82 J. Indian Assoc. Child Adolesc. Ment. Health 2017; 13(2):82-106 Original Article Correlation between Reading and Spelling Performance of Egyptian Children with Reading Disorder and their Cognitive and Linguistic Abilities Neveen Nashaat, Haytham Hasan, Ayman Kilany, Ola Gebril, Suzette Helal, Ghada Orabi, Mostafa Gaballah, Ehab Abdelraouf Address for correspondence: Dr. Haytham Hasan, Psychological Medicine Hospital, 5 Ahmed Abdel Nabi St., Nozha El-Gedida, Cairo, Egypt. E-mail: [email protected] Abstract: Background: Proper cognitive and linguistic abilities have a basic role in reading development. However, the possible role of some cognitive and linguistic abilities in learning to read especially in Arabic language is still not well studied. Aims: This study investigated the correlation between the reading (decoding) and spelling performance of Egyptian Arabic speaking children with reading disorder and their different cognitive and linguistic abilities in order to determine the possible abilities that have essential role in reading development. Methods: The participants were 25 Egyptian Arabic-speaking children with reading disorder (age range: 6.5–10.7 years). The dyslexia assessment test, Illinois test of psycholinguistic abilities, phonological awareness test and the test of semantics were used for evaluating the participants’ aptitudes. Results: The disorders in the abilities reflecting rapid naming, semantics, phonological awareness deficits and one minute writing were the most common deficits noticed in the participants. The scores of reading (decoding) and spelling subtests were correlated with the phonological awareness scores. The spelling performance was further related to verbal working memory. 83 Although the rapid naming deficits were the most common among participants, no associations were found between them and the decoding or spelling abilities. Conclusions: Phonological awareness and verbal working memory have a role in reading development in Egyptian Arabic speaking children with reading disorder. The other examined writing, cognitive and linguistic deficits are most likely to be considered cooccurring disorders with the reading disorder rather than influential on reading. Keywords: reading disorder, cognition, Arabic language. Introduction The Reading disorder, which is also known as developmental dyslexia, is a multifactorial neurodevelopmental deficit which is characterized by persistent difficulty in learning to read in people with normal intelligence [1]. The human brain is not genetically ‘hardwired’ for the reading process. Therefore, proper cognitive and linguistic abilities have a basic role in reading development. Moreover, the phonological, semantics and syntactic development was suggested to be related to reading development in languages other than Arabic [2]. In addition to reading, oral language is suggested to influence spelling development [3]. However, the relation between some cognitive and linguistic abilities such as rapid automatized naming (RAN) and phoneme-grapheme correspondence and their possible role in learning to read is little uncertain [4]. Furthermore, the abilities related to reading development could vary between typically developing children and those who have reading or spelling disorders [5]. 84 The relation between language and reading was not adequately investigated in Arabic which is one of the Alphabetical languages. An alphabetic language is the one depending on alphabets to represent unique graphemes for the phonemes of that language. Furthermore, the written Arabic language (the language used for literacy) is different from the spoken Colloquial Egyptian Arabic which could make the literacy process challenging [6]. Moreover, the prevalence of reading / spelling disorder in Egypt is rising. It was estimated to be 12.6 % among third-grade children in 2014 while it was estimated to be 28/1000 in 1995 which necessitates better understanding of the related abilities influencing the reading and spelling development for better intervention [7, 8]. Besides, different orthographies were reported to influence reading measures and their correlation with linguistic abilities [9]. Therefore, the aim of this study was to determine the extent of deficits in the cognitive and psycholinguistic aptitudes of the Egyptian native Arabic-speaking children with a reading disorder and to investigate the associations between such abilities and the reading (decoding) and