Original Research Article Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 2012;33:125–131 DOI: 10.1159/000337843 Accepted: February 29, 2012 Published online: April 21, 2012 A Retrospective Analysis of the Sentence Writing Component of the Mini Mental State Examination: Cognitive and Affective Aspects Yan Press a–e Natalia Velikiy a–c Alex Berzak a–e Howard Tandeter a–c Roni Peleg a–c Tamar Freud a–c Boris Punchik a–e Tzvi Dwolatzky b, d–f a Department of Family Medicine, b Faculty of Health Sciences, and c Siaal Research Center for Family Medicine and Primary Care, Faculty of Health Sciences, Ben-Gurion University of the Negev, dComprehensive Geriatric Assessment Unit, e Community Geriatric Service, Southern District, Clalit Health Services, and f Geriatric Department and Memory Clinic, Mental Health Center, Beer-Sheva, Israel Key Words Mini Mental State Examination ⴢ Screening ⴢ Cognitive impairment ⴢ Dementia ⴢ Depression ⴢ Dysgraphia ⴢ Language ⴢ Emotion ⴢ Aging Abstract Background: One of the components of the Mini Mental State Examination (MMSE) is the request to write a sentence. We investigated the relationship between the characteristics of the written sentence of the MMSE and the cognitive and affective status of elderly patients. Methods: The characteristics of the sentence were compared to the total MMSE score, sociodemographic characteristics, tests evaluating cognition and affective status, and diagnoses. Results: The number of words was significantly associated with the degree of cognitive impairment, whereas the emotional polarity of sentences and concerns about health were associated with depression. Conclusions: Characteristics of the MMSE sentence may provide important additional information regarding both cognition and affect when assessing older people. Copyright © 2012 S. Karger AG, Basel © 2012 S. Karger AG, Basel 1420–8008/12/0333–0125$38.00/0 Fax +41 61 306 12 34 E-Mail [email protected] www.karger.com Accessible online at: www.karger.com/dem Introduction The written word and language serves as a mirror to many aspects of human function and behavior. Handwriting is closely related to the functioning of the mind and is therefore linked with how people think, feel and behave [1]. Agraphia can be defined as the partial or total loss of the ability to produce written language associated with brain pathology [2]. Alzheimer’s disease (AD) is a neurodegenerative condition resulting in progressive cognitive and functional decline, and agraphia or dysgraphia are common features of this disease. As AD progresses there is a deterioration in spelling, poor narrative organization, content word errors and perseverations, reduced syntactic complexity, and shorter sentences [3, 4]. In a study of patients with AD, the ability to formulate a complete sentence was related to dementia severity [5]. When asked to write a story about a picture, patients with AD of mild to moderate severity wrote shorter descriptive paragraphs than normal subjects matched for age and gender (28.4 vs. 79.9 words) [6]. The Mini Mental State Examination (MMSE) [7] is widely used as a screening instrument for cognitive impairment. It assesses a number of cognitive domains, namely orientation, registration, concentration, shortterm memory, language and visual spatial function. The MMSE includes a request to ‘write a sentence’, with 1 Yan Press, MD Comprehensive Geriatric Assessment Unit, Clalit Health Services, Yassky Clinic 24 King David Street Beer-Sheva 84539 (Israel) Tel. +972 8640 7743, E-Mail yanp @ bgu.ac.il point awarded if the sentence has a subject and a verb and makes sense regardless of spelling, grammar or content. Depending on the setting the MMSE has been shown to have reasonable sensitivity and good specificity [7–12]. Not all components of the MMSE are equal as indicators of cognitive function [13–15], and writing a sentence is one of the components that contributes to the variance in global MMSE scores. In a retrospective review by McCarthy et al. [16] the sentences written by 280 patients from a