AMER. ZOOL., 14:303-315 (1974). Exploration and Social Play in Squirrel Monkeys (Saimiri) JOHN D. BALDWIN AND JANICE I. BALDWIN Department of Sociology, University of California, Santa Barbara, California 93106 SYNOPSIS. Squirrel monkeys (Saimiri) have been studied in a variety of laboratory and natural environments. The frequency and form of exploration and social play vary considerably among different environments. For example, in some environments, young monkeys have been observed to play for 3 hr per day; but in one natural environment, not a single bout of social play was seen during a 10-week intensive study. Numerous intermediate levels of play activity have been observed. Whereas many theories of play make it appear that play is essential for the development of sexual behavior, integrated roles in troop structure, control of aggressive responses, social cohesion, etc., the data on squirrel monkeys indicate that social organization and many normal social behaviors can develop without social play. However, the opportunity to play socially provides learning experiences that increase the variety of each animal's behavioral repertoire and the subtly of social cues to which it can respond. An adaptive modicum of competence can appear without social play, but the opportunity to play socially develops the competence of animals beyond that modicum. (1967, 1968) conducted the first field study on squirrel monkeys in 1965 on the llanos Socially living groups of squirrel mon- of Colombia. Our research has included keys (Saimiri) have been studied in an in- studies on a semifree-ranging troop in Florteresting variety of different environments. ida, a field study in Panama, a survey of 43 Several of these studies have dealt with troops in Panama, Colombia, Brazil, and exploration and play, and taken together, Peru, and a laboratory study at the Maxthey show that the development of play Planck-Institute (Baldwin, 1969; Baldwin varies among different environments. Ploog and Baldwin, 1971, 1972). Klein and Klein and his colleagues at the Max-Planck-Insti- (personal communication) made observatute for Psychiatry in Munich have been tions on social play in squirrel monkeys conducting diversified laboratory research during a 19-month field study on spider for over a decade (Ploog et al., 1963, 1967). monkeys (A teles belzebuth) in the La MacaRosenblum (1968) has conducted labora- rena area of Colombia. Recently, Bailey tory research on infant development and has been conducting observations on freemother-infant relationships. Thorington ranging troops on Santa Sophia Island in the Amazon River in Colombia. Although We would like to thank the following people for has been only one target of research play their generous assistance in various aspects of our in these studies, all of the studies interest work: Mr. and Mrs. Julio Arauz, Mr. Alberto Cadena, Mr. Frank DuMond, Dr. Sigrid Hopf, Dr. have provided valuable data on the relaC. W. Marinkelle, Dr. M. Maurus, Dr. Allan Mazur, tionship between play and the environment. Dr. Martin Moynihan, Dr. Detlev Ploog, Dr. L. A. It is already clear that there is considerRosenblum, Dr. Charles Southwick, and Dr. Richard able variation in the play of squirrel monThorington. Our studies were supported by a Public Health keys. For example, in some environments, Service Fellowship (1-F1-MH-29, 131; BEH) from young squirrel monkeys have been reported the National Institute of Mental Health, a small to play for 3 hr per day, but in one natural grant (GS-1474) from the National Science Founda- environment not a single bout of social play tion, Public Health Service grants (MN-16482-01) and (1 ROS MH17882-01) from the National Insti- was observed during a 10-week, intensive study. Numerous intermediate levels of play tute of Mental Health. INTRODUCTION 303 304 JOHN D. BALDWIN AND JANICE I. BALDWIN activity have been observed. The differences in play patterns covary with numerous complex ecological and social factors, such as forest size, troop size, food abundance, adult sociability, etc. The large number of interacting conditions that vary among environments demonstrates the complexity of factors that influence and are influenced by play. In spite of the complexity, however, certain patterns do emerge. In this paper we will review the literature on exploration and social play in squirrel monkeys in order to provide descriptive material on the frequency and style of play in a variety of different environments. Also, the role of social play in squirrel monkey socialization and social organization will be discussed. A subsequent paper (Baldwin and Baldwin, 1975) will present an analysis of the factors that appear to integrate the diversity of data on squirrel monkey play. METHODS AND DEFINITIONS The methodology of observation, data recording, and analysis used in the several studies on squirrel monkeys has varied according to the observer and research setting. For the specific research strategies used in the various studies, the reader is referred to the methodology section of the papers cited below. Among the observers who have studied exploration and play in squirrel monkeys there appears to be a general concensus in the definitions of terms, but quantitative data have been collected according to differing criteria. Although there have been few explicit definitions of exploration or social play for squirrel monkeys, most observers appear to have used similar criteria. "Exploration" tends to refer to nonsocial manipulation, investigation or inspection of the physical environment, although the term "social exploration" has been used to refer to the earliest playful social contact among infants. The term "social play" clearly refers to those social interactions that include