Beetle Horn Dimorphism: Making the Best of a Bad Lot Author(s): William G. Eberhard Source: The American Naturalist, Vol. 119, No. 3 (Mar., 1982), pp. 420-426 Published by: The University of Chicago Press for The American Society of Naturalists Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/2460939 . Accessed: 05/09/2011 03:11 Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at . http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact [email protected]. The University of Chicago Press and The American Society of Naturalists are collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to The American Naturalist. http://www.jstor.org 420 BEETLE THE AMERICAN NATURALIST HORN DIMORPHISM: MAKING THE BEST OF A BAD LOT There has been interestrecentlyin the occurrencein a single species of more than one adaptive behavior or morphology.One kind of explanationfor dimorphismsof thistypeis thattheyconstitutefrequency-dependent evolutionarystable strategies(Gadgil 1972; Maynard Smith 1976), withthe reproductivepayoffsfor one typeequal to those fortheotherbecause of the balances of costs and benefits forthe two forms,and thattheyare thus maintainedin a sortof balanced genetic polymorphism.A second possibilityis thatpayoffsforthetwo formsare notequal but thatthe alternativeformsrepresentfacultativealternativesdesignedto funccircumstances(different tionin different body size, season, etc.; see Warneret al. 1975; West-Eberhard1979; Dawkins 1980 discusses both typesof explanation). A classic example of such a dimorphismoccurs in some species of homed beetles in which thereare two more or less distinctforms:large individualswith well-developed horns ("majors"), and smaller ones which lack or have only poorly developed horns ("minors") (Arrow 1951). Evidence is accumulating (Eberhard 1977a; Palmer 1978; Eberhard 1979and references;Eberhard 1980; M. Peinador, in prep.) that beetle horns commonlyfunctionas weapons in intraspecificbattles. The fact that the horns of many species representsubstantial proportionsof the beetles' body weightsargues that natural selection in these species has stronglyfavoredincreased fighting ability.Since beetles do not grow aftertheirlast moult(and minorsare thuscomdemnedto remainminorsfortheir entireadult lives), the continuedexistence of hornlessor nearlyhornlessforms seems paradoxical since one mightthinkthat selection should eliminatethose individualspoorly equipped to fight. This studyof the dimorphichorneddynastinebeetle Podischnus agenor Oliver shows that the behavior and ecology of minormales differsfromthat of major males in ways which suggest that the minor formis a facultativeadaptation designed to reduce directcompetitionwithmajor males, and thus to compensate partiallyforthecompetitivedisadvantageresultingfromtheminors'smallersize. THE BEETLE Larval Podischnus agenor live undergroundand feed on humus in the soil, while adults emergeabovegroundin the rainyseason (Septemberto December in Cali, Colombia, where this study was conducted) to burrow in thick-stemmed grasses such as sugar cane and corn where theyfeed and mate (Guagliumi1962; Eberhard 1977b). Laboratoryrearingdata (Guagliumi1962) indicatethatthe life cycle lasts about a year, and field observations (Eberhard 1977b) show that females become common in sugar cane stalks about 1-3 wk aftermales have begun to appear abovegroundin large numbers. Only adult males have horns, and they use them as clamps and levers in Am. Nat. 1982. Vol. 119, pp. 420-426. ? 1982 by The Universityof Chicago. 0003-0147/82/1903-0011$02.00. All rightsreserved. NOTES AND COMMENTS 421 complex fightsforpossession of burrowsin cane stalks (Eberhard 1977b, 1979). Larger males consistentlydefeatsmallerones. Large males have well-developed hornson bothhead and prothorax,but smallones have relativelyshorthead horns and only vestiges of thoracichorns. These differencesin horn developmentare not simply extremes of a smooth gradient,but rather representtwo distinct morphs,as shown by the bimodal distribution of head hornlengthsin the population and the sigmoid relation between head horn length and elytralwidth (a convenientmeasure of body size; fig. 1). Behavioral Differencesbetween the Morphs Podischnus agenor obviouslyevolved in an environmentdifferent fromthatof today in whichthe beetles findhuge plantationsof food plantsavailable. It is thus not appropriateto