geriatric day hospital performing the MMSE were analyzed. Significant correlation was detected between the overall MMSE score and the number of words. The mean MMSE score of subjects writing ‘positive’ sentences was marginally but significantly higher than that of those writing a ‘neutral’ or ‘negative’ sentence. In another study [17], the sentences written as a task of the MMSE were compared between cognitively healthy older subjects and those with dementia. Sentences with inadequate emotional content seemed to indicate a dementing illness. In this study we investigated the relationship between the characteristics of the written sentence of the MMSE and the cognitive and affective status of patients attending the Comprehensive Geriatric Assessment Unit (CGAU) of Clalit Health Services in the Southern District of Israel. Materials and Methods Participants and Instruments The CGAU was established in the Southern District of Israel by the Clalit Health Services in 2005. The staff of the CGAU includes geriatricians, a geriatric nurse, a social worker, an occupational therapist and a secretary. The average annual turnover of the CGAU is approximately 150 patients. Patients are referred to the CGAU for cognitive, functional and affective symptoms, as well as problems with mobility or falls, and social difficulties. As part of the multidisciplinary assessment, cognitive status is evaluated by means of the MMSE, and in patients with mild to moderate cognitive impairment further evaluation includes the Montreal Cognitive Assessment instrument [18], as well as computerized neuropsychological testing using the Mindstreams battery (Neurotrax, Newark, N.J., USA) [19]. Affective status is evaluated using the 15-item Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS-15) [20]. The diagnoses of dementia and depression are based on clinical findings according to the DSM-IV criteria [21], and that of mild cognitive impairment (MCI) is based on the criteria of the Expert Conference on Mild Cognitive Impairment [22]. Functional assessment includes the Barthel Index [23] for basic activities of daily living as well as the Older Americans Resources and Services scale for instrumental activities of daily living [24]. For measuring comorbidity we use two instruments. The first is 126 Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 2012;33:125–131 the Cumulative Illness Rating Scale-Geriatrics [25], which permits an estimate of illness burden and diversity on the basis of a fivepoint physician rating scale (scores of 0–4, the higher score indicating a greater degree of burden) reflecting the severity of pathology in each of 14 categories (maximum score 56). The second instrument used for assessing comorbidity is the Charlson’s Comorbidity Index [26]. Because this index was designed primarily as a predictor of mortality it presents a list of 19 conditions with fixed degrees of severity according to the relative risk of death. For risk calculated at 61.2 but !1.5, the disease receives a score of 1; for relative risk 61.5 but !2.5, a score of 2, and for relative risk 62.5 but !3, a score of 3; both a second metastatic solid tumor and HIVAIDS receive a score of 6. A total score is calculated. The age score represents an extra point for each decade above age 50 and is used for adjusting the Charlson’s Comorbidity Index for age. The total combined score is derived by adding the age score to the total score. Procedures and Characteristics of the Written Sentence Three independent investigators (N.V., Y.P. and A.B.), blinded to the results of geriatric assessment and all tests performed, were asked to assess the following characteristics of the written sentence of the MMSE: number of words in the sentence, emotional polarity (positive/neutral/negative) and concerns about health (yes/no). Initially, the investigators analyzed 30 MMSE written sentences to determine interrater reliability. The concordance between raters was an acceptable 100% for characteristic one, 92% for characteristic two and 96% for characteristic