spelling performance of such children. Consequently, the possible abilities that have a role in reading development in Arabic will be identified. MATERIALS AND MEHODS Participants Forty-nine children (36 boys, 13 girls) with reading disorder participated in the present study. They were recruited from the Learning Disability clinic, Medical Research Centre of Excellence, National Research Centre, Cairo, Egypt. They were Egyptian native Arabic speaking children who were enrolled in the national education system. The 85 diagnosis of the reading disorder was based on the criteria of DSM-IV-TR [10]. Consents were obtained from the participants’ parents. Verbal assents were obtained from the participants. The study was approved by the medical research ethics committee of the National Research Centre, Cairo, Egypt. Exclusion criteria included presence of sensory deficits, below average intelligence quotient (IQ), children with EEG abnormalities and presence of comorbid neuropsychiatric disorders. Ten children with comorbid attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, eight children with IQ less than 90, three children with EEG abnormalities in their one hour EEG records, a child with high functioning autism, a child with neuromuscular disorder and a child with mild to moderate sensory neural hearing loss were excluded from the study. The remaining 25 children (18 boys, 7 girls; age range: 6.5–10.7 years; age mean 8.68 ±1.14) were enrolled in the study and were assessed using the Arabic dyslexia assessment test, the phonological awareness test, the test of semantics and the Illinois test of psycholinguistic abilities. Tools • Psychiatric examination using Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview for Children (M.I.N.I. Kid) which is a short structured diagnostic interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10 psychiatric disorders [11]. It was used to exclude other psychiatric comorbidities. The Arabic version was validated with good reliability and validity and it is used in many studies in Arab Countries [12]. 86 • IQ assessment using the Arabic version of Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scale, 4th Edition (SB-IV) to exclude children with below average IQ [13,14]. • The Arabic Dyslexia Assessment Test which has the following subtests: rapid naming, bead threading, one minute reading, postural stability, phonemic segmentation, two minute spelling, backward digit span, nonsense passage reading, one minute writing, verbal fluency and semantic fluency [15,16]. The one minute reading, nonsense passage reading, and two minute spelling subtests investigate the reading (decoding) and spelling abilities of the child. The rapid naming, verbal fluency and semantic fluency investigate the semantics ability of the child at word level which is a verbal linguistic function. The one minute writing is used for assessment of the speed and quality of writing. The phonemic segmentation is concerned with deleting syllables or phonemes and it is a phonological awareness function. The following subtests reflect some cognitive abilities and some related motor functions: backward digit span (verbal working memory), bead threading (fine motor coordination) and postural stability (gross motor control). • The Phonological Awareness (PA) test was performed to assess the word awareness, syllable awareness, rhyme awareness, phoneme awareness (at the beginning, the end and middle of the word), phoneme-grapheme correspondence and sound production ability. The total score of the test was used to determine the presence of deficits in phonological awareness which is a verbal linguistic function [17]. 87 • The Semantics test was used for assessment of semantics at word and sentence levels. The synonym, antonym and hyponym reflected the at-word level part. The at-sentence level was assessed by testing the ability to arrange 3-step or 4-step sequencing picture cards [18]. The semantic abilities are considered to be a part of verbal linguistic abilities. • The Illinois test of psycholinguistic abilities (the Arabic version) has the following subtests: auditory reception, visual reception, auditory association, visual association, verbal expression, manual expression, grammatic closure, visual closure, auditory sequential memory and visual sequential memory [19, 20]. The test is applied for obtaining raw scores for each child in each subtest. The raw scores of each subtest are converted to scaled