wrestling, chasing, sham-biting, jumping on, pulling tails, carrying, steep leaps, and other related activities. Although the term "play" usually tends to refer to social play, the term has often been used to include both nonsocial exploration and social play. In quantifying the amount of time that individual squirrel monkeys spend in social play, at least three different criteria have been utilized, (i) The simplest type of measurement is a rough estimate of the time that animals are "for the most part" active in play, or the amount of time that play seems to be the dominant activity. Thus, short pauses in play are included in this estimate. Most of the observers reviewed in this article have at one time or another used this type of rough estimate to quantify the amount of play they were observing, (ii) A more accurate means of measuring play is to use a stop watch or other cumulative time recorder to measure only the moments when players are physically active in any of the behaviors considered to be play. Short pauses in play and moments when wrestlers are inactive are not included. It has been our experience that the second method leads to estimates of duration of play that are approximately 50 to 75% as large as those from the first method, (iii) An alternative estimate of play has been used in some of the later publications from the Max-Planck-Institute. Only wrestling or contact play is measured during moments when physical movement is visible (chasing play is not included in these time recordings). Hopf (personal communication) estimates that this method produces measures 10% smaller than the second method above. It is not known what level of interobserver reliability exists among the different research teams that have estimated duration of play. Since most of the reports that describe play activity have used method number one for measuring the duration of play, all estimates of play presented in this paper are based on method (i), i.e., the total time in which play is the dominant activity, including the short pauses in play. THE DATA ON EXPLORATION AND SOCIAL PLAY Laboratory studies: Ploog et al. (1967) In the early 1960's, careful observations on squirrel monkey social behavior were begun in the laboratories of the Max-Planck- PLAY IN SQUIRREL MONKEYS (Saimiri) Institute for Psychiatry in Munich. Ploog et al. (1967) published the first detailed observations on the ontogeny of squirrel monkey behavior. The descriptions were based on 1200 hr of observation on 10 animals that were followed for varying periods of time from birth (or shortly thereafter) up to the third or fourth year of life. The young animals lived in groups varying from 3 to 9 in size, in which there were 0, 1, or 2 other young animals with whom to play. The sample consisted of 7 males and 3 females. During the first 5 to 7 weeks of life, the infant was very closely associated with its mother. Between 3 and 16 days of age, the infant began exploring the environment by touching both objects and other animals in its cage. By 17 to 29 days of age the infant climbed off the mother and broadened its explorations. By the age of 5 to 7 weeks, the infant made its first jumps and became accustomed to being off the mother. Also, the mother began weaning the infant at 5 to 7 weeks. If there were other animals in the infant's group, they established play relationships, but when no other young animals were present, the infant "teased" the adults, who generally would not play with it. Ploog et al. (1967) identified several types of peer play. The two main forms were (i) contact play, with wrestling and scuffling, and (ii) distance play with chasing. During contact play one animal holds and shambites the second, while the second struggles to get away. The two players of a dyad often exchange roles frequently and quickly. They roll around on the floor until one animal pulls away, and a pause occurs in play until they jump on each other again to resume contact. During distance play, the animals chase each other through the cage, often switching between the roles of chasing and fleeing. They keep eye contact, but they stay out of each other's reach. Several times a day, young animals were observed to engage in periods of social play that lasted up to an hour each. Young animals could spend as much as several hours a day in active social play. Other types of social play began to emerge 305 later in behavioral development. By the first year of age, young males began play fights with the colony's alpha male. With the minimum of effort, the adult male controlled the yearling. As Winter (1968, p. 249) described, the play of the young male and the adult male may be regarded as a "fighting tournament." "The immature animal tries to attack the aduk male repeatedly whereas the adult male just seems to defend itself." Occasionally real fights broke out between the two. The young male began to be put in the role of Priigelknabe (scapegoat) and was attacked whenever there was a disturbance in the colony. Attacks directed to animals under 2.5 years of age never led to injuries (Hopf, 1972). Elements of sexual behavior appeared in early play, and by 1 year of age, animals of both sexes began to show adult-like sexual patterns: pursuit, mounting, and thrusting by the male, and courtship posturing, looking over the shoulder, and coquettish fleeing by the female. Ploog et al. (1963) described a play posture in which one animal looked between its legs as an element of sexual play. Animals could switch sex roles during play and even long into adulthood. Elements that parallel mother-infant interactions also occurred in play. One animal would carry its play partner in the dorsal-ventral position typical of mother-infant carrying, though these bouts were usually brief because