three. Subsequently, all analyses were performed by one investigator who speaks fluent Hebrew and Russian (N.V.). The Ethics Committee of the Meir Hospital approved the study. Statistical Analysis The characteristics of the sentence were compared to the total MMSE score, the language of administration, age, gender, education, tests evaluating cognition and affective status, and clinical diagnoses. Continuous variables are shown as means and standard deviations. Categorical variables are described as frequencies. t tests and 2 tests were used to analyze statistically significant differences of continuous and categorical variables, respectively. Two-tailed p ! 0.05 were considered statistically significant. Results Data of 344 patients evaluated in the CGAU during the years 2007–2009 were assessed. The basic characteristics of the study population are presented in table 1. Associations between MMSE Sentence Characteristics and the Language of Assessment A total of 209 (60.8%) were Russian speakers who underwent assessment in Russian; 135 (39.2%) were fluent in Hebrew and were assessed in Hebrew, although Hebrew was the native language of only 6 (1.7%). No differences were found between the number of words in the MMSE sentences of Hebrew and Russian speakers (3.9 8 1.3 vs. 3.9 8 1.2, p = 0.85) Press /Velikiy /Berzak /Tandater /Peleg / Freud /Punchik /Dwolatzky Impact of Age, Gender and Education on the MMSE Sentence Characteristics (table 2) Of the female patients, 41 (18.1%) expressed concern about their health in the MMSE sentence versus 11 (10.1%) of the male patients, p value = 0.04. No other associations between the characteristics of the MMSE sentence were found regarding gender, age and education. Associations between MMSE Sentence Characteristics and Cognitive and Affective Tests The association between the number of words in the MMSE sentences and the overall MMSE score did not reach statistical significance (Pearson’s correlation 0.103, p = 0.057). A statistically significant association was found between the number of words and GDS-15, i.e. patients with higher GDS-15 scores wrote shorter sentences (Pearson’s correlation –0.329, p = 0.03). With regard to the emotional polarity of MMSE sentences, patients writing ‘negative’ or ‘neutral’ sentences had higher GDS-15 scores (10.6 8 4.1 and 10.1 8 3.4, respectively) than those writing ‘positive’ (7.5 8 3.8) sentences (p = 0.03). No associations were found between the emotional polarity of MMSE sentences and overall MMSE scores (MMSE score 8 SD of the ‘negative’ sentences were 23.5 8 4.1, ‘neutral’ sentences 24.3 8 4.7 and ‘positive’ sentences 24.2 8 4.7, p = 0.64). With regard to overall MMSE scores, there was no association between patients who expressed concern about their health and those not expressing this concern (mean MMSE 23.2 8 4.3 vs. 24.1 8 4.8, p = 0.18). Similarly, no association was found with regard to GDS-15 (10.5 8 3.6 vs. 9.6 8 3.6, p = 0.36) Associations between MMSE Sentence Characteristics and Clinical Diagnoses (table 3) Of the 344 patients, there were 142 (41.3%) with a diagnosis of dementia, 84 (24.4%) with MCI and 118 (34.3%) without cognitive impairment. During the geriatric assessment a total of 128/344 patients (37.2%) were diagnosed with depression. Number of Words. The number of words in MMSE sentences had a statistically significant association with the degree of cognitive impairment. While cognitively normal patients wrote longer (4.18 8 1.41) sentences, sentences were shorter in those with MCI (3.82 8 1.14) and dementia (3.74 8 1.22, p value = 0.02). No associations were found between the number of words in the sentence and a diagnosis of depression. Emotional Polarity of Sentence. Overall 323 sentences of the MMSE were classified according to their emotional Sentence Writing Component of the MMSE Table 1. Characteristics of study population (n = 344) Characteristics Mean 8 SD Range (patients with available data) Age (n = 334), years Gender, female (n = 335) Marital status, married (n = 219) Education (n = 178), years Language