Published October 1, 1968 Stimulation of the LabeUar Sugar Receptor of the Fleshfly by Mono- and Disaccharides H I R O M I C H I M O R I T A and AKIO SHIRAISHI From the Department of Biology,Faculty of Science, Kyushu University,Fukuoka,Japan. Dr. Shiraishi's present address is Willis H. Booth Computing Center, California Institute of Technology,Pasadena, California91109 INTRODUCTION Among the taste receptors, the sugar receptor has the unique property t h a t it is stimulated by uncharged molecules. This is quite remarkable, since almost all types of structures on the cell m e m b r a n e (muscle end plates, neuron synapses, etc., as well as salt taste receptors) are stimulated only by charged molecules or ions. H o w can uncharged molecules produce electrical changes in t h e receptor membrane? This is one of the most interesting problems in m e m b r a n e physiology. One approach to this problem is to study the relative stimulating effectiveness of different sugars and to evaluate the relation between the molecular structure of the sugar and its stimulating effectiveness. T h e final goal of this approach would be to discover the molecular structure of the site on the receptor m e m b r a n e with which sugar molecules combine. In fact, m u c h work thus oriented has been done in both vertebrates and invertebrates. These studies have been based on two groups of experiments. One estimates the threshold concentration of sugars for a definite behavioral response, including psychophysical experiments in man. Accordingly, the 559 The Journal of General Physiology Downloaded from on June 14, 2017 ABSTRACT Responses of the labellar sugar receptor of the fleshfly, Boettcherisca peregrina, were studied over a wide range of concentrations of several sugars (sucrose, maltose, glucose, fructose, and mannose) in single solutions and in mixtures. The results suggest (a) that the receptor sites are not completely differentiated for glucose and for fructose combination, (b) that the receptor site is composed of two subunits. Such suggestions are based on the classical model, where the response is proportional to the number of the sites, two subunits of each site being simultaneously occupied with one molecule of disaccharides or two molecules of monosaccharides. It is shown, however, that an allosteric model gives a somewhat better interpretation of the experimental results. Published October 1, 1968 560 THE JOURNAL OF GENERAL PHYSIOLOGY • VOLUME 52 " 1968 Downloaded from on June 14, 2017 sugars are tested at their very dilute concentrations. T h e other compares magnitudes of neural response of the receptor to a fixed concentration of different sugars. However, the order of effectiveness is not necessarily the same at different concentrations. Therefore, the effectiveness must be compared by recording the response of single receptors to sugars over a wide range of concentrations. T h a t is, we have to investigate the response magnitude of the receptor vs. the concentration of each sugar. Such a study will give us another aspect of information; i.e., the mode of complex formation between the receptor site and the sugar molecules. T h e labellar sugar receptor of m a n y species of flies is the best material for quantitative studies. First, we can easily record its sensory activities from the sidewall of the labellar chemosensory hair (Morita, 1959). Second, it has been ascertained in this receptor that the receptor potential can be considered proportional to the impulse frequency (Morita and Yamashita, 1966). This implies that we can quantitatively, though not in their absolute values, discuss the displacement of the receptor m e m b r a n e potential and the receptor m e m b r a n e current by measuring the impulse frequency, since they are also proportional to the recorded receptor potential. Third, there is a phase which can be regarded as stationary in the sensory adaptation curve of the impulse (in the blowfly, Steinhardt, Morita, and Hodgson, 1966). We can deal with the response magnitude in this phase in the same way as with the rate of enzyme reaction, where a steady state is assumed in the process of formation of the enzyme-substrate complex. Last, but not least, we can obtain quantitatively reproducible responses in this receptor if the stimulus duration is kept below 0.5 sec (Steinhardt et al., 1966). This method of short stimulation was introduced by Evans and Mellon, and was applied successfully by t h e m to the labellar water receptor of the blowfly (1962 a) and to the salt receptor (1962 b). Morita, Hidaka, and Shiraishi (1966) showed that the results obtained in the sugar receptor of the fleshfly could be explained by assuming that the response magnitude is proportional to the n u m b e r of the receptor sites, each of which is occupied by one molecule of sucrose. Such an assumption is the basis of Beidler's taste theory (Beidler, 1954), and was found to hold for the salt receptor of the rat (Beidler, 1954) and the labellar salt receptor (Evans and Mellon, 1962 b; but see Gillary, 1966). As to the receptor site, Evans (1963) has claimed that there are at least two different types in a single sugar receptor of the blowfly, one being the glucose-combining site and the other the fructose-combining one. In the present work we have tried to clarify the properties of the receptor site of the labe]lar sugar receptor of the fleshfly, investigating the responses to solutions of sugars and mixtures of different sugars over wide ranges of concentration. Published October 1, 1968 H. MOR~TA ANn A. SHIRAISm Mono- and Disaccharides and Sugar Receptor MATERIAL ~6 t v" Y. AND METHODS Downloaded from on June 14, 2017 T h e fleshfly, Boettcherisca peregrina, was used throughout this work. T h e larvae were raised on minced horse meat, and the imagos were raised in the same way, but fed also with 5 % sucrose solution. Imagos between 3 and 6 days old were used in the experiments. T h e recording and stimulation systems were almost the same as described elsewhere (Morita, 1959). An isolated proboscis was mounted on a piece of platinum wire which was inserted into the proboscis through the cut end and served as an indifferent electrode. A long hair on the marginal zone of the labeUum was selected, and the sidewall of the hair was cracked with a microneedle about 50 # from the tip by supporting the hair with the tip (about 15 p in diameter) of a capillary electrode on the opposite side. Then, the cracking needle was replaced with a second capillary with a tip