measure the present day reproductivesuccesses of the two formsand then make subtle interpretations of theirevolutionaryorigins. It is, however,valid to look forbehavioraldifferencesbetweentheforms,assume that these differencesmust have been adaptive when they evolved, and then draw formsmay have arisen. inferencesabout how and why the different By markingmales with numbersscratchedon theirelytraand thenfollowing theiractivitiesin selected portionsof cane fieldsthroughoutthe approximately 3-mo season of maximumadult activityfortwo successive years,I foundthatthe behavior of minorsdifferedin two importantways-fromthat of majors. First, minorswere active earlierin the season than majors (fig.2); they constituteda substantialpartof the male populationwhen femaleswere stilluncommon,then later virtuallydisappeared. In 1976 but not 1977 they reappeared in reduced numberstowardthe end of the season. It is not knownwhetherthe smallermales' earlieremergenceswere simplythe resultof theirhaving shorterdevelopmental times or whetherother mechanismsare involved; the latterseems more likely since smallerfemalesdid not appear earlierthanlargerones. The importantpoint is that earlier emergence could reduce the probabilityof direct conflictwith majors. In addition,as shown in table 1, minorswere less oftenrecapturedin tunnels other than those in which they were marked (the beetles' burrowingactivities usuallycaused the stalkto die, and burrowswere inhabitedforan average of only 4.3 days, N = 322). This was truedespitethefactthatproportionally moreminors were marked early in the season each year and thus were more available for recapture.This differencein recapturerates could conceivablyresultfromminor males having eitherlower survivorshipor greatertendencyto disperse. Direct data on longevityin the field are not available, and of course are extremely difficult to obtainin mobile species such as thisone, but thereare indicationsthat thedifferences in recaptureratesprobablyresultedat least partlyfromdifferences in dispersal tendencies. Most recapturesof both major and minor males were of individuals which had been found only a short time before. In 1976, for example, 56% of the 57 beetles which were recapturedhad firstbeen seen and marked less than 8 days previously. Minor males kept under very unfavorable conditions(capturedlate in the season and kept withoutfood in moistenedfrass 422 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST A N= 625 5 OF HE.40H00] LENGTH 10 B N = 625 5 Cl- ~ ~ ~ Cu WIDTHOF ELYTRA .5 . -.6 ,b U-~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~C:1 = I.2. .7 I~~~~~~~~~~ .3 MAXIMlUM 119TH1flF[LYTPA FIG. 1.-A, Distribution ofheadhornlengths; ofelytrawidths(an indicator B, distribution ofbodysize); andC, relationship betweenelytrawidthandheadhornlength inPodischnus agenor. NOTES AND COMMENTS 423 1976 5X- lOX(N = 74) I Ir I Jik 4lK _i_ 14X-(N76) I1o- 6XI- l (N 99 r.--I-,r 16X- 23X(N 94) 9X r*71-{f{1I11 6X1 19X1(N =122) 28I .3 II 3XI - 11XL (N = 91) 22XI- 27XII (N = 127) .2 F 5 -IOX o (N =77) 15X- 25X(N=117) 26X -3X1 1977 15VIII- 301X(N = 132) .4 _ 14XI (N = 78) HTF1I 1-- OF HEADHORN LENGTH .5 .6 Fl f rnm4ThEm .2 .3 .4 .5 .6 LENGTH OF HEADHORN during the headhornlengths numbers ofmaleswithdifferent FIG. 2.-Changes inrelative earlyintheseasonin malesweremorecommon rainyseasonsof1976and 1977.Small-horned bothyears. fromburrowentrances)lived on the average morethana week (8.5 days,N = 22). Underbetterconditions(capturedearlyin theseason and keptwithabundantfood) they lived for an average of nearly 2 mo and their longevitydid not differ fromthatof majors (P > .4, Mann-WhitneyU test). Thus the more significantly than eightfolddifferencein recapturerates was probablynot due exclusivelyto senescence differences;even under extraordinarilydifficultnutritionalcircumstances in captivity,old minorslived on the average longerthanthe medianlapse of time between firstsightingand recapture. It is still possible that the differencesin recapture rates could be due to differencesin predationrates on the two forms,althoughthere is no reason to expect this. I saw only scattered,occasional evidence of predationin the field (small accumulationsof elytraand hardbody parts)duringseveral hundredhours ofobservationbothat nightand duringtheday. There was no signofpredationon beetles in theirburrowsin the cane. 424 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST TABLE 1 RECAPTURESOF MALES OF DIFFERENT SIZES (largermales recapturedmore often) Major (Head Horn > 1.17 cm) 1976 .......... 