of assessment (n = 344) Hebrew Russian Comorbidity scores Total CIRS-G Score (n = 125) TCS of CCI Barthel Index (n = 186) OARS (n = 182) MMSE (n = 344) GDS-15 (n = 79) 78.285.4 Patients n (%) 65–92 226 (67.5) 109 (49.8) 10.984.2 0–20 135 (39.2) 209 (60.8) 13.484.9 5.681.8 81.8815.7 7.383.1 23.984.7 9.7583.64 2–23 3–11 35–100 1–14 8–30 1–15 Total CIRS-G = Total Cumulative Illness Rating Scale Geriatric Score; TCS of CCI = total combined score of the Charlson Comorbidity Index; OARS = Older Americans Resources and Services Score. polarity (in 21 instances MMSE sentences were ‘bizarre’ or without any information, such as ‘I am here’, ‘My sister’, etc.). Sentences were determined to be ‘negative’ in 32 (9.9%) patients (e.g. ‘My life is very hard’, ‘I am very pessimistic about the future’, etc.), ‘neutral’ in 215 (66.6%) and ‘positive’ in 76 (23.5%) (e.g. ‘I am always very satisfied with life’, ‘Very beautiful and interesting life’, etc.). The emotional polarity of sentences had no association with cognitive diagnoses but had a statistically significant association with the diagnosis of depression, with 21 patients (17.5%) of those writing a ‘negative’ sentence being depressed compared to 11 (5.4%) patients without a diagnosis of depression writing a ‘negative’ sentence (p value !0.0001). Concerns about Health. Concerns about health expressed in the sentence were not associated with the cognitive diagnosis. However, while 30 of the 128 patients (23.4%) with depression wrote a sentence expressing this concern, only 23 of the 216 patients (10.6%) without depression expressed concerns about their health (p value !0.0001). Impact of Age, Gender and Education on Cognitive and Affective Status (table 4) Age was inversely proportional to MMSE scores (Pearson’s correlation –0.233, p = 0.000). However, there was Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 2012;33:125–131 127 Table 2. Impact of age, gender and education on MMSE sentence characteristics Number of words in sentence Emotional polarity of sentence Pearson’s mean 8 SD range correlation patients Age, years Pearson’s correlation –0.032 Mean 8 SD Range Patients 334 p value 0.56 Gender Female Male Patients p value 2–10 1–9 3.981.3 3.981.3 226 109 335 negative positive 77.586.1 68–88 31 78.585.4 78.285.3 65–91 66–92 76 208 0.71 24 (11.2) 7 (6.9) 31 52 (24.3) 24 (23.5) 76 15 (14.6) 5 (8.1) 20 17 (16.5) 16 (25.8) 33 0.61 Education ≤12 years ≥13 years Patients p value 3.781.2 4.081.2 2–9 2–9 115 63 178 0.16 Concerns about health in sentence neutral patients yes no patients 315 79.285.6 68–91 52 77.985.3 65–92 282 0.13 138 (64.5) 71 (69.6) 209 0.44 214 102 316 41 (18.1) 11 (10.1) 52 185 (81.9) 98 (89.9) 283 0.04 226 109 335 71 (68.9) 41 (66.1) 112 0.22 103 62 165 25 (21.7) 7 (11.1) 32 90 (78.3) 56 (88.9) 146 0.06 115 63 178 334 Figures are numbers (%) unless otherwise specified. Table 3. Associations between MMSE sentence characteristics and diagnosis of MCI, dementia and depression Cognitive diagnosis dementia (n = 142) Number of words Mean 8 SD 3.7481.22 Range 0–20 Patients, n 142 Depression MCI (n = 84) normal (n = 118) total p value yes (n = 128) no (n = 216) total p value 3.8281.14 0–20 84 4.1881.41 0–17 118 3.9181.23 0–20 344 0.02 3.8381.19 2–9 128 3.9681.33 1–10 216 3.9181.23 1–10 344 <0.36 Emotional polarity of sentence Negative 12 (9.5) Positive 28 (22.2) Neutral 86 (68.3) Patients, n 126 9 (11.3) 21 (26.3) 50 (62.5) 80 11 (9.4) 27 (23.1) 79 (67.5) 117 32 (9.9) 76 (23.5) 215 (66.6) 323 0.94 21 (17.5) 24 (20.0) 75 (62.5) 101 11 (5.4) 52 (25.6) 140 (69.0) 203 32 (9.9) 76 (23.5) 215 (66.6) 323 <0.001 Concerns about health Yes 25 (17.6) No 117 (82.4) Patients, n 142 12 (14.3) 72 (85.7) 84 16 (13.6) 102 (86.4) 118 53 (15.4) 291 (84.6) 344 0.63 30 (23.4) 98 (76.6) 128 23 (10.6) 193 (89.4) 216 53 (15.4) 291(84.6) 344 <0.001 Figures are numbers (%) unless otherwise specified. no significant difference in age between cognitively impaired patients and those without impairment. No correlation was found between GDS-15 scores and age, nor was age correlated