the rider was usually thrown off. Players also attempted to assume the nursing position. Young females often carried small infants for prolonged periods on their backs. Near the end of the second year, the young squirrel monkeys began to initiate play less frequently. Often the playfulness of the young animals was inhibited by the rejection responses of the adults. The alpha male dominated young males and adult females dominated young females. After the second year, social play was uncommon and was usually begun by the initiative of younger animals. Laboratory studies: Rosenblum (1968) Rosenblum studied the development of six infants (five of which were female) that 306 JOHN D. BALDWIN AND JANICE I. BALDWIN were conceived and born in the laboratory. Visual exploration of the environment began after the first several days of life. The infant began to reach for objects in the environment and to explore manually by the beginning of the third week. "This manual exploration, while still clasping the mother . . . rises sharply during the second month and occurs at almost every opportunity at that age" (p. 218). As manual exploration began to decline in the third month, object exploration and social play became more common (see Fig. 1, 2). Nonsocial play, i.e., running, jumping, swinging, etc., developed rapidly from the beginning of the second month. Social play appeared in the beginning of the third month and remained at a stable level for the remainder of infancy. Social play consisted primarily of "infants alternately chasing one another and occasional playful wrestling together . . ." (p. 230). Llanos of Colombia: Thorington (1967,1968) In 1965 Thorington conducted the first systematic field study on squirrel monkeys in a 15-hectare forest on the llanos of Colombia. At the beginning of the study, the troop of 18 animals consisted of 3 adult males, 5 adult females, and 10 young, and during the 10-week study 4 infants were born. The troop tended to fragment into subgroups of 5 to 8 animals during foraging periods in which the animals looked for dispersed fruits and insect foods. There was usually a 1- to 2-hr midday rest period in the heat of the day, and most activities subsided as the animals descended to low, dense thickets. It was during these periods when the adults were resting and the troop remained in one place that play among the juveniles was most common. Play behavior "principally involved hanging by the hind 60-1 Age (months) FIG. 1. The development of manual exploration while on the mother and object explorations made while free of the mother in squirrel monkey infants. (After L. A. Rosenblum, 1968, by permission of Academic Press.) PLAY IN SQUIRREL MONKEYS (Saimiri) 307 30-, Nonsocial play Social play 20- T —1 10 Age (months) FIG. 2. The development of social and nonsocial play in squirrel monkey infants. (After L. A. Rosen- blum, 1968, by permission of Academic Press.) legs and wrestling, or mock biting and chasing" (Thorington, 1968, p. 79). "All the play observed took place in trees. Since the troop was surprised very near the ground on only one occasion, it is doubtful that any play took place on the ground . . .." "No vocalizations were observed to be given by the juveniles as they played . . .." After the midday rest period, the troop again fragmented into several subgroups for afternoon foraging. From Thorington's report and from, personal observations on 12 troops on the llanos of Colobmia, we would estimate that a young monkey in these troops plays between 5 and 30 min per day. chronized with the Florida rainy season, and each year's cohort of players could be discriminated for about the first 2 years of life. DuMond (1968) observed that the infant began active exploration from the mother's back in the second or third week. Social play was common by the 8 to 10 week period. At times it was active and rough, and it involved sexual elements even at that time. Most social play tended to occur on the ground, in clear areas where the animals could run, hop on each other, wrestle and roll. "When play occurs in the trees, it is quite often in the form of a play pair hanging by their feet and arm wrestling" (p. 110). Arboreal play, "once initiated, quickly turns into ground play as the animals tumble toward the ground while wrestling." "The general play pattern . . . is usually initiated by one animal hopping bipedally to another monkey and grasping it on the back with its hands. The second monkey then twists around toward his 'attacker' Seminatural environment: DuMond (1968) In 1960, DuMond established a colony of squirrel monkeys in a 1.6-hectare forest in southern Florida. Provisioned with abundant food and protected from molestation, the colony grew from 37 to 110 animals in the first 6 years. Seasonal births became syn- 308 JOHN D. BALDWIN AND JANICE I. BALDWIN and a wrestle pair forms which usually ends a few seconds later with one pulling away and scampering off, being chased by the partner" (p. 129). When play became very rough, the molested animal would begin to vocalize. During the first 6 or 7 months, mothers frequently retrieved their infants from such situations. DuMond commented that the posture of one animal looking between the legs at another appeared to be a play-invitation posture rather than a form of sex play, as Ploog et al. (1963) had interpreted it. The young were independent of their mothers by 1 year of age. Most social play occurred among animals of the same age cohort; however, play between animals of different cohorts did occur. Also, play between young uakaris (Cacajao rubicundus) and young squirrel monkeys was not uncommon. At times even young tamarins (Saguinus fuscicollis) were observed to play with the squirrel monkeys. Young males showed interest in smelling