression Analysis for Variables Predicting Self-Esteem, Life Satisfaction, and Social Competence Leung et al. 661 warmth or both warmth and RFP statistically controlled. With warmth only controlled, AFP was negatively, though nonsignificantly, associated with all three outcome variables (range of r from –.01 to –.10). With both warmth and RFP statistically controlled, correlations ranged from –.08 (life satisfaction) to –.15 (for social competence); only the association with social competence was significant. These results suggest some degree of suppression. Both warmth and RFP were generally more strongly correlated with all measures of psychological adjustment than was AFP. The AFP was also relatively strongly correlated with both warmth and RFP (rs for both >.50). Therefore, regression results likely represented the unique and less positive aspects of AFP. In other words, with RFP and warmth statistically controlled, AFP in fact negatively explained early adolescents’ psychological adjustment. Discussion Findings from the present study suggest that filial piety may be a unique dimension to consider in future studies of parenting in Chinese families. Despite a strong association between children’s perceived parental warmth and their psychosocial adjustment, different aspects of filial piety were nevertheless uniquely associated with several psychosocial outcomes in upper primary school Chinese children, beyond parental warmth, an established universal parenting style variable that is important to children’s development (Chen et al., 2000; Kağitçibaşi, 2007; Lau et al., 1990; Peterson & Leigh, 1990). The present study supported the dual filial piety model by demonstrating that reciprocal filial piety had positive associations with both children’s life satisfaction and social competence, while authoritarian filial piety had negative associations with children’s self-esteem and social competence. Children with high reciprocal filial beliefs are likely to be more motivated to support and care for their parents in appreciation for their parents’ efforts in bringing them up. This motivation might help them build and maintain good parent-child relationships, promoting children’s satisfaction within the family. Children’s reciprocal filial beliefs also significantly and positively explained perceived peer acceptance in the present study. It is likely that children generalize their positive affective social orientation acquired from parents to other social interactions (Putallaz & Heflin, 1987). Thus, children high in reciprocal filial beliefs might tend to reciprocate in social interactions more often, promoting interpersonal skills and relationships. In contrast, authoritarian filial piety emphasizes children’s submission to hierarchical authority and oppression of self-autonomy. Children who are high in authoritarian filial piety have been socialized to respect but never Downloaded from jea.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on September 12, 2016 662 Journal of Early Adolescence 30(5) question authority. This characteristic of authoritarian filial piety may foster suppression of children’s own will, perhaps resulting in feelings of relative incompetence, helplessness, or frustration. This is one plausible explanation for the fact that authoritarian filial piety negatively explained children’s selfesteem and perceived social competence in the present study. Indeed, some previous studies have also demonstrated negative associations of filial piety, not only with personality variables (Yeh & Bedford, 2003) but also with cognitive skills. For example, measures of filial piety have been positively related to cognitive conservatism in children and used to explain parents’ negligence or inhibition of children’s opinions, creativity, independence, and all-round personal development (Ho, 1996). Correspondingly, Boey (1976) found that filial piety beliefs may be harmful to children’s cognitive development. At the same time, authoritarian filial piety has been found to have some beneficial effects on family relationships as well. For example, such beliefs are credited with reducing parent-child conflict in Chinese families (Yeh & Bedford, 2004). Young adolescents were the participants in the present study because their beliefs in filial piety and perception of parental warmth may be particularly important for their psychological adjustment given that family is a core part of their everyday lives. It is possible that these patterns might change among older adolescents, who may have more conflicts and arguments with their parents. Given these intriguing