diameter of about 30 p, its tip having been previously dipped briefly into the same electrolyte solution as that in the capillary electrode. When the tip of the capillary electrode was brought into contact with the surface of the solution in the second capillary, the solution began to move from the electrode to the second capillary. Thus, the solution near ~.he surface was renewed continuously and condensation by evaporation at the electrode tip was prevented. Sensory activity was recorded from the cracked part of the hair which was kept in contact with this continuously renewed surface, and the receptor responded at least for 2 hr in a quantitatively reproducible manner unless the receptor was injured during the cracking procedure. T h e electrolyte solution used for the capillary electrode was Waterhouse's saline (Buck, 1953). Stimuli contained in a third capillary whose tip diameter was 50-100 p were applied to the receptor at the hair tip. Movement of this capillary was controlled by a small electromagnet, which was supplied with electric current by an electronic stimulator. T h e duration of stimulation never exceeded 0.5 see. Intervals between stimuli were adjusted with various stimulus strengths. For example, in sucrose stimulation, the intervals were 1.5, 3, and 5 min after stimulation by solutions below 0.1, 0.1-0.2, and above 0.2 M, respectively. All experiments were done at ambient temperatures of 25°C 4- 0.2°C, and at relative humidities of 60-70 %. T h e sensory impulses picked up from the cracked part on the sidewall were fed into an oscilloscope through an amplifier with grid leak of 10u ohm and of low grid current (below 10-12 amp) in its head stage. T h e y were then photographed on running oscillographic paper, and the impulses were counted for 0.15-0.3 see from 0.15 see after the beginning of the stimulus, as a measure of the magnitude of the stationary response. I n the present paper this value will be referred to as the magnitude of response. All sugars used were of special grade of Wako Chemical Industries, Ltd., J a p a n (D-form for monosaeeharides), except for D-fructose, which was made by the British Drug Houses, Ltd., England. T h e specification attached to the fructose sample showed that the specific rotation [a]~° was --89 to --92 and that contamination from Dglucose was less than 1%. The sugars were dissolved in distilled and deionized water for experimental use. Unless otherwise stated, concentrations of sugars are expressed in molarity. This was Published October 1, 1968 562 THE JOURNAL OF GENERAL PHYSIOLOGY • VOLUME 52 • 1968 for convenience in preparing solutions over a wide range of concentrations. We plotted concentrations principally on logarithmic scales, so that the difference between molarity and molality (and thermodynamic activity, too) is relatively small. RESULTS Responses to Single Sugars C O M P A R I S O N O F SUCROSE= G L U C O S E , A N D F R U C T O S E Fig. 1 shows one of the examples in which the responses of a single sugar receptor to sucrose, 5 i 20 x x2° .---~:2.~ . . . . . . . . . . 19 bJ O~ _j B a. - .,--=il,/-/ o GLucosE 14. 0 it= 0 0,5 MOLAR 1.0 CONCENTRATION FmuPa~ I. Comparison of response-concentration relation in stimulations by sucrose, glucose, and fructose. Number attached to each symbol indicates the order of stimulation. glucose, and fructose were studied over a range of concentration between 0.01 and 1.0 M. T h e numbers attached to the circles represent the order of stimulation. As the numbers show, stimulations were given in ascending order as to the concentration of the sugar used. T h e receptor was stimulated by 0.2 M sucrose at times to check the reproducibility of the magnitude of the response. Such procedures were routine for other experiments in the present work. T h e results of Fig. 1 show that the order of stimulating effectiveness was sucrose > fructose > glucose below 0.3 ~, but that it changed to sucrose > glucose > fructose above 0.3 M. Sucrose was the most effective at all concentrations and in all preparations. Between fructose and glucose the concentration at which the order changed varied with the preparation, ranging from 0.1 to 0.4 M, but the reversal was observed in all preparations. Table I shows a comparison of the responses to sucrose, glucose, and fruc- Downloaded from on June 14, 2017 lal Published October 1, 1968 H . MORITA AND A. SmRAISHI Mono- and Disaccharides and Sugar Receptor 563 tose at their m a x i m u m values experimentally obtained at high concentrations. These values would not be the same as the m a x i m u m response each sugar could produce. T h e values were obtained, however, from a series of experiments, in which the concentration range extended above 1.0 M and the response-concentration curve showed a practically horizontal line at high concentrations. Therefore, as rough estimations of the m a x i m u m response, relative to that to sucrose, we m a y take averages of 0.71 and 0.46 in responses to glucose and fructose, respectively. Hassett, Dethier, and Gans (1950) studied the relative sensitivity of the blowfly to the three sugars, comparing individual ascending tarsal thresholds to the sugars for behavioral response. Their results indicated that fructose TABLE I C O M P A R I S O N OF M A X I M A L R E S P O N S E S T O S U C R O S E , G L U C O S E , AND F R U C T O S E Sucrose Glucose Fructose 0.26 0.73 0.33 0.4 0.52 0.56 0.45 0.4O 0.53 275 274 273 218 216 215 214 213 208 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 0.78 0.73 Average 1.0 0.71 0.69 0.46 was the most effective (twice as effective as sucrose), sucrose next, and glucose least. T h e y used different experimental methods, species of fly, and receptor locations, b u t their results correspond to ours at low stimulus concentrations. Discrepancy between the two works is obvious when the relative sensitivities to sucrose and fructose are under consideration. W e considered impurity in our fructose as one of the causes for the discrepancy, and used D-fructose (extra pure for injection) made by E. Merck (Germany) and found no difference in the results for the two fructose samples. However, there is also the possibility of the same sort of impurity existing in the fructose m a d e b y E. Merck. TENTATIVE MODEL For the case in which the response magnitude is proportional to the n u m b e r of sties, each of which is occupied by one stimulus molecule, we can use an equation similar to the Michaelis-Menten equation which describes enzyme reactions. Lineweaver and