the genitalia of estrus females, but their following and attempts to smell the females were much briefer and less elaborate than the complex smelling interactions between adult males and adult females. By three years of age, the females were adult and entered the reproducing female group. The young males spent the years from 3 to 5 in a subadult male stage: They may have been reproductively mature, but they were not integrated into the adult male section of the troop social organization. Social play occurred at many times of the day and in all seasons of the year. In the heat of the day during the summers there was a decrease in play that correlated with increased resting in cool, shaded areas. Seminatural environment: Baldwin (1969) Our research on squirrel monkeys began with a 1966-1967 study in the seminatural forest established by DuMond in Florida. In most ways our observations on play agree with DuMond's, but having had the opportunity to spend many more hours making observations on social behavior, we were able to trace the ontogeny of play in greater detail (Baldwin, 1969). Infants began visual and tactile exploration during the first week of life, as other observers have noted. However, in the seminatural environment, the infants did not venture off their mothers' bodies until the fifth week of life. This is 1 or 2 weeks later than Ploog et al. (1967) and Rosenblum (1968) observed in the laboratory. Perhaps the relatively greater complexity of the seminatural environment made early exploration more arousing or anxiety-inducing, and thus infants would be more inhibited about engaging in early exploration. In the seminatural environment, the first social play was seen at about the eighth week of life. This also is several weeks later than the time that Ploog et al. (1967) noted the onset of play in the laboratory. In the seminatural environment "the first infantinfant interactions were very exploratory in nature: the animals touched or gently pulled at each other, often mouthing or sham-biting any body part that was available. The interactions were short at first, often only incidental to nonsocial exploration" (Baldwin, 1969, p. 51). Early play appeared to be gentle, but two lines of evidence suggested that it was very arousing and perhaps sometimes painful to the animals involved. First, the players often gave play peep and shriek vocalizations, and frequently individuals withdrew temporarily from active interaction. Second, mothers occasionally retrieved their infants from such gentle-appearing play. During the ninth and tenth weeks of life, infants became less vocal while playing; they played more, they retreated or paused less during play activities, and they returned quickly to play if their mothers retrieved and removed them from play groups. Thus, infant play patterns changed significantly during the first 2 weeks as the infants showed decreasing signs of aversive experiences. During the second, third and fourth months of life, the format of infant social play changed as the animals became more coordinated and competent. The earliest play exploration consisted of tentative touching, pulling, and sham-biting. This PLAY IN SQUIRREL MONKEYS (Saimiri) 309 "Occasionally a very persistent infant could keep a juvenile engaged in play if there were no alternative play partners for the juvenile; but the incompatibility of the infant and juvenile styles of play prevented a continuous play interaction. Infantjuvenile play usually progressed as follows: an infant would approach a juvenile and grab or tackle it; the two would tumble once or twice until the juvenile had constrained the infant underneath it in the dorsal-ventral position; they would lay ijuicLly foi a few seconds (occasionally, the juvenile would thrust at the infant for a few seconds at this point) until the juvenile released its grasp and the infant escaped. A persistent infant could cause this pattern to be repeated many times; but the interaction was far from the continuous activity of playing age-mates" (p. 53-54). quickly changed to include more complete and close body contact: "first the infants engaged in lying together while holding and pulling each other with hands and feet; then they progressed to rolling and tumbling play as their pulling and wrestling became more vigorous" (p. 52). Dorsalventral holding was common in wrestling play. With time, there was an increase in the frequency of tumbling, jumping on, tackling, and hopping around during play at greater distances. Players often solicited play "by lying on the side or shoulder or looking between the legs" (p. 53). During the first 3 months of play, infants sometimes spent 1 or 2 hr per day in social play; however, they often travelled together or came near each other without engaging in play activities. In this respect they had weaker play habits than the older infants and juveniles. Play between the early infants and the juveniles that were 1 year older was not common. Infants often attempted to initiate such play if juveniles came near them in play periods, but the juveniles showed little interest in playing with early infants. During the second half of infancy (fifth through eleventh month), peer group activities increased steadily as mother-infant separation became more complete. By the end of infancy, the young animals travelled with their peer group nearly all day—engaging in foraging, resting, or play—although the young often tried to be near their mothers at night. Infant play had developed to include most of the juvenile patterns: chasing, hopping, presenting, sparring, mild threatening, and rougher tumbling and wrestling. As the late infants developed 50 - 40 - cfd1 30 - 20 - 10 —i— 5-7 (Infant) 1 11-13 (Early Juv.) 1 1 17-19 25-27 (Mid Juv.) (Late Juv.) FIG. 3. The number of players of various age and sex in randomly sampled social play groups (N = 272.) 