1977 .......... Mean ......... NOTE.-N 24%(N = 289) I O%(N = 296) 17%(N = 585) Intermediate (1.17 ? Head Horn ? .56 cm) Minor (Head Horn < .56 cm) 8%(N = 95) 9%(N = 114) 9%(N = 209) 4%(N = 75) O%(N = 47) 2%(N = 122) = total numbersof individualsmarked. The probable differencesin emergencetimesand dispersalmay representways in which minormales reduce directcompetitionwith majors, by avoidingthem both in timeand in space. Both behaviors would minimizethe minors'disadvantages as poor fighterswhich resultfromtheirsmall size. Maintainingthe Dimorphism For the complex behavioraland morphologicaldimorphismof thisspecies to be maintainedby genetic differencesbetween morphsthere would have to be (1) linkageamong genes forhorn morphology,body size, emergencetime,and dispersal tendency,and (2) absence of environmentaleffects,especially larval nutrition, on adult size. This combinationof conditionsseems veryunlikely.A large numberof genes must be involved in determiningthese characters,and larval nutritionis known to influenceadult size in many insects (Wigglesworth1965). Beetle horn developmentis rigidlyassociated withextremesof adult size; there are no small "majors" nor large "minors" known. Furthermore,size-related facultativepolymorphismsare well known in insects. Numerous well-studied examples occur in the social Hymenopterain whichthe extensivebehavioraland morphologicaldifferencesbetween workers and queens are determinedby differencesin quantityand qualityof larval food (Brian 1979). Similar size-related behavioralpolymorphismsare also knownin solitaryaculeate Hymenoptera(Alcock 1979). Finally,experimentalmanipulationof the nutritionof nymphsof the dimorphicearwigForficulaauricularia showed thatthe amountof food consumed determinedthe adult morphadopted (Kuhl 1928). There is an additional,theoreticalreason forexpectingdimorphismslike thatof P. agenor to be facultative.If a characterdepends on or is stronglyinfluencedby and larger body size for its effectivefunctioning(e.g., horns serve for fighting, theneven ifthedimorphismoriginally males are strongerand thusbetterfighters), arises as a geneticpolymorphismtherewill be selection forthe abilityto switch forms on the basis of body size. This argumentapplies equally well to the possibilitythatindividualscan choose eithermorphbut make the choice in some probabilisticway withoutregardto theirown particularcircumstances("mixed ESS" strategy;see Dawkins 1980). In effect,males whichcan choose the alternative behavior and morphologymost appropriatefor their body sizes will be superiorto those condemnedto eitherof the alternativesirrespectiveofbody size (similarargumentsare givenin West-Eberhard1979; Cade 1979a; Dawkins 1980). NOTES AND COMMENTS 425 DISCUSSION The followingsequence of events could lead to the evolutionof behavioraland to body size (or otherparameterssuch structuraldimorphismslinkedfunctionally as age, season, etc.). (1) Under conditionsof intenseintraspecificcompetition, natural selection could favor "alternative behavior" (e.g., early emergence). Alleles for this behavior mightspread eitherby the kind of ESS envisioned by Gadgil (1972) or, if theirexpression was conditionedon appropriatecues (e.g., body size), by the selectiondescribedabove. In thefirstcase selectionwould then favorany allele which repressedthe alternativebehavior in appropriatecircumstances. (2) As such a "switch" became established,individualsnotcarryingboth the switchand alternativebehavioralleles would be selected against.At thispoint alternativeforms(e.g., small males) would have a fitnessless than that of the others,but the differencewould be less thanit was originally.