Burk (1934) modified this equation and introduced two types of plots, giving straight-line relation- Downloaded from on June 14, 2017 preparation Published October 1, 1968 564 THE JOURNAL OF GENERAL PHYSIOLOGY • VOLUME 52 • I968 ships. Fig. 2 corresponds to the second type of the Lineweaver-Burk plot, and is the same as that which Beidler (1954) first applied to the chemoreceptor. A straight-line relationship was obtained for sucrose, as observed by Morita et al. (1966), but not for glucose. This means that the response to glucose is not proportional to the n u m b e r of 1 : 1 complexes as formed between the glucose molecule and the receptor site. We, therefore, examined a model of a 2:1 I • GLUCOSE . X 0 0.5 MOLAL 1.0 CONCENTRATION ( C | FIGURE2. Beidler's (or Lineweaver-Burk) plot in stimulations by glucose and sucrose. Deviation from a straight line in stimulation by glucose shows that the response cannot be described as Beidler's (or the Michaelis-Menten) type. complex simplest When into two formation between stimulus molecules and the receptor site as the after that of a 1 : 1 complex. the 2:1 complex is formed, the reaction m a y generally be divided steps as ,,t + S ~ - A S (1) A --b AS.-~-A2S, (2) and where A, ,.7, AS, and A~S represent a molecule of stimulus substance, the receptor site, and the 1 : 1 and 2 : 1 complexes, respectively. Allowing a, nl , and n2 to be the concentration or number of A, AS, and A~S, respectively, and s the Downloaded from on June 14, 2017 . Published October 1, 1968 H. MORITAAND A. Sr~RAISm Mono-and Disaccharidesand SugarReceptor 565 sum of the numbers of S, AS, and A2S, we m a y assume the existence of constants,/(1 and/(2 (not necessarily equal to dissociation constants), in a steady state, as K1 = a ( s - n x - n2)/nl, (3) K~ = anx/n2. (4) and If the magnitude of response, r, is proportional to the number of the complexes, A2S, as formed, the following equation is obtained: r/r., = 1/(i + XUa + XllC2/a~), (5) r/r,• = 1/(1 q- a/c q- ct/c~). (6) This equation shows that the curve representing the value of r/r,, plotted against c is determined b y the value of a; that is, the curves representing equation (5) with the same value of 1£2/1£1 are the same in shape when plotted against a on a logarithmic scale, even if the values of K1 are different. Thus, the test of the experimental results with equation (5) is facilitated to a great extent. Disaccharides Before going into the results with monosaccharides, let us describe the results with sucrose, when the 1:1 complex model was considered to hold as shown b y Fig. 2. In this case, the response is proportional to nx, and n2 is zero because K2 = co. Accordingly, the relative response is expressed as r/r,, = 1/(1 .-[--K1/a). (7) T h e theoretical curve calculated from equation (7) is compared in Fig. 3 with experimental values obtained in a single sugar receptor. In this and the two following figures, the experimental values were reduced by 2 - 5 % to get the best fit at high sugar concentrations. As seen in Fig. 3, the theoretical curve fits the experimental values fairly well over the range 0.001 to 1.5 M sucrose. Slight deviation from the curve is seen at concentrations from 0.003 to 0.03 M, and m a y be significant as shown also in Fig. 4 by plots of results with six preparations. T h e value of the Downloaded from on June 14, 2017 where r,, is the maximal response resulting from all the sites being occupied b y two molecules of the stimulant, so that the value of r/r,~ m a y be called the relative response. Introducing the "relative concentration," c = a/K~, and a constant, a = K,./Kx, we can rewrite equation (5) as Published October 1, 1968 566 THE JOURNAL GENERAL OF PHYSIOLOGY • VOLUME 52 • I968 c o n s t a n t , K1, v a r i e d w i t h p r e p a r a t i o n s , b u t , if the response is expressed b y e q u a tion (7), all e x p e r i m e n t a l values should b e r e p r e s e n t e d b y a single c u r v e plotted a g a i n s t t h e r e l a t i v e c o n c e n t r a t i o n , e v e n w i t h different KI values in differe n t p r e p a r a t i o n s . As Fig. 4 shows, t h e results of six different p r e p a r a t i o n s (filled circles) c a n b e t h o u g h t of as expressed b y a single t h e o r e t i c a l curve, 1.0 W El Z 0 fit. El UJ r¢ • I ' ' I .... I I i ' i ''''1 I , ~ i ~ I , , i .... " 0.5 hi > _ ~ 0.001 , ~ o:Y 1 0.002 I 0.005 0.01 0,02 , , KI II,.,,J 0.05 0.1 0.2 0.5 i 1,0 2,0 MOLAR CONCENTRATION OF SUCROSE FIGUR~ 3. Responses relative to the maximum vs. concentration of sucrose plotted on a logarithmic scale (in a single receptor). Open circles represent experimental values and the continuous line represents the theoretical curve calculated by the equation (response of Beidler's or Michaelis-Menten type) : r/r~ = 1/(1 + Kx/C) where r is the magnitude of response; rm, the maximum response when all the sites are occupied each by one molecule of sucrose; K1, a constant corresponding to the dissociation constant of a 1 : 1 complex between the site and sucrose molecule; C, molar concentration of sucrose. The maximum responses experimentally obtained are estimated as 95% of the true maximum which the receptor could reach, so that the best fit is obtained at high concentrations of sucrose. b u t , h e r e also, d e v i a t i o n f r o m t h e c u r v e is o b v i o u s o v e r the r a n g e f r o m 0.05 to 0.5 in the relative c o n c e n t r a t i o n . Fig. 5 shows t h e results w i t h m a l t o s e chosen as a n o t h e r d i s a c c h a r i d e a n d t r e a t e d in the s a m e w a y as in Fig. 4. Fig. 5 includes t h e results w i t h five different p r e p a r a t i o n s . T h e results w i t h the i n d i v i d u a l p r e p a r a t i o n s w e r e f o u n d to b e expressed b y t h e t h e o r e t i c a l curves c a l c u l a t e d f r o m e q u a t i o n (7), a n d t h e r e w a s n o t the d e v i a t i o n seen w i t h sucrose. I n t h e results s h o w n in Figs. 4 a n d 5, the v a l u e of Kx for sucrose was 0.06 M, r a n g i n g f r o m 0.05 to 0.14 M (with six p r e p a r a t i o n s ) ; t h a t for m a l t o s e was 0.1 g, r a n g i n g f r o m 0.05 to 0.15 M (with 5 p r e p a r a t i o n s ) (see T a b l e I I ) . Downloaded from on June 14, 2017 ,J ttl tic Published October 1, 1968 H. MOIRITA AND A. SI-IIRAISttI Mono- and Disaccharidesand Sugar Receptor 1.0 I ' ' I .... I I ' ' I .... I I,. 'i''1 .... *'.. I.