30+ Months 310 JOHN D. BALDWIN AND JANICE I. BALDWIN greater competence in the play patterns typical of juveniles, they began to play more frequently with the juveniles that were 1 year older than they. The young animals were classified as juvenile between the ages of 11 and 30 months. As is shown in Figure 3, play activities reached a peak for the females during the early juvenile period, and they peaked for the males in the mid-juvenile period. Sex differences in play activity had begun to appear in late infancy, but they became much more important in the juvenile period. For the females, play was the roughest at the early juvenile stage, then became progressively milder such that by the late juvenile stage the females engaged only in quiet play interactions. By the late juvenile age, the females spent much time traveling near or huddling with the adult females, and they had become almost as passive as the adult females. The juvenile males, on the other hand, played progressively more roughly and more aggressively throughout the juvenile age period, although they played less frequently after the mid-juvenile period. Quiet play did occur among the juvenile males, but it was prone to escalate into rough activity. With increasing roughness, the animals showed an increasing tendency to avoid physical contact. Juvenile play often consisted of "standing back from or hopping around just beyond reach of each other, as if somewhat hesitant to engage in contact; and more threats were given during play, as if to keep others from making contact" (p. 60). By the mid-juvenile age, both males and females began to show brief organized patterns of copulatory behavior. "During play, from infancy on, both sexes engaged in thrusting behavior; but this sexual play remained relatively infrequent, compared with wrestling, chasing and other play forms, until the mid-juvenile stage. By the mid-juvenile stage, the males mounted and thrusted significantly more frequently during play than did the juvenile females. It was apparently because the stronger, rowdier animals seemed to have the advantage in reaching the top position in dorsal-ventral holds that the active males succeeded in thrusting more than the females" (p. 62). The juvenile males did not engage in any sexual behavior other than thrusting; however, as the males matured during the subadult period, they developed much more complex behavioral patterns that closely approximated the adult consorting and copulatory activities. By the time females reached 2.5 years of age and entered their first active reproductive season, they had acquired most of the passivity and social preference patterns typical of adult female squirrel monkeys. Also, they had experienced many copulatory approaches from juveniles and subadult males. At adulthood, the hormonally-induced changes of the estrus cycle clearly began to affect their behavior, too. The nonreceptive adult female was usually intolerant of approaches by adult males, but her behavior changed abruptly on those days when she was in behavioral estrus, i.e., receptive. There was no abrupt transition between the juvenile male and the subadult male stages. Between 2.5 and 4.5 years of age, the males continued to spend progressively less time engaged in play. The frequency of rough, aggressive play declined the most rapidly, and at 4 years of age males tended to play quietly more than roughly. During the subadult male phase, sexual play became increasingly complex. "Although the young subadult males tended to be rough in their general play and sexual play with juveniles, often causing the juveniles to shriek and run away, the older subadults were usually quite gentle, especially when playing with the less rowdy early juveniles or female juveniles. Occasionally older subadult males 'consorted' with animals as young as late infant: they would engage in many of the adult-like precopulatory approach patterns, then mount, gently holding the young animal, and thrust until the least agitation of the partner caused them to dismount. Such play often continued for many minutes without the younger animal giving vocalizations of discomfort or trying to leave the subadult male. The early subadult sexual play interactions did not involve the 'consort' or precopulatory elements; the young subadult male merely found a partner, mounted, and thrusted until the partner could wrestle loose. It seems likely that the subadult males learned to be gentle with their partners in order not to disturb the juveniles and hence to prolong the interaction" (p. 66). It is interesting to note that the adult males that succeed in copulating most frequently during the mating season were not the very PLAY IN SQUIRREL MONKEYS (Saimiri) aggressive ones, but were the quieter males (Baldwin, 1968). The transition from subadult male to adult male status was apparently a difficult one. The adult males did not permit young males to travel with the troop: they chased them off every time they detected them near the troop, and several young adult males received serious wounds in fights with adult males. In the 1966-1967 study, not one out of the five developing young males succeeded in establishing himself in the adult male subgroup. Follow-up observations made in 1971 showed that subadult males do eventually succeed in taking over and even replacing original adult males in the tix>op's adult male cluster. At present there are insufficient data to evaluate to what degree the play fight experience of juvenile and subadult males may benefit the individuals when the maturing males attempt to gain entrance into the adult male subgroup. Panama, Colombia, Brazil, and Peru: Baldwin and