with the diagnosis of depression. No statistically significant correlations were found between education on the one hand, and MMSE, GDS-15 128 Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 2012;33:125–131 scores and cognitive or affective diagnoses on the other. Although there was no association between the gender of patients and GDS-15, more women were diagnosed as depressed than men [98 women (43.4%) vs. 30 men (27.5%), p = 0.03]. Press /Velikiy /Berzak /Tandater /Peleg / Freud /Punchik /Dwolatzky Table 4. Impact of age, gender and education on the cognitive and affective tests MMSE Age Pearson’s correlation Mean 8 SD Range Patients p value Dementia MCI Normal 79.285.1 67–92 139 0.12 77.385.2 65–87 84 0.0 77.685.6 67–92 111 76 0.95 10.983.8 0–20 84 0.33 10.484.9 0–20 52 11.683.8 0–17 42 –0.233 334 0.000 Education Mean 8 SD Range Patients p value GDS-15 Depression Normal 78.185.2 69–88 128 0.92 78.285.5 65–92 206 10.984.4 0–20 93 0.95 11.083.9 0–17 85 ≤12 years Mean 8 SD Range Patients 23.284.6 8–30 115 10.483.9 1–15 26 ≥13 years Mean 8 SD Range Patients p value 24.685.4 8–30 63 0.08 9.183.8 1–15 8 0.42 24.384.7 8–30 109 9.083.5 3–14 20 30 (27.5) 79 (72.5) 10.083.8 1–15 56 0.32 98 (43.4) 0.03 128 (56.6) Gender Male Mean 8 SD Range Patients, n (%) Female Mean 8 SD Range Patients, n (%) p value 23.884.7 8–30 226 0.36 37 (34.0) 102 (45.2) 0.14 Discussion In this retrospective study we demonstrated a clear relationship between the characteristics of the sentence written as part of the MMSE test and the cognitive and affective features of older patients evaluated by the CGAU. The characteristics of the patients seen at the CGAU were indicated by low functional performance, high comorbidity indexes, and a high prevalence of cognitive impairment and depression (table 1). Apart from a gender predisposition, with more women expressing concern about their health in the MMSE sentence than men, no other associations were found between the characteristics of the MMSE sentence and age, gender or education. The greater degree of health concerns expressed by the women may reflect the fact that more women were diagnosed as being depressed. Sentence Writing Component of the MMSE 32 (29.4) 52 (23.0) 40 (36.7) 72 (31.9) The finding that age does not affect the sentences is not consistent with that of a previous study [27], which found that with increasing age individuals use more positive and fewer negative words, use fewer self-references, use more future-tense and fewer past-tense verbs, and demonstrate a general pattern of increasing cognitive complexity. The lack of correlation with age in our study may be explained at least partially by the fact that our study population had a relatively high degree of cognitive and affective impairments. An important finding of our study is that there was no association between the number of words in the MMSE sentence and the overall MMSE score. This result is similar to that reported by Shenkin et al. [28] but is in contrast with the finding of McCarthy et al. [16]. We found a significant association between the number of words per sentence and the cognitive diagnosis. The shortest senDement Geriatr Cogn Disord 2012;33:125–131 129 tences were written by patients with dementia, those with MCI wrote slightly longer sentences, and the longest sentences were written by patients without cognitive impairment. This result was expected and coincides with that of previous studies, confirming that the ability to write a sentence is adversely affected by cognitive impairment [3, 6, 27]. A previous study found a correlation between the degree of cognitive impairment and a decline in the ability for sentence writing: while 85% of nondemented elderly wrote a correct spontaneous sentence, only 67% of those with mild dementia, 41% of those with moderate dementia and none with severe dementia were able to write such a sentence [29]. In comparing the length of the sentence with a diagnosis of depression, we found that although patients with depression wrote shorter sentences than those