• 567 .J LU Z o o:: 0.5 b.I •. "" .J • ' ~ 0.02 • e • Oe :':l-''~,i 0.05 -'," i [ 0.1 I I Illi 0.2 RELATIVE 'I 0.5 I CONCENTRATION I , , [iiirl 2 5 OF I tO 20 SUCROSE Monosaccharides Figs. 6 and 7 show tests of the results with fructose and glucose, respectively, by equation (6). As seen in these figures, the experimental values (filled circles) are in good agreement with the theoretical curves (continuous curves) of the 2:1 complex model. Fig. 6 includes the results with nine different preparations, and the value of a (= K~/K1) for f,o " • • ' e• W $0 Z 0 t 0. W n~ 0.5 • • * . ee • W e• • e .J hJ eee rr 0 • ee ~ ! .... 0.05 • I _ 0.1 RELATIVE l h G u ~ 5. receptors, eoe I 0.5 , , lil*,l 0.5 i I 2 , , I .... 5 i I I0 20 CONCENTRATION OF MALTOSE T h e same as in Fig. 4, but in stimulation by maltose, with five different Downloaded from on June 14, 2017 Fmum~ 4. T h e same type of test as in Fig. 3, but with experimental values (filled circles) in six different receptors. Relative concentration is C/Kx, where C is molar concentration. Continuous line represents the theoretical curve calculated with the same equation used in Fig. 3. Experimental values were reduced as in Fig. 3. Published October 1, 1968 568 THE JOURNAL OF GENERAL PHYSIOLOGY • VOLUME 52 • ~968 the theoretical curve was 4. Fitting the theoretical curve with the experimental values obtained from one preparation, the value of/(1 for this preparation was determined by reading the concentration corresponding to unity in the relative concentration. T h e values of K1 thus obtained for individual preparations ranged from 0.014 to 0.095 M and were averaged as 0.015 M. Accordingly, the average of K2 values was 0.06 ~ ( K 2 / K ~ = a = 4). Similarly, for glucose (Fig. 7) tested on 10 different preparations, a was unity, and the averaged value of KI was 0.1 M, ranging from 0.08 to 0.12 M. 1.0 ', .... i , I .... i ~ * LU (D Z • O oe~e %,• • o• • • • LU > 0.5 o • .J w • ~ I _ : 0.5 • I I , ~ I ,,,,I 2 5 10 I 20 , , I .... I 50 100 RELATIVE CONCENTRATION OF FRUCTOSE FmURE 6. Comparison of experimental results (filled circles) in nine receptors stimulated by fructose with the theoretical curve (continuous line) calculated from the equation: r/r,, = 1/(1 + a / c --k ot/c2), where c = C / K 1 (relative concentration), a t = K ~ / K 1 ) = 4.0 (see equations 3 and 4 in the text as to K1 and K s ) , t h e others the same as in Fig. 3. T h e r e was a distinct difference between the results with mono- and disaccharides. While the results with disaccharides with the individual preparations were all expressed by the same theoretical curve plotted against the relative concentration, those with monosaccharides were not. This is because the value of a varied with preparations. Nevertheless, the results with different preparations were on the whole expressed by a single theoretical curve for each sugar as shown in Figs. 6 and 7. This means that there was a mean in the value of a for each monosaccharide, and this value varied r a n d o m l y about the mean with different preparations. In T a b l e II are summarized the values of K1 a n d / f 2 estimated as above. Downloaded from on June 14, 2017 D(/) hi IV Published October 1, 1968 Mono- and Disaccharidesand Sugar Receptor H. MORITA AND A. SmRAISm 569 T h e table shows that fructose is the most effective in the sense that it has the highest affinity for the receptor site. Interactions between Different Sugars It has been shown above that stimulations b y disaccharides and b y monosaccharides are explained by the 1 : 1 and 2 : 1 complex models, respectively. 1.0 i ' ' i .... I i " ' J '",.-,I.--LJ..- O|e • ee •I l a ~ " Li.I (/) Z oo e O 0,. ¢.t) W O~ • 0.5 W >_ ee o ~e • n.. .~...~_ 0 0.1 0.2 RELATIVE _~ ",:1,,,,I 0.5 I I , 2 , I ,,,,f 5 CONCENTRATION I I0 20 F t o u a s 7. T h e same as in Fig. 6, but with 10 receptors stimulated by glucose. T h e value of a used for calculation of the theoretical curve is 1.0. OF GLUCOSE TABLE II A P P A R E N T K1, Ks V A L U E S F O R T H E S U G A R S TO COMPLEX WITH THE RECEPTOR SITE Sugars KI Range of KI Sucrose 0.06 0.05-0.14 Maltose 0.1 0.015 0.1 0.05-0.15 0.014-0.025 0.08-0.12 M Fructose Glucose K~ M 0.06 0.1 For the meaning of Kt and K2, see equations (3) a n d (4) in the text. T h e structure or nature of the receptor site will be described below while the interaction between different sugars stimulating the same receptor is also investigated. "GLUCOSE AND FRUCTOSECOMBININGSITES" Evans (1963) has postulated that there are at least two types of combining sites on the m e m b r a n e of one sugar receptor, one for glucose and the other for fructose. H e has also claimed that sucrose acts predominantly at the "fructose site" (Evans, 1961). If these sites are strictly specific for each substrate (i.e. sucrose combines only with the fructose site), there would be little interaction between sucrose and glucose Downloaded from on June 14, 2017 J w Published October 1, 1968 57° THE JOURNAL OF GENERAL PHYSIOLOGY • VOLUME 52 • I968 w h e n the two sugars are given in the same solution. O n e of the e x p e r i m e n t a l results is shown in Fig. 8, w h e r e the responses to plain sucrose solutions are c o m p a r e d w i t h the responses to m i x t u r e s of 1 M glucose a n d various c o n c e n trations of sucrose. C o n c e n t r a t i o n s of sucrose in the plain a n d m i x e d solutions are p l o t t e d on the X axis. T h e results show t h a t the response to the m i x t u r e of 1 M glucose a n d one of the various c o n c e n t r a t i o n s of sucrose could b e r e g a r d e d as slightly additive o n l y at a low c o n c e n t r a t i o n of sucrose. T h e response to t h e same c o n c e n t r a t i o n of sucrose was h i g h e r in the plain solution t h a n in the 30 3 24 14 13 5.10fl3 u~ ~J 3 Oil 8 9 T 20 4 _ _ ~ ,,KI'7 6 00 hi ~O J I1. F/ • iM GLUCOSE+SUCROSE LJ o COINCIDENCE OF o AND • IO I/ l/ 6 r x 2,19 19 I I 0 I i I IM GLUCOSE I I I 0.5 MOLAR CONCENTRATION I ] LO OF SUCROSE FIGUP,~8. Effects of I Mglucose on stimulation of a single receptor by sucrose. Numbers attached to symbols represent the order of stimulations (the same in the following figures). The broken line shows the average of responses to 1 ~ glucose. Values on X axis represent concentrations of sucrose both in plain solutions and in mixtures with 1 ~ glucose. m i x t u r e at h i g h c o n c e n t r a t i o n s of sucrose. T h e r e f o r e , we c a n n o t c o n c l u d e t h a t the sucrose m o l e c u l e c o m b i n e s only with one t y p e of site, q u i t e i n d e p e n d e n t l y of the ,'glucose site." O n the c o n t r a r y , we will h