Baldwin (1971) In 1968 and 1969 we surveyed 31 separate troops of squirrel monkeys in Panama, Colombia, Brazil, and Peru. The purpose of the studies was to locate possible sites for extended field work on squirrel monkeys and to note possible variations in ecology and behavior. Troops were observed in the following locations: eight troops in small forests in Panama, twelve troops in the small to moderate-size forests on the llanos of Colombia, four troops in natural areas of the rain forests of Amazonia, and seven troops in areas of Amazonia that had been altered by human activities. In the small forests of Panama and the llanos of Colombia, troop size varied between 10 and 35 animals, but in the natural forests of Amazonia, troop size varied from 120 to 300 or more individuals. In altered Amazonia, troop size appeared to range from 20 to 80 animals, though good estimates were difficult to obtain. The frequency of social play varied considerably among the troops in the different locations. In the small troops of Panama 311 and the llanos of Colombia, play was not nearly so common as in the large troops of natural Amazonia. It is difficult to make estimates of the time that individuals spent in play, because of the limited contact that we had with each troop. However, our observations in conjunction with Thorington's (1968) data suggest that the young animals in the small troops of Panama and the llanos may play between 5 and 30 min per day. Our subjective impression from spending 5.9 hr with the large troops in natural Amazonia was that they played about half as much as the seminatural troop in Florida: this would imply about 1 to 2 hr per day of play per individual. An interesting correlation with the frequency of play was the fact that the adults of the large troops maintained closer individual distances and interacted more frequently than the adults of the small troops. The comparative data from the 31 study sites suggested the following hypothesis (Baldwin and Baldwin, 1971): Young animals in small troops have fewer potential play partners and hence less opportunity to play than animals in large troops; this restricted experience with play gives diem less opportunity to learn strong habits for social activity, and thus animals maturing in small troops tend to be less social as adults than animals that grow up in large troops with ample opportunity to play. In other words, the young monkeys in small troops have few age mates with whom to play, and this apparently creates a low probability that there will be two or more animals ready to play at any point in time. In large troops with many infants and juveniles, there is usually a subset of young animals ready to play during many hours of the day. The higher frequency of play in large troops provides the maturing monkeys with many and varied social experiences in conjunction with the positive reinforcers that are intrinsic in social play. Thus, the monkeys in large troops are reinforced for maintaining close individual distances during social play, they develop larger repertoires of social interaction patterns and they emit these social behaviors at higher frequencies as long as the behaviors remain 312 JOHN D. BALDWIN AND JANICE I. BALDWIN reinforcing. The data from the 31 study sites in South America consistently supported the hypothesis that increases in social play experience lead to decreases in the average individual distances maintained by animals during the infant, juvenile, and adult years. In other words, the peer group that plays together stays together. Play increases the reinforcers for maintaining close proximity and strengthens the habits of interacting socially. Barqueta, Southwestern Panama: Baldwin and Baldwin (1972) Between December 19, 1970, and February 25, 1971, we observed two troops of squirrel monkeys consisting of 23 and 27 animals in a natural forest on Hacienda Barqueta in Southwestern Panama. The squirrel monkeys habituated to our presence within 2 weeks and could be observed from distances of 3 to 15 m for long periods of time. In a total of 261 hr of observations on the squirrel monkeys during all hours of their waking day, not one instance of social play was observed among any of the animals in either troop. Presumably, the lack of play at the Barqueta study site was due to the dearth of foods preferred by the squirrel monkeys. The monkeys spent 95% of each hour of their 14-hr waking day engaged in foraging and in traveling between foraging areas. This pattern of daily activity left little time for any social interactions, including the two social activities that are most common for squirrel monkeys in other environments, resting together and playing. During the 5% of the time when the animals were not foraging or traveling, however, the young animals came into many situations that would have led to play in the other environments studied, but not once did the animals play. In spite of the lack of play, the Barqueta troops were very cohesive and the animals maintained very close individual distances. The earlier hypothesis that playing together was a necessary precondition for staying together in dose, integrated troops was clearly inadequate. The following evidence shows a great difference between the non-playing squirrel monkeys at Hacienda Barqueta and the playful squirrel monkeys at other study sites. The fact that the two Barqueta troops consisted of only 23 and 27 members respectively places them near the small end of squirrel monkey troop size (which varies from 10 to 300 or more animals). In general, the young monkeys in small troops tend to have few play partners and to engage in less play than monkeys in larger troops (Baldwin and Baldwin, 1971). However, animals in small troops do play. At Barqueta there were