without depression, this association was not statistically significant. The relationship between depression and cognitive impairment has been observed in some [30–33] although not all studies [34, 35]. The fact that in this study no association was found between the length of the sentence and a diagnosis of depression may indicate that the length of the sentence itself does not reflect the affective state of patients with cognitive impairment. Our findings are similar to those of Shenkin et al. [28] who also failed to find an association between the number of words in the MMSE sentence and depressive symptoms. It is of interest that in our study patients with higher GDS-15 scores wrote shorter sentences. It is possible that the GDS-15 is quite sensitive to cognitive changes. This can be explained by the fact that three questions included in the GDS-15 may partially reflect cognitive function (‘Have you dropped many of your activities and interests?’, ‘Do you prefer to stay at home, rather than going out and doing new things?’, ‘Do you feel you have more problems with memory than most?’). Another important finding of our study is that the negative emotional polarity of the sentence correlated with a diagnosis of depression and with GDS-15 scores. The hazard ratio for depression among patients with negative emotional polarity of sentences compared with patients with positive or neutral polarity was 3.70 (CI = 1.72–7.99, p = 0.001). No correlation was found between the emotional polarity of the MMSE sentence and cognitive diagnosis or overall MMSE scores. These findings contradict the results of previous studies. According to the study by McCarthy et al. [16] the polarity of the sentence was inversely related to the MMSE score, while Rösler et al. [17] found that ‘sentences with inadequate emotional content seem to indicate a dementing illness’. On the other hand, as in our study, Shenkin et al. [28] 130 Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 2012;33:125–131 found no association between the polarity of the sentence and MMSE scores. Since in all these studies [16, 17, 28] the affective status of patients was not determined, it is difficult to compare our results with these authors. It seems reasonable to assume that those patients attending the geriatric day hospital in the study by McCarthy et al. [16] or the demented patients of a geriatric clinic in the sample of Rösler et al. [17] were more depressed than the community-dwelling individuals without dementia in the study by Shenkin et al. [28]. Finally, like emotional polarity, the expression of concerns about health in the MMSE sentence was associated with a diagnosis of depression but not with cognitive impairment. The hazard ratio for depression among patients expressing health concerns was 2.57 (CI = 1.42– 4.66, p = 0.001). To the best of our knowledge, this is the first time that this characteristic of the MMSE sentence has been investigated. Our study has several strengths. As far as we have been able to ascertain, this is the first study comparing the characteristics of the MMSE sentence not only with cognitive aspects but also with the affective status of the elderly. In addition, our evaluation involved other languages, namely Hebrew and Russian. Finally, health concerns expressed by the subjects in the MMSE sentence were found to be influenced by their cognitive and affective status. On the other hand our study has obvious limitations. Being a retrospective data analysis, possible confounders may have been missed including cognitive and affective diagnoses. There were also missing data, especially for education and GDS score. Also, the population was not culturally homogeneous. In addition, we did not evaluate some characteristics of the MMSE sentences that were reported by previous studies [16, 17, 28], namely legibility of writing, letter case, use of first person, and wishes. In conclusion, the results of our study suggest that the MMSE sentence provides important relevant information when assessing older people. Short sentences