a v e to assume fairly strong c o m p e t i t i o n b e t w e e n sucrose a n d glucose for the same r e c e p t o r site. A l m o s t the same e x t e n t of i n t e r a c t i o n was observed b e t w e e n sucrose a n d fructose (Fig. 9). C o m p a r e d with the inhibition by glucose shown in Fig. 8, t h a t b y fructose was n o stronger. T h e s e results show t h a t sucrose molecules c o m b i n e with the glucose site as well as with the fructose site, if t h e r e is a n y d i f f e r e n t i a t i o n a m o n g the r e c e p t o r sites. I n t e r a c t i o n b e t w e e n glucose a n d fructose should give us i n f o r m a t i o n a b o u t the differentiation b e t w e e n the glucose a n d fructose sites. T h e results of the Downloaded from on June 14, 2017 (J ,,i 15 Published October 1, 1968 I--I. MORITAAND A. Sn~RAzsm Mono- and Disaccharidesand Sugar Receptor 57 z 25 20 rj LU O3 oJ I0 ~8 Ig Xl2 I 2 =E 0 X17 0 SUCROSE ONLY I t • I M FRUCTOSE 4- SUCROSE o COINCIDENCE x I M FRUCTOSE t I 0 , OF o A N D I [ I I I I I, 1.0 0.5 FIGURE 9. T h e same as in Fig. 8, but with interaction between fructose and sucrose. experiment as they affect this problem are shown by Fig. 10. T h e responses to the mixtures of 0.05 M glucose and various concentrations of fructose were higher, apparently by an a m o u n t of the response to 0.05 ~ glucose, than those to plain fructose solutions over the entire range of fructose concentrations tested. Accordingly, the glucose site might be assumed to be differentiated from the fructose site. However, the lack of significant difference between the response to the mixtures of 1 ~ glucose with fructose (half-filled circles) and . ro i,i o3 . . - / • IM GLUCOSE ~" e ! M GLUCOSE + FRUCTOSE L / N (~ u) . 14 ~ - 29 16 IO LIJ CO --I 3 O. X15 //,4/v'- 0 . 0 5 M GLUCOSE + FRUCTOSE X FRUCTOSE ONLY &k A O.OSM GLUCOSE ~X: 0 is . o , i 0 ~ i I i t ~a A-- ---A--- , B 0.5 MOLAR CONCENq'RATION I 1.0 OF FRUCTOSE FIGum~ 10. Interaction between glucose and fructose, Concentration of glucose in mixtures was fixed at 1.0 or 0.05 z~. Averages of responses to 1.0 and 0.05 M glucose are represented by broken and dot and dash lines, respectively. Downloaded from on June 14, 2017 MOLAR CONCENTRATION OF SUCROSE Published October 1, 1968 572 THE JOURNAL OF GENERAL PHYSIOLOGY • VOLUME 52 • I968 that to plain 1 M glucose (filled circles) suggests that glucose molecules can occupy the fructose site fairly accurately, competing with fructose molecules. This means that the differentiation is poor, if it exists at all. T h e apparent additivity mentioned with the mixture of 0.05 M glucose and fructose m a y be explained by assuming that the complex type, such as the fructose-glucosereceptor site, can be formed (see Fructose effects). MANNOSE EFFECTS Mannose has been known as a unique monosaccharide. In spite of a very weak stimulating effect, it is a strong competitive inhibitor for fructose stimulation according to the behavioral study on the blowfly by Dethier, Evans, and Rhoades (1956). T h e results shown in Fig. 11 L i i , i i ' ~,,i 1.0 , , , i, " , i 4 ' ' ' (~. 8 14 W 0.5 • CONTROL(0.4M FRUCTOSE) o 0.4M FRUCTOSE+MANNOSE / x MANNOSE ONLY ,=,~........//=,"~/ I 0.01 , , , i ,,,,,t I 0.05 0.1 t t I ,,,,I 0.5 = I.(3 , , t/ 5.0 MOLAR CONCENTRATION OF MANNOSE FIGURE l 1. Effectsof various concentrations of mannose on stlmu]ation by a fixed concentration of 0.4 Mfructose, where response was taken as unity. verify their conclusion. Here, again, the response to plain mannose solutions cannot be explained by the 1:1 complex model, since, in that case, the response-intensity curve should cover a concentration range of 1 to 103 for zero to the m a x i m u m response (see Figs. 3-5), whereas the curve in the mannose response covers a concentration range of less than 1 to l0 S (see also Figs. 12 and 13). Furthermore, mannose has a definite effect on the response to fructose at concentrations at which mannose does not have any stimulating effect by itself. This suggests that a mannose molecule, at these concentrations, occupies one of the two units for fructose molecules in a receptor site and blocks the response. T h e results of the same type of experiments for glucose and sucrose are shown in Figs. 12 and 13. At concentrations (around 0.05 M) of no stimulating effect by itself, mannose apparently had a synergistic effect on the responses to glucose and sucrose. Also in the results shown in Figs. 12 and 13 an inhibition Downloaded from on June 14, 2017 m Z Published October 1, 1968 Mono-and Disaccharides and Sugar Receptor H . MORITA AND A . SHIRAISHI I ' ' ~ I ''"1 , ' ' I 'L"I ' ' ' 573 I 4 1,0 5 9 12 • 13 liJ O~ Z O O. ,,, 0.5 . CONmOLtO.SM GLUCOSE~ > o 0.6 M GLUCOSE+ MANNOSE J'~ x MANNOSE ONLY ~ O::: ¢.....~.j| ] 1 / x ~=9'2V'x o . . I . . ' .~ . .. ~ ., . . 0.05 0.1 I 0.01 t , I .... 0.5 l , I.O 5.0 by mannose is observed, but it was weak compared with the effect on fructose and occurred at concentrations at which mannose could stimulate by itself. FRUCTOSE EFFECTS When the results presented above are considered, we might imagine the simplest picture of sugar stimulation as follows. The receptor site with which one sucrose molecule combines is comprised of two subunits. For excitation the receptor site has to be simultaneously occupied ' I 1.0 w z oB. w it. ' ' ' I .... I ' ' ' I ''"I ' ~ ' ~ I dP 23 o.5 >_ • CONTROL( 0.2 M SUCROSE) SUCROSE + MANNOSE x MANNOSE ON .._i hi I O.OI , , I I .... 0.05 ~_..----r'~-, O.I . t .... 0.5 19 ~o 2t I , , I.O , I 5.0 MOLAR CONCENTRATION OF MANNOSE FIGuP-E 13. E f f e c t s of v a r i o u s c o n c e n t r a t i o n s o f m a n n o s e o n s t i m u l a t i o n b y a fixed c o n c e n t r a t i o n o f 0.2 M sucrose, w h e r e r e s p o n s e w a s t a k e n as u n i t y . Downloaded from on June 14, 2017 MOLAR CONCENTRATION OF MANNOSE FzGUP.~ 12. Effects of various concentrations of marmose on s t i m u l a t i o n b y a fixed c o n c e n t r a t i o n of 0 . 6 M glucose, w h e r e r e s p o n s e w a s t a k e n as u n i t y . Published October 1, 1968 574 THE JOURNAL OF G E N E R A L PHYSIOLOGY " VOLUME 52 - I968 at the two subunits. Disaccharides could fill the two units with one molecule, but monosaccharides would have to fill t h e m with two molecules. A problem here is the behavior of fructose. As shown in Table I I fructose is considered to have the highest affinity (even if the value of Ks is taken into account) among the sugars tested. Nevertheless, its competitive effect on stimulation by other sugars was very weak (see Figs. 9 and 10), and its stimulating effect was deeply depressed by low concentrations of mannose (Fig. 11 ). Another example of such a property of fructose is shown by experiments in which the receptor was stimulated by mixtures of 0.1 g sucrose and various 20 O O.I M SUCROSE + FRUCTOSE x 0.1 M SUCROSE ~'~I M (.) 