also opportunities for play. There were 5 infants and 7 juveniles in the main study troop of 23 animals. These young animals were frequently within 5 m of one or more age-mates. The two main food sources at Barqueta often attracted the whole troop into an area of 180 m2. The fruits of the palm Scheelea and the cactus-like plant Achmea both grew in tight clusters that forced the monkeys to come into very close contact as they spent 15 to 120 sec locating and picking suitable fruits. The animals regularly came into physical contact, often crawling over each other's bodies, without a single playful interaction. Playful infant and juvenile squirrel monkeys in other environments often began bouts of social play when they came equally close. At Barqueta during the periods between bouts of foraging when the monkeys did rest, they occasionally rested within arm's reach of each other. Yet, on only six occasions did one animal reach out and touch another. All of these touches were very brief; none led to a bout of social play, and one evoked a mild threat from the touched animal. Playful infants and juveniles in other environments very often became drawn into play under such circumstances. In short, the opportunities for play (i.e., the stimuli of the environmental setting and social cues) were present at Barqueta, but the animals never showed playful responses. If the monkeys were playful in other seasons of the year, one might expect that their play habits would appear with at least some moderate frequency under these favorable social cir- PLAY IN SQUIRREL MONKEYS (Saimiri) cumstances. Loy (1970) studied rhesus monkeys on Cayo Santiago Island before, during, and after a 22-day period when very little food was available. The starvation period was so severe that 6 of the 69 animals died. The frequencies of all behaviors except foraging dropped significantly during the 22-day period. The frequency of social play decreased from 2.54 to 0.15 bouts per hour. The fact that play did not decrease to zero under these severe starvation contingencies suggests that more factors beside temporary food deprivation must be operating to explain the total absence of play at Barqueta. If food were abundant in other seasons at Barqueta, and the monkeys played then, we doubt that a temporary deprivation period would reduce play to zero bouts in 261 hr of observation. Exploration behavior at Barqueta was somewhat less frequent than in other environments we have studied, but it is difficult to quantify the amount of time spent in exploration. Infants and juveniles at Barqueta showed explorative tendencies at various times during their daily traveling. Most of this explorativeness was directed to investigating things in the environment, like tree holes, abandoned squirrel nests, wasp nests, sources of food, broken branches, birds, insects, etc. The young squirrel monkeys often traveled near each other in the troop. The infants still nursed from their mothers and they spent less time foraging for fruits and insects than did the adults. This gave them more time to explore and/or interact, but no investigatory or play behavior was directed towards another monkey. Nor did the young rest together, cuddle together, or show other friendly interactions. Despite the absence of play and a reduced amount of exploratory behavior, the troops at Barqueta appeared to be cohesive and stable. The troops seldom fragmented and animals often traveled at close individual distances without signs of uneasiness or tension. Agonistic interactions were somewhat less frequent than in other environments: chases and displays occurred 40 to 313 45 times a day, and no fights were ever observed. Copulations were observed, though they consisted of very brief and simple episodes of mounting and thrusting, without any of the consort activities seen in other environments. The fact that 85% of the adult females were accompanied by infants indicates that the reproductive success rate was normal for the species.1 When comparing the behavior of the adults at Barqueta to adults in other environments, the most outstanding features are that their social behaviors were very simple and infrequent. However, a simple, basic repertoire can be adequate to allow survival. The Barqueta data suggest that squirrel monkeys can survive, reproduce, and maintain cohesive troops even in the absence of social play. There are other developmental routes to "normal" adult behavior besides the socializing experiences of social play; several of these are described and discussed elsewhere (Baldwin and Baldwin, 1973). Hopf (1972, personal communication) also presents data that indicate that social play experience is not crucial to social development. Infants raised in all-adult laboratory groups, without peers to play with, had minimal play experience but showed no marked differences from animals raised with peers and with social play experience. Hopf points out that there are numerous socializing factors that all make overlapping contributions to social development. These factors combine so differently for each maturing individual that each animal develops its own unique personality and role in the group structure. Presumably, the individuals raised without social play are exposed to enough other socializing factors that they can develop within the limits of what is perceived as "normal." observations in several natural and seminatural environments indicate that 85% of the adult females in a troop usually bear one infant per year: on the llanos of Colombia (Thorington, 1968; Baldwin and Baldwin, personal observation), on Santa Sophia Island near Leticia, Colombia (Bailey, personal communication), and in a seminatural environment in Florida (Baldwin, 1969). 