scores according to the child’s mental age. The mean of the scaled scores of the child is calculated. The child’s performance in a subtest is considered delayed when both (a) and (b) occur: (a). the standard score of this subtest is less than the mean of the child’s standard scores. (b). the difference between the mean of standard scores and the standard score of this subtest was more than 6. The auditory association, verbal expression and grammatic closure are verbal linguistic functions while manual expression is a non-verbal linguistic function. The cognitive abilities include: auditory reception, visual reception, visual association, visual closure, auditory sequential memory, and visual sequential memory. However, an overlap between cognitive and linguistic abilities could occur as some cognitive abilities depend on language comprehension and vice versa. 88 Data analysis Data were analyzed using SPSS computer package version 17 (SPSS, Chicago, IL, USA). The percentage of participants manifested deficits in the reading, writing, spelling, cognitive and linguistic abilities was determined. The one minute reading, nonsense passage reading and the two minute spelling subtests of the dyslexia assessment test were the tests that represented the reading and spelling skills of the participants. Each one of these three subtests which represented reading and spelling skills was investigated for correlation with all the remaining subtests used for assessing different abilities of the participants. Spearman’s correlation coefficient was used to correlate various variables. When (p) for any item is less than 0.05, it is considered to be statistically significant. Results Percent of Participants with Defective Abilities in the Utilized Tests: Figures 1, 2 and 3 illustrate the percent of participants having deficits of the reading, spelling, writing, cognitive and linguistic abilities. All participants manifested rapid naming deficit which makes it the most common deficit manifested by the participants. Other common cognitive and verbal linguistic deficits that were not addressed to reading and spelling assessment included verbal fluency, phonemic segmentation, one minute writing, phonological awareness, semantics (especially semantic fluency, hyponyms and story sequencing), backward digit span, and auditory reception. None of the participants manifested manual expression or visual sequential memory deficits. Figure (1): The percent of participants who manifested deficits in the dyslexia assessment test Figure (2): The percent of participants who manifested deficits in the phonological awareness (PA) test and the semantics test 89 Figure (3): The percent of participants who manifested deficits in the Illinois test of psycholinguistic abilities Correlations: The correlations between the one minute reading subtest and the remaining other abilities of the participants revealed significant correlation only with the phonological awareness total scores (r=0.520, p=0.008). Other reading, writing, cognitive and linguistic abilities were not correlated with the one minute reading subtest. The correlation between nonsense passage reading subtest and the other abilities of the participants revealed significant correlation only with the two minute spelling (r=0.4, p=0.03). Other reading, writing, cognitive and linguistic abilities were not correlated with the nonsense passage reading subtest. The figures 4,5,6 and 7 illustrate the statistically significant correlation between the twominute spelling subtest and phonemic segmentation, backward digit span, verbal fluency 90 91 and phonological awareness total scores. Other reading, writing, cognitive and linguistic abilities did not show statistically significant correlation with the two-minute spelling subtest. Phonseg=phonemic segmentation twomspelling=two-minute spelling Figure (4): Statistically significant correlation between the two-minute spelling and phonemic segmentation BWDS=backward digit span twomspelling=two-minute spelling Figure (5): Statistically significant correlation between the two-minute spelling and backward digit span 92 Verbfluency=verbal fluency twomspelling=two-minute spelling Figure (6): Statistically significant correlation between the two-minute spelling and verbal fluency 93 PA=phonological awareness twomspelling=two-minute spelling Figure (7): Statistically significant correlation between the two-minute spelling and phonological awareness Discussion To the extent of the authors’ knowledge this is the first study that was addressed to investigate the possible relation of certain cognitive and linguistic abilities on the reading and spelling performance of the native Egyptian Arabic speaking children with the reading disorder (developmental dyslexia). This would contribute to identifying the possible abilities that have influential role in the reading development. Furthermore, identifying the percentage of deficits in these abilities will help to understand such a complicated disorder and underscore the size of the manifested disorders. 