findings, more empirical research on the nuances of filial piety in relation to children’s adjustment, perhaps among different age ranges, is clearly warranted. The present study was limited in its findings by our assessments of children’s psychological well-being, perceived filial piety, and maternal warmth based only on self-report measures. Children’s self-reports of parenting are relatively common and have been used by a number of past studies (e.g., Pomerantz, 2001; Sessa, Avenevoli, Steinberg, & Morris, 2001) that have suggested that children’s ratings were similar to those of parents and others. After all, children’s own subjective evaluation of parental warmth and perceived level of filial piety may be especially important for their own psychological adjustment. Yet, using a single informant approach may still limit our interpretation of the data because we know nothing about parents’ perceptions and behaviors from them. Thus, in future studies of filial piety, parents’ reports or observations from others should also be included. Furthermore, future research could also extend the investigation of different aspects of filial piety by looking not only at their main effects, but also at the extent to which they moderate parent-child relationships. For example, Wong, Leung, and McBride-Chang (in press) found that filial piety moderated Downloaded from jea.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on September 12, 2016 Leung et al. 663 the association between adolescents’ perceived maternal control and both perceived support and conflicts in mother-child relationships. However, in that study, no distinction was made across different aspects of filial piety. Thus, the respective moderating roles of authoritarian and reciprocal filial piety could be included in future work. Despite these limitations, however, the importance of this research lies in testing age-old philosophical notions of social interactions empirically. This research demonstrates that filial piety is potentially a multifaceted and empirically testable construct. Differences in associations of outcome variables with reciprocal and authoritarian filial piety suggest that the notion of filial piety requires careful attention to possible multiple dimensions of it (Yeh & Bedford, 2004). Our findings also highlight the potential practical and theoretical importance of filial piety for Chinese children beyond parental warmth for explaining psychological adjustment. Certain aspects of filial piety, such as felt gratitude and responsibility to family, may be particularly helpful for students’ overall development, whereas other aspects, such as suppression of one’s own desires, might have more mixed effects, such as decreasing selfesteem or even social competence. Although our results are preliminary, they confirm the ideas put forward by Yeh and Bedford (2004) that filial piety is an important psychological construct for understanding children’s adjustment. The problems of incorporating Western notions of parent-child relationship into understanding Chinese family dynamics have been well argued (e.g., Chao, 1994, 2000) and continue to generate controversy (e.g., Chen et al., 2000). The study presented here provides another way in which to conceptualize interactions within Chinese families under the notion of filial piety. As a traditional Confucian concept, filial piety has existed for thousands of years in Chinese families. It has been regarded as a fundamental belief that children should understand and endorse. Nevertheless, belief in the importance of authoritarian filial piety has diminished, while reciprocal filial piety has gained support in contemporary Chinese society (Yeh & Bedford, 2003). Hwang (1999) proposed that the decreasing popularity of authoritarian filial piety is attributable to modernization and exposure to Western influence. The concept of filial piety is also a Western one, dating back at least to ancient Greece and the dilemmas of the treatments of fathers and sons. However, this concept is particularly salient and dominant in Asian cultures, so perhaps it is fitting that most studies examining this concept have tested it, thus far, on Asian children, adolescents, and adults. We hope that the present study goes one more step toward empirically testing the importance of filial piety in relation to other family relations variables, ultimately across cultures. Downloaded from jea.