314 JOHN D. BALDWIN AND JANICE I. BALDWIN SUMMARY The frequency and form of exploration and social play in squirrel monkeys vary considerably across different environments. The onset of active exploration away from, the mother's body can occur as early as the third week of life in the laboratory (Ploog et al., 1967), or as late as the fifth or sixth week in more complex environments (Baldwin, 1969). The onset of social play can begin as early as the fifth week in the laboratory (Ploog, et al., 1967; Hopf, 1972), or in the 8- to 10-week period in other environments (Baldwin, 1969), and in some environments, such as Barqueta, social play apparently never begins (Baldwin and Baldwin, 1972, 1973). Also, the amount of social play that young monkeys engage in can vary from 0 to over 3 hr per day (Baldwin and Baldwin, 1971, 1972). Many theories of play make it appear that social play is essential for the development of sexual behavior, integrated roles in the troop structure, control of aggressive responses, social cohesion, etc. (Beach, 1947; Welker, 1961; Harlow and Harlow, 1965; Marler and Hamilton, 1966; Loizos, 1967; Dolhinow and Bishop, 1970; Suomi and Harlow, 1971). The Barqueta data, however, indicate that social organization and many normal social behaviors can develop in squirrel monkeys without social play. On the other hand, the comparative data on squirrel monkeys indicate that the opportunity to play socially provides learning experiences that increase the variety of each animal's behavior repertoire and the subtlety of social cues to which it can respond. An adaptive modicum of competence can develop without play, but the opportunity to play develops the competence of animals beyond that modicum. Briefly stated, (i) social play is not necessary for the development and/or learning of an adaptive modicum of social interaction patterns and troop cohesion, but (ii) the opportunity to play provides socializing experiences in which young animals can develop more complex, varied social interaction patterns and stronger habits for engaging in frequent, overt social exchanges. The causes and effects of the variability in exploration and play are numerous and complex. A subsequent paper will present data that suggest a multifactor explanation of exploration and social play in squirrel monkeys (Baldwin and Baldwin, 1975). REFERENCES Baldwin, J. D. 1968. The social behavior of adult male squirrel monkeys (Saimiri sciureus) in a seminatural environment. Folia Primatol. 9:281314. Baldwin, J. D. 1969. The ontogeny of social behavior of squirrel monkeys (Saimiri sciureus) in a seminatural environment. Folia Primatol. 11:35-79. Baldwin, J. D., and J. I. Baldwin. 1971. Squirrel monkeys (Saimiri) in natural habitats in Panama, Colombia, Brazil and Peru. Primates 12:45-61. Baldwin, J. D., and J. I. Baldwin. 1972. The ecology and behavior of squirrel monkeys (Saimiri oerstedi) in a Natural Forest in Western Panama. Folia Primatol. 18:161-184. Baldwin, J. D., and J. I. Baldwin. 1973. The role of play in social organization: comparative observations of squirrel monkeys (Saimiri). Primates. (In press) Baldwin, J. D., and J. I. Baldwin. 1975. In F. E. Poirier and S. Chevalier-Skolnikoff [ed.], Primate socialization. Aldine Press, Chicago. (In press) Beach, F. A. 1945. Current concepts of play in animals. Amer. Natur. 79:523-541. Dolhinow, P. J., and N. Bishop. 1970. The development of motor skills and social relationships among primates through play. Minn. Symp. Child Psychol. 4:141-198. DuMond, F. V. 1968. The squirrel monkey in a seminatural environment, p. 87-145. In L. A. Rosenblum and R. W. Cooper [ed.], The squirrel monkey. Academic Press, Inc., New York. Harlow, H. F., and M. K. Harlow. 1965. The affectional systems, p. 287-334. In A. M. Schrier, H. F. Harlow, and F. Stollnitz [ed.], Behavior of non-human primates. Vol. 2. Academic Press, New York. Hopf, S. 1972. Sozialpsychologische Untersuchungen zur Verhaltensentwicklung des Totenkopfaffens, p. 1-137. Ph.D. Thesis. Phillipps-Universitat, Marburg/Lahn. Loizos, C. 1967. Play behaviour in higher primates: a review, p. 176-218. In D. Morris [ed.], Primate ethology. Weidenfeld and Nicolson, London. Loy, J. 1970. Behavioral responses of free-ranging rhesus monkeys to food shortage. Amer. J. Phys. Anthrop. 33:263-272. Marler, P. R., and W. J. Hamilton III. 1966. Mechanisms of animal behavior. John Wiley and Sons, New York. Ploog, D. W., J. Blitz, and F. Ploog. 1963. Studies on social and sexual behavior of the squirrel monkey (Saimiri sciureus). Folia Primatol. 1:29-66. PLAY IN SQUIRREL MONKEYS Ploog, D., S. Hopf, and P. Winter. 1967. Ontogenese des Verhaltens von Totenkopfaffen (Saimiri sciureus). Psychol. Forsch. 31:1-41. Rosenblum, L. A. 1968. Mother-infant relations and early behavioral development in the squirrel monkey, p. 207-233. In L. A. Rosenblum and R. W. Cooper [ed.], The squirrel monkey. Academic Press, New York. Suomi, S. J., and H. F. Harlow. 1971. Monkeys at play, p. 72-75. Natural History Special Supplement. Thorington, R. W., Jr. 1967. Feeding and activity of Cebus and Saimiri in a Colombian Forest, p. 180-184. In D. Starck, R. Schneider, and H. J. (Saimiri) 315 Kuhn [ed.], Neue Ergebnisse der Primatologie. Gustav Fisher Verlag, Stuttgart. Thorington, R. W., Jr. 1968. Observations of squirrel monkeys in a Colombian Forest, p. 69-85. In L. A. Rosenblum and R. W. Cooper [ed.], The squirrel monkey. Academic Press, New York. Wclker, W. I. 1961. An analysis of exploratory and play behavior in animals, p. 175-226. In D. W. Fiske and S. R. Maddi [ed.], Functions of varied experience. Dorsey Press, Chicago. Winter, P. 1968. Social communication in the squirrel monkey, p. 235-253. In L. A. Rosenblum and R. W. Cooper [ed.], The squirrel monkey. Academic Press, New York.
© Copyright 2025 Paperzz