94 95 The manifested deficits The deficits of rapid automatized naming (RAN) were constantly manifested by the participants. This could be driven by the commonalities in the brain networks activated while performing reading and RAN tasks in normal individuals. These networks include supramarginal gyrus, middle temporal gyrus and cerebellum along with the left inferior frontal gyrus [21]. In addition, the one minute writing subtest was defective in most of the participants. The most common noticed abnormality among participants was the slow speed copying of the required text. This could be attributed to the presence of similar graphemes in Arabic language. These graphemes differ in the number and position of dots, such as, the graphemes (ث، )تwhich represents the phonemes / تt/ and / ثᶿ /. They also differ according to their position in the word (e.g ، ت ــ،) ــت ـ تــ. These criteria together with their reading disorder make the children rely on their visual processing abilities. Common co-occurrence among the reading and writing disorders has been reported in other languages [22]. Yet, the percentage of this co-existence in Arabic was more than other languages [23] which suggests a language specific difficulty in the writing performance in Arabic. Both auditory and visual deficits were noticed among the participants with more frequent auditory abilities observed. These outputs are in agreement with Gabrieli et al. [24] who reported common auditory deficits among dyslexic children. These deficits were explained by problems in rapid auditory processing, visual attention and visual magnocellular system [24, 25, 26]. Furthermore, syntactic (grammatical) abnormalities were observed among some participants. These abnormalities could be explained by 96 changes in the temporoparietal language areas on the two brain sides. They were reported to be symmetrical in dyslexic individuals without the normal left-sided advantage [26]. The correlation outputs The reading (decoding) ability was only correlated with the phonological awareness abilities of the participants in this study. Such relation was also reported in Portuguese which is an alphabetic language [27]. Moreover, low performance in phonological awareness was reported in children with reading disability and was related to their reading development [28]. The phonological awareness tasks that involved printed letters or words were related to superior temporal gyrus, middle temporal gyrus, fusiform gyrus and left inferior frontal gyrus. On the other hand, the phonological awareness tasks that involved auditory activities such as rhyming were related to left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Reduced activation of these areas was noticed in dyslexic children [29]. Friend & Olson [30] reported that poor spelling performance was correlated with poor phonological awareness abilities, which is consistent with the present study. The spelling abilities for the participants in this study were also correlated with backward digit span representing verbal working memory (WM) (p=0.006). Beneventi et al. [31] reported that the verbal WM deficits were attributed to abnormalities of the prefrontal cortex, the superior parietal cortex and their inter-connections. Additionally, they reported that the precentral gyrus was related to phonological processing disorders in the children with reading disorder as elaborated by functional magnetic resonance imaging. Consequently, they suggested that both a phonological deficits and a distinct deficit in 97 WM could underlie working memory abnormalities in children with reading disorder. Thus, disorders in phonological processing seem to explain reading, spelling and working memory deficits in reading disorder and highlight its role in reading development. The spelling abilities further showed statistically significant correlation with the nonsense passage