may indicate cognitive impairment, whereas negative emotional polarity or concerns about health expressed in the sentence may allude to the presence of an affective disorder. Since the MMSE is widely used as a screening instrument for cognitive impairment in primary practice, more focused attention on the content of the MMSE sentence may help to identify patients at increased risk not only for cognitive impairment but also for depression. Disclosure Statement None. Press /Velikiy /Berzak /Tandater /Peleg / Freud /Punchik /Dwolatzky References 1 Wellingham-Jones P: The Mind/Body Connection: Neurophysiological Basis for Handwriting, ed 2. Tehama, PWJ Publishing, 1989. 2 McNeil MR, Tseng CH: Acquired neurogenic agraphias: writing problems; in LaPointe LL (ed): Aphasia and Related Neurogenic Language Disorders, ed 3. New York, Thieme Medical Publishers, 2005. 3 Lezak MD: Neuropsychological Assessment. New York, Oxford University Press, 1995. 4 Croisile B: Agraphia in Alzheimer’s disease. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 1999; 10: 226– 230. 5 LaBarge E, Smith DS, Dick L, Storandt M: Agraphia in dementia of the Alzheimer type. Arch Neurol 1992;49:1151–1156. 6 Neils J, Boller F, Gerdeman B, Cole M: Descriptive writing abilities in Alzheimer’s disease. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 1989; 11: 692– 698. 7 Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR: ‘Mini-mental state’. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res 1975;12:189–198. 8 O’Connor DW, Pollitt PA, Hyde JB, Fellows JL, Miller ND, Brook CP, Reiss BB: The reliability and validity of the Mini-Mental State in a British community survey. J Psychiatr Res 1989;23:87–96. 9 Wind AW, Schellevis FG, Van Staveren G, Scholten RP, Jonker C, Van Eijk JT: Limitations of the Mini-Mental State Examination in diagnosing dementia in general practice. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 1997;12:101–108. 10 Kim KW, Lee DY, Jhoo JH, Youn JC, Suh YJ, Jun YH, Seo EH, Woo JI: Diagnostic accuracy of mini-mental status examination and revised Hasegawa dementia scale for Alzheimer’s disease. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 2005;19:324–330. 11 Ringdal GI, Ringdal K, Juliebø V, Wyller TB, Hjermstad MJ, Loge JH: Using the minimental state examination to screen for delirium in elderly patients with hip fracture. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 2011; 32: 394– 400. 12 De Marchis GM, Foderaro G, Jemora J, Zanchi F, Altobianchi A, Biglia E, Conti FM, Monotti R, Mombelli G: Mild cognitive impairment in medical inpatients: the MiniMental State Examination is a promising screening tool. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 2010;29:259–264. 13 Braekhus A, Laake K, Engedal K: The MiniMental State Examination: identifying the most efficient variables for detecting cognitive impairment in the elderly. J Am Geriatr Soc 1992;40:1139–1145. 14 Shigemori K, Ohgi S, Okuyama E, Shimura T, Schneider E: The factorial structure of the mini mental state examination (MMSE) in Japanese dementia patients. BMC Geriatr 2010;10:36. Sentence Writing Component of the MMSE 15 Noale M, Limongi F, Minicuci N: Identification of factorial structure of MMSE based on elderly cognitive destiny: the Italian Longitudinal Study on Aging. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 2006;21:233–241. 16 McCarthy F, Kennedy F, Duggan J, Sheehan J, Power D: A retrospective analysis of the sentence writing component of Folstein’s MMSE. Ir J Psychol Med 2004;21:125–127. 17 Rösler A, Fickenscher V, von Renteln-Kruse W, Billino J: Sentences written during the Mini-Mental State Examination: content and diagnostic value in cognitively healthy elderly people and patients with dementia. J Am Geriatr Soc 2005;53:2240–2241. 18 Nasreddine ZS, Phillips NA, Bedirian V, Charbonneau S, Whitehead V, Collin I, Cummings JL, Chertkow H: The Montreal Cognitive Assessment, MoCA: a brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment. J Am Geriatr Soc 2005;53:695–699. 