12 1515 13 ~ • ~ 0.5 M FRUCTOSE i7 18 I (5 I0 ~7 XIo G : - . . - . 9 . _ -.~.. - - 14 (/) ._I 0. I I I I I I t 2 5 4 5 6 MOLAR CONCENTRATION OF FRUCTOSE FIGURE 14. Effectsof various concentrations of fructose on stimulation by a fixod concentration of 0.1 ~ sucrose. Averages of responses to 0.1 M sucrose and the maximal response to fructose experimentally obtained are represented by broken and dot and dash lines, respectively. concentrations of fructose, up to 6 M (Fig. 14). F r o m the values of K1 (shown in Table II) and the concentration ratio, sucrose molecules should occupy less than 1/240 of the total of the receptor sites in the mixture of 0.1 M sucrose and 6 M fructose, and the response to the mixture should be almost the same as the m a x i m u m response to fructose, provided that a sucrose moleucle never shares one of the sites with another molecule. T h e response was not reduced significantly, however, compared with that to single solutions of 0.1 u sucrose. One explanation for such a result is simply to assume that one receptor site can be shared by each of the sucrose and fructose molecules. If such a heterogeneous complex is more effective in excitation than the 2:1 complex of fructose and the receptor site, the inhibitory effect of fructose on stimulation by other sugars should be relatively slight. T h e above-mentioned assumption predicts that the complex (fructose- Downloaded from on June 14, 2017 ill O~ 0,I Published October 1, 1968 H. MORITA AND A. SHIRAISHI Mono- and Disaeeharides and Sugar Receptor 575 glucose-receptor site) is formed when the sugar receptor is stimulated b y a mixture of fructose and glucose, and can be detected as a synergism at low concentrations. Dethier et al. (1956) reported that this was the case, b u t probably because of the variability of the K1 value with different preparations and other factors, the prediction was fulfilled b y only one preparation out of several (Fig. 15). In this preparation, the responses to the mixtures of 0.02 M fructose with glucose (notice the results below 0.04 ~r glucose) are shown to be higher than those to the pure solution of 0.02 M fructose, though responses to glucose below 0.04 M were zero. ~ o O.02M • GLUCOSE ONLY J FRUCTOSE + GLUCOSE x o o2M F ./72 ~,~/25 / / C,=E ~o I0 d if) bJ 01 .j a. 16 5 >~ d ' 14 . 0 20 27 . . . .xs. ./. 7 ~ ' _ _ _ _ _ _ J ~_ __ _£" XZS s .L4 - 0.01 .6, ~1 .... 0.05 , 0.1 , , = I,,HI 0.5 Xz X31 , 1.0 , I .... I 5.0 MOLAR CONCENTRATION OF GLUCOSE F 1 o u ~ 15. Effects of various concentrations of glucose o n stimulation by a fixed concentration of 0.02 M fructose. Average of response to 0.02 M fructose is represented by b r o k e n line. DISCUSSION Multimolecular Complex Model Formation of a multimolecular complex between stimulating molecules and the receptor site described here is not the first example to be shown in chemoreceptors. Quite recently, T a t e d a and H i d a k a (1966) studied the receptor for sweet substances in the rat, and have suggested that more than four molecules of glycine can combine with the single receptor site. In the present work, we need not have assumed any model of a complex combining more than two molecules of stimulant. In describing the results, we have assumed only one type of receptor site, which is divided into two subunits: when the two subunits are simultaneously filled with stimulating molecules, excitation results. F r o m this picture of the sugar receptor, it also Downloaded from on June 14, 2017 0 1/1 N Published October 1, 1968 576 THE JOURNAL OF GENERAL PHYSIOLOGY • VOLUME 52 • 1968 follows that the single receptor site could combine with two molecules of disaccharides. This point of view might assist in interpreting the slight deviation from the theoretical curve shown in the results with sucrose stimulation. GSo Sv where GKo -- [G][So Sv] [GSo SF] ' ( DI-I ) G -t- So Sv ~ So S~, G, where rKa - [G][So SF] ( D1-2 ) G+ Sa Sv o and [So &, G] " Each symbol in equation (D I) corresponds to each one in equations (1) and (3) as G = A, [G] = a, SoS~, = S, and SoSFG = AS, GSoSy and [SoS,] = s -- (nx + n,.), [GSaSr] + [SoSFG] = n,. Comparing equation (D 1) with equations (1) and (3), we obtain 1 1 1 aK-~ + ~K~ - K1 " (for glucose) (D2) T h e second step of reaction is assumed to proceed as G -t- GSo Sp ,-" GSo SF G, where FKo = [G][aSo SF] [GSo S~, G]" ( D3-1 ) Downloaded from on June 14, 2017 Differentiation of the Receptor Site The results of strong interaction between glucose and fructose suggest that no definite groups of receptor sites are differentiated for combining only with two molecules of glucose or only with two of fructose. T h e same results, however, do not exclude the possibility that the two subunits of the receptor site differentiate to combine with glucose and fructose, respectively. From the affinity, which is measured by the reciprocals of/£1 and Ks values listed in Table II, it is also unlikely that there is any complete differentiation between the subunits, since the Ks value for fructose is less than the/£1 value for glucose. (If there is any differentiation for glucose and for fructose, the first step of the reaction in stimulation by glucose should occur mainly at the subunit specific for glucose, and the second step mainly at the one specific for fructose.) However, some degree of differentiation is suggested by the difference in the effects of mannose on stimulation by fructose and by glucose or sucrose. If we assume temporarily that the subunits are differentiated, and, therefore, denote them by So and Sp, respectively (the receptor site, S, accordingly, being expressed as SoS~,) and the glucose molecule by G, equation (1) is divided into Published October 1, 1968 H. MORITAAND A. SHIm~ISHI Mono- and Disaceharides and Sugar Receptor 577 and G + So Sv G ~ GSa S~, G, where oKo = [G][So S~, G] [Gso s~ G] ( D3-2 ) Then, comparing equation (D3) with equations (2) and (4), we get oKo "1- FKo = K2 . (for glucose) (D4) T h e value of a ( = K~/K1) for glucose can be obtained from equations (D2) and (D4) as oz = K2/K1 = (aKo -b ~,Ko)2/(oKa • ~,Ko). (D5) Allosteric Model Such an interaction as the one mentioned above has been claimed as strong evidence