reading which confirms the mutual relation between these abilities. The associations between the two are in agreement with Mohamed et al. [7]. They reported that reading disorder is mostly associated with the spelling disorder. The spelling abilities were also correlated with verbal fluency in the present study (p=0.003). Verbal fluency seems to be more related to vocabulary deficit (higher order process disorder). The relation between spelling performance and verbal fluency were previously attributed to a semantic orthographic connection deficit [3]. However, referring this correlation output to a vocabulary deficit should be taken with caution for two reasons. First, verbal fluency in the present study was assessed by asking the child to say as many words starting with /s/ as he/she can in a minute. Some participants produced words not commencing with /s/ but with a similar one (e.g./ᶿ/or /ŝ/). Others mentioned words with the targeted phoneme within the word or at its end (i.e. not starting with it). Hence, a phonological awareness deficit could have hindered their ability to perform this task and could explain the correlation output that was observed. The second reason is that there was no correlation detected between spelling abilities and other tests reflecting vocabulary deficit or word finding difficulty including the rapid naming test which was defective in all participants. 98 The absence of correlation between rapid naming scores and the reading and spelling abilities of the participants could be attributed to the difference in origin of these abilities. Decoding deficit is one of the lower order process disorders while the RAN is related to vocabulary deficit which is a higher order process disorder [32]. Moreover, other higher order process language-based disorders manifested by the participants such as semantic and syntactic disorders (semantic fluency, synonyms, antonyms, hyponyms, sequencing, auditory association and grammatical closure) did not show correlation with their reading or spelling abilities. Therefore, these abilities could be considered as coexisting deficits with the reading disorder. It is worth mentioning that the relative importance of phonological awareness and RAN as the basis of reading and spelling abilities would differ between different languages i.e. non-alphabetic languages and English in comparison to languages whose orthographies have more consistent letter-sound associations [33, 34]. Unlike English, Arabic language mostly has consistent phoneme-grapheme correspondence. There are few exceptions such as Shadda /ﱢ/ (a diacritic mark) and Lam Shamsiya (a definite article which proceeds words starting with certain letters) [35, 36]. Therefore, the role of phonological awareness is most likely to have the basic influence on reading development in Arabic. In accordance with the present study, Vaessen et al. [37] and Ziegler et al. [38] highlighted the role of phonological awareness in reading performance and limit the role of rapid naming in the reading abilities. On the contrary, the significant correlation outputs of this study disagree with Wimmer et al. [39] who argued that in consistent orthographies RAN is the predominant predictor of variations in reading 99 ability whereas phoneme awareness and letter–sound knowledge are much less important. A possible explanation for this disagreement is that their notion stemmed from longitudinal studies on German and English which are less transparent than Arabic. In addition, our interpretations disagree with Georgiou et al. [40] who reported that neither RAN nor phonological awareness was related to reading development in Greek which has consistent orthography. Conclusions: The decoding and spelling abilities are strongly related to the phonological awareness performance in Egyptian native Arabic-speaking children with reading disorder. Moreover, verbal working memory has a role in their spelling performance. Accordingly, the phonological awareness and the verbal working memory are suggested to have an essential role in the reading development in Arabic. Other cognitive, writing and linguistic deficits could be considered associations or co-occurring disorders with the reading disorder rather than to have an influential role. Nevertheless, deficits in these abilities should be screened among children with reading disorder and should be targeted in the remediation plan for such children. Conflict of Interest: The authors declare that they have no competing interests. References 1. Hosseini SM, Black JM, Soriano T, Bugescu N, Martinez R, Raman MM, et al.: Topological properties of large-scale structural brain networks in children with familial risk for reading difficulties. Neuroimage 2013, 71: 260-274. 