(3) Selection would favor alleles which furtherrefinedthe effectsof both the switchand alternative would become morepronouncedand behavioralleles. (Phenotypicmanifestations more consistentlyassociated withappropriatecues, e.g., body size; bothcharacters would often become multi-allelic.) It would also favor morphological modificationsappropriateto the alternativebehavior (e.g., reductionof hornsin small individuals).These changes would furtherreduce the differencesbetween the fitnessesof the two alternativeformsbut stillnot necessarilymake themequal. This scheme contrastswith several discussions of polymorphismsin insects (Gadgil 1972; Alcock 1979; Cade 1979a, 1979b, Brockman 1979; Brockmanet al. even thoughtheir 1979) in thatalternativemorphscould be maintainedindefinitely reproductivesuccesses were not equal to those of the originalforms. AlthoughP. agenor shows structuralas well as behavioral componentsof its dimorphism,it can clearlybe added to the growinglistof species withalternative behavioralstrategiessuch as satellitemales, etc. (e.g., Warneret al. 1975; Emlen 1976 and references;Hamilton 1979; Cade 1979b; Alcock 1979; Thornhill1979). The acquisition of satellitemorphologyas well as satellitebehavior such as has happened in P. agenor is probablyparticularlylikelyin holometabolousinsects since theiradult size is fixedonce theymatureand is determinedlargelyby larval size priorto adulthood.A switchmechanismsensitiveto larval size could thusbe positivelyselected undera long historyof intensemale-malereproductivecompetition. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS I thank Mario Carpena for facilitatingwork at Ingenio Melendez, and M. J. West-Eberhardforcommentson the manuscript.Financial supportwas provided by the Comite de Investigaciones,Universidad del Valle, Cali, Colombia. LITERATURE CITED diversityin male reproductivestrategiesin some bees Alcock, J. 1979. The evolutionof intraspecific and wasps. Pages 381-402 in M. Blum and N. Blum, eds. Sexual selectionand reproductive competitionin insects. Academic Press, New York. 426 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST Arrow, G. H. 1951. Homed beetles. Junk,The Hague. and division of labor. Pages 121-222 in H. Hermann, ed. Brian, M. V. 1979. Caste differentiation Social insects. Vol. 1. Academic Press, New York. Brockman, H. J. 1979. Diversityin the nestingbehavior of mud-daubers(Trypoxylonpolitum Say; Sphecidae). Fla. Entomol. 63(l):53-64. Brockman,H. J., A. Grafen,and R. Dawkins. 1979. Evolutionarilystable nestingstrategyin a digger wasp. J. Theor. Biol. 77:473-496. Cade, W. 1979a. Alternativemale reproductivebehaviors. Fla. Entomol. 63(1):30-45. 1979b. The evolution of alternativemale reproductivestrategiesin field crickets. Pages 343-380 in M. Blum and N. Blum, eds. Sexual selection and reproductivecompetitionin insects. Academic Press, New York. Dawkins, R. 1980. Good strategyor evolutionarilystrategy?Pages 231-249 in G. W. Barlow and J. Westview, Boulder, Colo. Silverberg,eds. Sociobiology: beyond nature/nurture? Eberhard,W. G. 1977a. Fightingbehaviorof male Golofa porteribeetles (Scarabeidae: Dynastinae). Psyche 83(3-4):292-298. 1977b. La ecologia y comportamientodel cucarr6n de la cafia Podischnus agenor (Scarabeidae; Dynastinae). Rev. Col. Entomol. 3(1-2):17-21. 1979. The functionof horns in Podischnus agenor (Dynastinae) and other beetles. Pages 231-258 in M. Blum and N. Blum, eds. Sexual selection and reproductivecompetitionin insects. Academic Press, New York. 1980. Horned beetles. Sci. Am. 242(3):166-182. Emlen, S. T. 1976. Lek organizationand matingstrategiesin the bullfrog.Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 1:283-313. Gadgil, M. 1972. Male dimorphismas a consequence of sexual selection. Am. Nat. 106:574-580. Guagliumi,P. 1962. Las plagas de la cafia de azucar en Venezuela. Centro Invest. Agron. Maracay Monogr. no. 2, tomo 1. males in figwasps and otherinsects. Pages 167-220 in M. Hamilton,W. D. 1979. Winglessand fighting Blum and N. Blum, eds. Sexual selectionand reproductivecompetitionin insects. Academic Press, New York. Kuhl, W. 1928. Die Variabilitatder abdominalKorperanhangevon Forficulaauricularia, L. Z. Morph Oekol. Tiere 12:299-532. Maynard Smith,J. 1976. Evolution and the theoryof games. Am. Sci. 64:41-45. Palmer, T. J. 1978. A hornedbeetle which fights.Nature 274:583-584. Thornhill,R. 1979. Competitive,charmingmales and choosy females: was Darwin correct? Fla. Entomol. 63(1):5-30. Warner,R., R. Robertson,and E. G. Leigh. 1975. Sex change and sexual selection. Science 190:633638. West-Eberhard,M. J. 1979. Sexual selection, social competition,and evolution. Proc. Am. Philos. Soc. 123(4):222-234. Wigglesworth,V. 1965. Insect physiology.Methuen, London. WILLIAM SMITHSONIAN TROPICAL RESEARCH INSTITUTE ESCUELA DE BIOLOGIA DE COSTA RICA UNIVERSIDAD CIUDAD UNIVERSITARIA, COSTA RICA SubmittedOctober 6, 1980; Accepted June 18, 1981 G. EBERHARD
© Copyright 2025 Paperzz