19 Dwolatzky T, Whitehead V, Doniger G, Simon ES, Schweiger A, Jaffe D, Chertkow H: Validity of a novel computerized cognitive battery for mild cognitive impairment. BMC Geriatr 2003;3:4. 20 Sheikh JI, Yesavage J: Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS): recent evidence and development of a shorter version; in Brink TL (ed): Clinical Gerontology: A Guide to Assessment and Intervention. New York, Haworth Press, 1986, pp 165–173. 21 American Psychiatric Association: Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th Edition, Text Revision. Washington, American Psychiatric Association, 2000. 22 Gauthier S, Reisberg B, Zaudig M, Petersen RC, Ritchie K, Broich K, Belleville S, Brodaty H, Bennett D, Chertkow H, Cummings JL, de Leon M, Feldman H, Ganguli M, Hampel H, Scheltens P, Tierney MC, Whitehouse P, Winblad B, International Psychogeriatric Association Expert Conference on mild cognitive impairment: Mild cognitive impairment. Lancet 2006;367:1262–1270. 23 Mahoney FI, Barthel DW: Functional evaluation: the Barthel Index. Md State Med J 1965;14:61–65. 24 Doble SE, Fisher AG: The dimensionality and validity of the Older Americans Resources and Services (OARS) Activities of Daily Living (ADL) Scale. J Outcome Meas 1998;2:4–24. 25 Miller MD, Paradis CF, Houck PR, Mazumdar S, Stack JA, Rifai AH, Mulsant B, Reynolds CF 3rd: Rating chronic medical illness burden in geropsychiatric practice and research: application of the Cumulative Illness Rating Scale. Psychiatry Res 1992; 41: 237– 248. 26 Charlson ME, Pompei P, Ales KL, MacKenzie CR: A new method of classifying prognostic comorbidity in longitudinal studies: development and validation. J Chronic Dis 1987;40:373–383. 27 Pennebaker JW, Stone LD: Words of wisdom: language use over the life span. J Pers Soc Psychol 2003;85:291–301. 28 Shenkin SD, Starr JM, Dunn JM, Carter S, Deary IJ: Is there information contained within the sentence-writing component of the mini mental state examination? A retrospective study of community dwelling older people. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 2008; 23: 1283–1289. 29 Ericsson K, Forssell LG, Holmén K, Viitanen M, Winblad B: Copying and handwriting ability in the screening of cognitive dysfunction in old age. Arch Gerontol Geriatr 1996; 22:103–121. 30 Palmer K, Berger AK, Monastero R, Winblad B, Bäckman L, Fratiglioni L: Predictors of progression from mild cognitive impairment to Alzheimer disease. Neurology 2007; 68:1596–1602. 31 Lyketsos CG, Lopez O, Jones B, Fitzpatrick AL, Breitner J, DeKosky S: Prevalence of neuropsychiatric symptoms in dementia and mild cognitive impairment: results from the cardiovascular health study. JAMA 2002; 288:1475–1483. 32 Ravaglia G, Forti P, Lucicesare A, Rietti E, Pisacane N, Mariani E, Dalmonte E: Prevalent depressive symptoms as a risk factor for conversion to mild cognitive impairment in an elderly Italian cohort. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 2008;16:834–843. 33 Solfrizzi V, D’Introno A, Colacicco AM, Capurso C, Del Parigi A, Caselli RJ, Scapicchio PL, Scafato E, Gandin C, Capurso A, Panza F, Italian Longitudinal Study on Aging Working Group: Incident occurrence of depressive symptoms among patients with mild cognitive impairment – the Italian longitudinal study on aging. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 2007;24:55–64. 34 Panza F, Capurso C, D’Introno A, Colacicco AM, Zenzola A, Menga R, Pistoia G, Santamato A, Scafato E, Gandin C, Capurso A, Solfrizzi V: Impact of depressive symptoms on the rate of progression to dementia in patients affected by mild cognitive impairment. The Italian Longitudinal Study on Aging. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 2008; 23: 726– 734. 35 Becker JT, Chang YF, Lopez OL, Dew MA, Sweet RA, Barnes D, Yaffe K, Young J, Kuller L, Reynolds CF 3rd: Depressed mood is not a risk factor for incident dementia in a community-based cohort. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 2009;17:653–663. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 2012;33:125–131 131
© Copyright 2025 Paperzz