2. Lundberg I: The child's route into reading and what can go wrong. Dyslexia 2002, 8: 1-13. 100 3. McCarthy JH, Hogan TP, Catts HW: Is weak oral language associated with poor spelling in school-age children with specific language impairment, dyslexia or both? Clin HYPERLINK "http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/?term=Is+weak+oral+language+associated+w ith+poor+spelling+in+schoolage+children+with+specific+language+impairment%2C+dyslexia+or+both" Linguist HYPERLINK "http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/?term=Is+weak+oral+language+associated+w ith+poor+spelling+in+schoolage+children+with+specific+language+impairment%2C+dyslexia+or+both"Phon 2012, 26: 791-805. 4. Hulme C, Snowling MJ: The interface between spoken and written language: developmental disorders. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 2013, 369(1634): 20120395. 5. Glass L, Graham DM, Akshoomoff N, Mattson SN: Cognitive factors contributing to spelling performance in children with prenatal alcohol exposure. Neuropsychology 2015, 29(6):817-28. 6. Farran LK, Bingham GE, Matthews MW: The relationship between language and reading in bilingual English-Arabic children in Egypt. Reading and Writing 2012, 25 (9): 2153–2181. 101 7. Mohamed W, Landerl K, Elbert T: An Epidemiological Survey of Specific Reading and Spelling Disabilities in Arabic Speaking Children in Egypt. In Handbook of Arabic Literacy: Insights and Perspectives. Edited by Saiegh-Haddad E, Joshi RM. Springer Netherlands; 2014:99-117. [Joshi RM (Series Editor): Literacy Studies, vol 9.] 8. Farrag AF, Shaker H, Hamdy NA, Wafaa MA: Clinical characteristics of population of dyslexic children in Assiut, Egypt. Neuroepidemiology 1995, 14(2):92-99. 9. Ibrahim R, Eviatar Z, Aharon-Peretz J: Metalinguistic Awareness and Reading Performance: A Cross Language Comparison. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 2007, 36 (4): 297–317. 10. American Psychiatric Association: Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. (4th edition Text revised). Washington DC, American Psychiatric Association; 2000. 11. Sheehan DV, Janavs J: Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview for Children / adolescents (M.I.N.I. Kid). Tampa, University of South Florida, College of Medicine; 1998. 12. Ghanem MH, Ibrahim M, El-Behairy AA, El Merghany H: Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview for Children / adolescents (M.I.N.I. Kid), Arabic version. (1st edition). Cairo, Ain-Shams University, Institute of Psychiatry; 2000. 102 13. Thorndike RL, Hagen EP, Sattler JM: Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scale. (4th Edition). Chicago, Riverside Publishing; 1986. 14. Melika L: Stanford Binet Intelligence Scale (4th Arabic version). (2nd edition). Cairo, Victor Kiorlos Publishing; 1998. 15. Fawcett AJ, Nicolson RI: Dyslexia assessment test. New York, The Psychological Corporation, A Harcourt Brace & Co. Ltd; 1996 16. Aboras Y, Abdou R, Kozou H: Development of an Arabic test for assessment of dyslexia in Egyptian children. Bull Alexandria Fac 2008, 44: 653-662. 17. El-Sady S, El-Shoubary A, El-Assal N, Abou-Elsaad T, Afsah O: Development of a screening test battery for assessing phonological awareness in Arabic-speaking children in the early elementary grades. Ain Shams Med J 2011, 62: 95-103. 18. El-Sady S, El-Shoubary A, El-Assal N, Abou-Elsaad T, Afsah O: The Efficacy of A Language-based Training Program as a Phoniatric Approach for Remediation of Egyptian Learning-Disabled Children. PhD thesis. Ain –Shams University, Department of Otorhinolaryngology, Phoniatrics unit; 2011. 19. Kirk S, McCarthy J, Kirk W: The Illinois test of psycholinguistic abilities. Urbana, IL, University of Illinois Press; 1986. 20. El-Sady S, El-Shobary A, Rifaie N, Azzam A: Illinois test of psycholinguistic abilities. Master thesis. Ain –Shams University, Department of Otorhinolaryngology, Phoniatrics unit; 1996. 103 21. Cummine J, Chouinard B, Szepesvari E, Georgiou GK: An examination of the rapid automatized naming-reading relationship using functional magnetic resonance imaging. Neuroscience 2015, 305: 49-66. 