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on September 12, 2016 664 Journal of Early Adolescence 30(5) Declaration of Conflicting Interests The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interests with respect to the authorship and/or publication of this article. Funding The author(s) received no financial support for the research and/or authorship of this article. References Boey, K. W. (1976). Rigidity and cognitive complexity: An empirical investigation in the interpersonal, physical, and numeric domains under task-oriented and egooriented conditions. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Hong Kong, People’s Republic of China. Chao, R. K. (1994). Beyond parental control and authoritarian parenting style: Understanding Chinese parenting through the cultural notion of training. Child Development, 65, 1111-1119. Chao, R. K. (2000). The parenting of immigrant Chinese and European American mothers: Relations between parenting styles, socialization goals, and parental practices. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 21, 233-248. Chao, R., & Tseng, V. (2002). Parenting of Asians. In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of parenting (Vol. 4): Social conditions and applied parenting (2nd ed., pp. 59-93). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Chen, X., Liu, M., & Li, D. (2000). Parental warmth, control, and indulgence and their relations to adjustment in Chinese children: A longitudinal study. Journal of Family Psychology, 14, 401-419. Cheung, C. K., Lee, J. J., & Chan, C. M. (1994). Explicating filial piety in relation to family cohesion. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 9, 565-580. Cohen, J., & Cohen, P. (1983). Applied multiple regression/correlation analysis for the behavioural sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Cohen, J., Cohen, P., West, S. G., & Alken, L. S. (2003). Applied multiple regression/ correlation analysis for the behavioral sciences. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Cournoyer, D. E. (2000). Universalist research: Examples drawn from the methods and findings of parental acceptance/rejection theory. In A. L. Comunian & U. P. Gielen (Eds.), International perspectives on human development (pp. 213-232). Lengerich: Pabst Science. Furman, W., & Buhrmester D. (1992). Age and sex differences in perceptions of networks of personal relationships. Child Development, 63, 103-105. Gray, M. R., & Steinberg, L. (1999). Unpacking authoritative parenting: Reassessing a multidimensional construct. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 61, 574-587. Downloaded from jea.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on September 12, 2016 Leung et al. 665 Greenberger, E., & Chen, C. (1996). Perceived family relationships and depressed mood in early and late adolescence: A comparison of European and Asian Americans. Developmental Psychology, 32, 707-716. Harter, S. (1982). The perceived competence scale for children. Child Development, 53, 87-97. Ho, D. Y. F. (1994). Filial piety, authoritarian moralism, and cognitive conservatism in Chinese society. Genetic, Social and General Psychology Monograph, 129, 349-365. Ho, D. Y. F. (1996). Filial piety and its psychological consequences. In M. H. Bond (Ed.), The handbook of Chinese psychology (pp. 155-165). Hong Kong, People’s Republic of China: Oxford University Press. Huebner, E. S. (1994). Preliminary development and validation of a multi-dimensional life scale for children. Psychological Assessment, 6, 149-158. Hwang, K. K. (1999). Filial piety and loyalty: Two types of social identification in Confucianism. Asian Journal of Psychology, 2, 129-149. Kağitçibaşi, C. (2007). Family, self, and human development across cultures: Theory and applications (2nd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Laible, D. J., & Thomspon, R. A. (2000). Mother-child discourse, attachment security, shared positive affect, and early conscience development. Child Development, 71, 1424-1440. Lau, S., Lew, W. J. F., Hau, K. T., Cheung, P. C., & Berndt, T. J. (1990). Relations among perceived parental control, warmth, indulgence, and the family harmony of Chinese in mainland China. Developmental Psychology, 26, 674-677. Lawrence, R. H., Bennett, J. M., & Markides, K. S. (1992). Perceived intergenerational solidarity and psychological distress among older Mexican-Americans. Journal of Gerontology, 47, S55-S65. Leung, C. Y.-W., McBride-Chang, C., Lai, B. P.-Y. (2004). Relations among maternal parenting style, academic competence, and life satisfaction in Chinese early adolescents. Journal of Early Adolescence, 24, 113-143. Leung, J. P., & Zhang, L. W. (2000). Modelling life satisfaction of Chinese adolescents in Hong Kong. International Journal of Behavioural Development, 24(1), 99-104. Liu, J. H., Ng, S. H., Westherall, A., & Loong, C. (2000). Filial piety, acculturation, and intergenerational communication among New Zealand Chinese. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 22, 213-223. Mehta, K. K., & Ko, H. (2004). Filial piety revisited in the context of modernizing Asian societies. Geriatrics and Gerontology International, 4, S77-S78. Michalos, A. C. (1991). Global report on student well-being. New York: Springer-Verlag. Peterson, G. W., & Leigh, G. K. (1990). The family and social competence in adolescence. In G. R. Adams & T. P. Gullotta (Eds.), Developing social competency in adolescence (pp. 97-138). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Downloaded from jea.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on September 12, 2016 666 Journal of Early Adolescence 30(5) Pomerantz, E. M. (2001). Parent × child socialization: Implications for the development of depressive symptoms. Journal of Family Psychology, 15, 510-525. Putallaz, M., & Heflin, A. H. (1987). Toward a model of peer acceptance. In J. N. Gottman & J. G. Parker (Eds.), Conversations of friends: Speculations on affective development (pp. 292-314). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self-image. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. Selig, S., Tomlinson, T., & Hickey, T. (1991). Ethical dimensions of intergenerational reciprocity: Implications for practice. The Gerontologist, 31, 624-630. Sessa, F. M., Avenevoli, S., Steinberg, L., & Morris, A. S. (2001). Correspondence among informants on parenting: Preschool children, mothers, and observers. Journal of Family Psychology, 15, 53-68. Stewart, S. M., Rao, N., Bond, M. H., McBride-Chang, C., Fielding, R., & Kennard, B. D. (1998). Chinese dimensions of parenting: Broadening Western predictors and outcomes. International Journal of Psychology, 33, 345-358. Stigler, J. W., Smith, S., & Mao, L. (1985). The self-perception of competence by Chinese children. Child Development, 56, 1259-1270. Sung, K. T. (1995). Measures and dimensions of filial piety in Korea. The Gerontologist, 35, 240-247. Wong, I. W-Y. (2004). Intergenerational maternal warmth and concept of filial piety in relation to children’s academic achievement and life satisfaction in Hong Kong. Unpublished undergraduate thesis. Chinese University of Hong Kong, People’s Republic of China. Wong, S. M., Leung, A. N.-M., & McBride-Chang, C. (in press). Adolescent filial piety as a moderator between perceived maternal control and mother-adolescent relationship quality in Hong Kong. Social Development (in press). Yang, K. S., Yeh, K. H., & Huang, L. L. (1989). A social attitudinal analysis of Chinese filial piety: Conceptualization and assessment. Bulletin of the Institute of Ethnology Academia Sinica, 65, 171-227 (in Chinese). Yeh, K. H. (1997). Changes in the Taiwanese people’s concept of filial piety. In L. Y. Cheng, Y. H. Lu, & F. C. Wand (Eds.), Taiwanese society in the 1990s (pp. 171-214). Taipei, Republic of China: Institute of Sociology, Academia sinica (in Chinese). Yeh, K. H. (2003). The beneficial and harmful effects of filial piety: An integrative analysis. In K. S. Yang, K. K. Hwang, P. S. Pederson, & I. Daibo (Eds.), Progress in Asian social psychology: Conceptual and empirical contributions (pp. 67-82). Westport, CT: Greenwood. Yeh, K. H., & Bedford, O. (2003). Filial piety: A test of the dual filial piety model. Asian Journal of Social psychology, 6, 215-228. Yeh, K. H., & Bedford, O. (2004). Filial piety and parent-child conflict. International Journal of Psychology, 39, 132-144. Downloaded from jea.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on September 12, 2016 Leung et al. 667 Bios Angel Nga-man Leung is now a PhD candidate in psychology in the Chinese University of Hong Kong, specializing in developmental psychology. Her major research interests are parenting and the influences of online gaming on the social development of early adolescents. Stephanie Siu-fong Wong obtained her bachelor’s degree in social sciences from the University of Hong Kong. Iris Wai-yin Wong obtained her bachelor’s degree in social sciences from the University of Hong Kong. Catherine McBride-Chang is a professor in the Psychology Department of the Chinese University of Hong Kong specializing in developmental psychology. Her research interests are cognitive and social development in children, especially on parenting, reading and vocabulary development, and impairment in children. Downloaded from jea.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on September 12, 2016
© Copyright 2025 Paperzz