for allosteric transition in proteins (Wyman, 1963). O u r receptor site is considered in m a n y respects to be composed of allosteric macromolecules. First, specificity for certain sugars m a y be attributed only to macromolecular structure. Second, noncharge molecules such as those of sugars m a y induce electrical changes in the receptor membrane only through structural changes in the receptor site, and these changes should be closely related to, or synonymous with, allosteric transitions. Third, the existence of subunits has been emphasized in the present paper, and an interaction between the subunits has been suggested. It is, therefore, justifiable to examine the present results from the viewpoint of allosteric transitions in the receptor site. Monad, Wyman, and Changeux (1965) have proposed a model for allosteric transitions. According to them, let us assume two states of the receptor site as Ro R~, ~-. To TF , where L - [To Tp] [Ro R~,] ( D6 ) and R o , R1,, T o , and TF represent two different subunits in the R and T state, respectively. T h e symbol, L, denotes an equilibrium constant for the Downloaded from on June 14, 2017 It can easily be shown from equation (D5) that the value of ~ is m i n i m u m and is 4 when oKo = FKo • Therefore, any value of ~ below 4 indicates that the assumption m a d e in equation (D3) is wrong, and that a "stabilizing interaction" exists between the subunits. T h e analyses of the results of glucose and fructose stimulation are summarized in Table II, and the value of ~ = K2/K1 is unity for glucose and 4 for fructose. As far as we can assume that there are two subunits in a single receptor site, we have to conclude that there is a stabilizing interaction between the subunits making a complex with molecules of monosaccharide. Published October 1, 1968 578 THE JOURNAL OF GENERAL PHYSIOLOGY • VOLUME 5° " I968 transition. T h e dissociation constants between the subunits and ligands (fructose and glucose) are defined as oKo _ [G][Ro] [Ro G] ' ,Ko - ,K, - [F][R,] oK, - [Re F] ' [G][Rr] [R, G] , [F][Ro] [Ro F]' , oKo - [G][To] , [To GI , [GI[T,] l,Ko -, [ T , G] eK'r - [F][Tr] oK; - F] [T, ' [el[rol [To F] " For convenience of derivation of the folllwing equations, ratios between the constants are defined as ! co = oKo/oXo, bo oKo/tKo, c, = ,X,l,X;. b, = , K , IOK,, = ? bo = o K o / t K o , b; ! = t ,X, loX,. relative concentrations of glucose and fructose are denoted by ao (= [G]/oKo)and a , ( = [ F ] / , K r ) , respectively. T h e n u m b e r s of all forms of the site in the R state, ZR, and that in the T state, ~ T, are ~R = [Roa,] + [CRoR,] + [RoR,G] + [CRoR,C] + [aR~,F] + [FRoR,a] + [R~R,F] + [FR~,] + [FR,RoF] ( D7-1 ) = [RoR~,]{I + (1 + bo)ao + bo~2o nt- (1 + b~,bo)otoap + (1 + b,)a, + b,o31 T~T = [ To Tr] + . . . . . ( t h e same types of complex as in ~R) t it 2 2 t ! = L[RoRa,]{ 1 + (1 + bo)coao + oocoao + (1 + b o b , ) e o c , a o a , ( D7-2 ) ? 71 2 2 + (1 + b,)c,a, + o,c, oo,}. T h e function of state (fraction of the site in the R state)• R, is R- ZR ZR + l~T" (D8) T h e maximal value of the function of s t a t e , / ~ , when the values of av and a o are infinitely large in an equimolar mixture of glucose and fructose, is written as ~ = 1 ? 2 ! 1 + co L(~,r + bo)(~'rb, + t) ('t + bo)('rb,, + 1) • (D9) where ¢/ = c p / c o and 3' = o o , / a o . In the case of pure glucose or fructose, respectively, equation (D9) reduces to Downloaded from on June 14, 2017 The Published October 1, 1968 H. Mortrra AND A. SHIRAISHI Mono- and Disaccharides and Sugar Receptor 579 1 ~®o = 1 q- c2o L(bo/bo) ' ( DI0-1 ) 1 (DIO-2,) 1 + c2r L(b'r/b~,)" I f we assume t h a t the response is p r o p o r t i o n a l t o / ~ , we c a n test this assumption with a p p r o p r i a t e values for the constants. 1.0 t~ i~8 0.5 -~-~M-. I , i l,li,l ,d I ,0 5 ,o 4 FIGURE 16. Test of the experimental results in stimulations by fructose used in Fig. 6 (filled circles) with allosteric model. The continuous curve was obtained from equation (D8) in the text. The values of the constants used: L ffi I 0 e, c r ffi 10-4, br •0.1, and b~ ffi 10. Resultant value of R,or is 0.5, and the estimated value of FKI, is 1.5 X 10-s M. T h e results of the test are shown in Figs. 16 a n d 17 for fructose a n d for glucose, respectively, a n d the e x p e r i m e n t a l values are the ones used in Figs. 6 a n d 7. T h e constants used are L = 10s, c a - - 10.-4, bo = 0.1, c~ = br = b~, = b~ = 1, (D:I) 10 -4 , 0.1, 10, so t h a t the m a x i m u m responses for glucose ( / ~ o) a n d for fructose (l~®v) are c a l c u l a t e d as 0.91 a n d 0.5, respectively; the values of aKo a n d r K r c a n also b e e s t i m a t e d a p p r o x i m a t e l y as 4 × 10 -6 a n d 1.5 × 10 -5 ~, respectively. I n Fig. 18, the c a l c u l a t e d values of R (R = 1.0 w h e n all sites are in the R state) with the same values of constants used in Figs. 16 a n d 17 are p l o t t e d against m o l a r c o n c e n t r a t i o n , so t h a t it is easy to c o m p a r e t h e m with the e x p e r i m e n t s Downloaded from on June 14, 2017 0 Published October 1, 1968 580 THE JOURNAL OF GENERAL PHYSIOLOGY • VOLUME 5° • x968 shown b y Fig. 1. T h e theoretical curves w h i c h were c a l c u l a t e d for glucose (curve A) a n d for fructose (curve B) well represent the e x p e r i m e n t a l d a t a . R e l a t i v e c o n c e n t r a t i o n s of 1 M glucose a n d fructose are a p p r o x i m a t e d f r o m the values of oKa a n d pKF (4 × 10 -5 a n d 1.5 X 10 -5 ~a, respectively) as a o = 2.5 × 104 a n d a p = 7 X 104. T h e r e f o r e , with these values, the response to the m i x t u r e of 1.0 M glucose a n d 1.0 M fructose c a n be c a l c u l a t e d b y e q u a t i o n (D8), or as a close a p p r o x i m a t i o n by e q u a t i o n (Dg). T h e resultant v a l u e is 0.77, a n d it is shown b y the b r o k e n line at the u p p e r p a r t of ] .0 i--e--ee~oe m • • • .,./: ee • • o • ... • 0.5 X IQ~: : • It, o• • oe • • • • 0 io* ,o FiGum~ 17. The same as in Fig. 16, but for stimulations by glucose. The experimental values (filled circles) are the same ones used in Fig. 7. The values of the constants used for calculation in equation (D8): L = 106, ca = 10 --4, be = 0.1, and b~ = 1. Resultant value of/~o0a is 0.91, and the estimated value of oKo is 4 X 10--5 M. Fig. 18. T h i s d e m o n s t r a t e s t h a t it is possible for the response to the m i x t u r e not to d e p a r t so m u c h f r o m the response to 1.0 M glucose, b u t to d o so f r o m the response