22. Gütinger A, Smith E, Reiter R, Brosch S: Reading and writing disorder (RWD) criteria for a speech therapist intervention. Laryngorhinootologie 2014, 93(6):385-91. 23. Ramaa S, Gowramma IP: A systematic procedure for identifying and classifying children with dyscalculia among primary school children in India. Dyslexia 2002, 8(2):67-85. 24. Gabrieli JD, Norton ES: Reading abilities: importance of visual-spatial attention. Curr Biol. 2012, 22(9):R298-9. 25. Lorusso ML, Cantiani C, Molteni M: Age, dyslexia subtype and comorbidity modulate rapid auditory processing in developmental dyslexia. Front Hum Neurosci. 2014, 8:313. 26. Stein J: The magnocellular theory of developmental dyslexia. Dyslexia 2001, 7: 12– 36. 27. Moura O, Moreno J, Pereira M, Simões MR: Developmental dyslexia and phonological processing in European Portuguese orthography. Dyslexia 2015, 21: 6079. 104 28. Vellutino FR, Fletcher JM, Snowling MJ, Scanlon DM: Specific reading disability (dyslexia): what have we learned in the past four decades? Journal of child psychology and psychiatry and allied disciplines (2004), 45: 2-40. 29. Kovelman I, Norton ES, Christodoulou JA, Gaab N, Lieberman DA, Triantafyllou C et al.: Brain basis of phonological awareness for spoken language in children and its disruption in dyslexia. Cereb Cortex 2012, 22(4):754-64. 30. Friend A, Olson RK: Phonological spelling and reading deficits in children with spelling disabilities. Sci Stud Read 2010, 12: 90–105. 31. Beneventi H, Tønnessen FE, Ersland L: Dyslexic children show short-term memory deficits in phonological storage and serial rehearsal: an fMRI study. Int J Neurosci 2009, 119: 2017-2043. 32. Supple MD: Dyslexia: oral and written language disorder. A new look at old links. Folia HYPERLINK HYPERLINK "http://www.karger.com/Journal/Home/224177"Phoniatr "http://www.karger.com/Journal/Home/224177" "http://www.karger.com/Journal/Home/224177"Logop "http://www.karger.com/Journal/Home/224177" 2000, 52: 7-13. HYPERLINK HYPERLINK 105 33. Caravolas M, Lervåg A, Mousikou P, Efrim C, Litavsky M, Onochie-Quintanilla E, et al.: Common patterns of prediction of literacy development in different alphabetic orthographies. Psychol HYPERLINK "http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/?term=Common+patterns+of+prediction+of+l iteracy+development+in+different+alphabetic+orthographies.+Psychological+science " HYPERLINK "http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/?term=Common+patterns+of+prediction+of+l iteracy+development+in+different+alphabetic+orthographies.+Psychological+science "Sci 2012, 23: 678-686. 34. Hulme C, Snowling MJ: Learning to read: what we know and what we need to understand better. Child Dev Perspect 2015, 7: 1-5. 35. Deitsch A, Czarnecki D: JAVA internationalization. (1st edition). CA: O’Reilly and Associates, Inc; 2001. 36. Abdur Rahim V: Madina Book1 English Key. Arabic Language Course Books. Canada, Toronto, the Institute of the Language of the Qur'an; 2009. 37. Vaessen A, Bertrand D, Tóth D, Cse´pe V, Faı´sca L, Reis A: Cognitive development of fluent word reading does not qualitatively differ between transparent and opaque orthographies. J Educ Psychol 2010, 102: 827–842. 106 38. Ziegler JC, Bertrand D, Tóth D, Csépe V, Reis A, Faísca L: Orthographic depth and its impact on universal predictors of reading: A cross-language investigation. Psychol Sci HYPERLINK "http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/?term=Orthographic+depth+and+its+impact+ on+universal+predictors+of+reading%3A+A+cross-language+investigation" 2010, 21(4): 551–559. 39. Wimmer H, Mayringer H, Landerl K: The double-deficit hypothesis and difficulties in learning to read a regular orthography. J Educ Psychol 2000, 92: 668–680. 40. Georgiou GK, Parilla R, Papadopoulos TC: Predictors of word decoding and reading fluency across languages varying in orthographic consistency. J Educ Psychol 2008, 100: 566-580. Neveen Nashaat, Doctoral degree, researcher of phoniatrics; Haytham Hasan, Doctoral degree, researcher of psychiatry; Ayman Kilany, Doctoral degree, researcher of neurology; Ola Gebril, Assistant professor of human genetics; Suzette Helal, Assistant professor of neurology; Ghada Orabi, M.Sc. degree, assistant researcher of phoniatrics; Mostafa Gaballah, M.Sc. degree, assistant researcher of pediatrics; Ehab Abdelraouf, Professor of pediatrics, head of research on children with special needs department, Psychological Medicine Hospital, 5 Ahmed Abdel Nabi St., Nozha El-Gedida, Cairo, Egypt.
© Copyright 2025 Paperzz