to 1.0 M fructose. F o r c o m p a r i s o n with the results in Fig. 10, the values for the m i x t u r e s of a dilute c o n c e n t r a t i o n of glucose a n d various c o n c e n t r a t i o n s of fructose ( a o = 103, a~ is variable) are plotted as c u r v e C. C o m p a r e d with the results shown in Fig. l0 the result is s o m e w h a t too high at low fructose concentrations. I t will be noticed t h a t the theoretical curves o b t a i n e d f r o m the allosteric a n d the classical c o m p l e x models are almost the same. T h i s is q u i t e u n d e r s t a n d a b l e since the q u a d r a t i c terms of c o n c e n t r a t i o n ( a ~ , o~ ,F a n d a o a p ) are b y far the largest in size in the n u m e r a t o r of e q u a t i o n (D8). I n o t h e r words, in the allosteric model also the response is practically p r o p o r t i o n a l to Downloaded from on June 14, 2017 I1~ Published October 1, 1968 H. MORITA AND A. SHIRAISHI Mono-and Disaccharides and Sugar Receptor 581 the n u m b e r of sites occupied by two molecules of ligand. Some i m p o r t a n t differences between the two models, however, exist in the assumptions on which the two are based. I t has been shown that the maximal responses are different with different sugars. According to the allosteric model, such differences result mainly from differences in the ratios between the dissociation constants of ligands for the R state and for the T state of the receptor site. T h e classical complex model, however, interprets the same result as the difference in the proportionality constant between the response m a g n i t u d e and the n u m b e r of the 2:1 complex. I t might be supposed, therefore, that the extent i.o F ~q~(o(,,-=o) 0 0.5 1.0 MOLAR CONCENTRATION FIOURE 18. Calculations of fractions of the site in the R state, R, for various concentrations of glucose (curve A), of fructose (curve B), both in plain solutions; and of fructose in mixtures with glucose whose relative concentration is 103 (curve C). Calculated values of/~ for the mixture of 1 M glucose (ao = 2.5 × 104) and 1 M fructose (c~v = 7 X 104) and for plain glucose at relative concentration of 103 are represented by the upper and lower broken lines, respectively. The values of the constants used are the same that were used in Figs. 16 and 17. of permeability change in the receptor m e m b r a n e could be different with different types of the complex. If we assume conventionally that the K1 values in T a b l e II are the dissociation constants and compare them with the values of oKo and vKF estimated from the allosteric model, the free energy change for forming a complex with the ligands has to be more negative in the R state of the allosteric model than in the site of the classical complex model by 7-8 kcal/mole. This amounts to the free energy change of hydrolysis of so-called " h i g h energy" phosphate compounds. In fact, in the allosteric model, combination of the ligands with the subunit in the R state causes the release of the free energy for transition from T to R. Downloaded from on June 14, 2017 0 Published October 1, 1968 5 8`, THE JOURNAL OF GENERAL PHYBIOLOGY • VOLUME 52 • 1968 This work was mpported in part by the Scientific Research Fund from the Ministry of Education of Japan and under the sponsorship of the United States Army Research and Development Group (Far East), Department of the Army, DA-CRD-AG-S92-544-67-G61. The contents of this paper fulfill in part the requirement for the Doctoral Thesis of one of us (A.S.) at Kynshu University. Receivedfor publication 15 April 1968. REFERENCES ANTONINI, E., E. BuccI, C. FKONTICELLI, J. WYMAN, a n d A. ROSSI-FANELLI. 1965. T h e p r o p - erties and interactions of the isolated a- and/3-chains of human haemoglobin. III. Observations on the equilibria and kinetics of the reactions with gases. J. )Viol. Biol. 12:375. BEn)LEa, L. M. 1954. A theory of taste stimulation. J. Gen. Physiol. 38:133. BUCK, J. B. 1953. Physical properties and chemical composition of insect blood. In Insect Physiology. K. D. Roeder, editor. John Wiley and Sons, Inc., New York. p. 147. DETHIER, V. G., D. 1~. EVANS, and M. V. RHGAD~S. 1956. Some factors controlling the ingestion of carbohydrates by the blowfly. Biol. Bull. 111:204. EVANS, D. R. 1961. Depression of taste sensitivity to specific sugars by their presence during development. Science. 133:327. EVANS, D. R. 1963. Chemical structure and stimulation by carbohydrates. In Olfaction and Taste. Y. Zotterman, editor. Pergamon Press, Oxford. p. 165. Downloaded from on June 14, 2017 The theory presented here, which is based on the allosteric model, is rather incomplete. It has not been shown that stimulations by disaccharides can be interpreted by the same model. It may be possible, however, to describe them with the allosteric model by using the constants in equation (D7), based on an assumption that one molecule of disaccharides can combine with each subunit at two different parts of the monosaccharide. We have assumed in this model (in the classical complex model, also) that there are only identical receptor sites, SoS~,, but there could be other types of receptor sites, for example, SoSo and SFSp. Such an additional assumption might give better agreement with the results of experiments on fructose effects on glucose stimulation, though a discrepancy has been pointed out between the theoretical curve and experimental values at low concentrations of fructose in Fig. 18 (curve C). Monod et al. (1965) have based their theory on polymers of identical subunits, in which case they have proved that any intermediate state such as R T is unstable and can be neglected. Some recent papers (Antonini, Bucci, Fronticelli, Wyman, and Rossi-Fanelli, 1965; Tyuma, Benesch, and Benesch, 1966) show, however, that artificially synthesized hemoglobin molecules composed of the same four subunits have weaker allosteric activities compared with those of two a and two/3 chains. Therefore, it may be justifiable to assume that the two subunits introduced here have different structures. At present, we have insufficient data to decide whether the classical complex model or the allosteric one is really correct, though the latter seems to give a somewhat better interpretation of the experimental results. Published October 1, 1968 H. MORITA AND A. SmRAISm Mono- and Disaccharides and Sugar Receptor 583 EVANS, D. R., and D. MELLON, JR. 1962 a. Electrophysiological studies of a water receptor associated with the taste scnsilla of the blowfly. J. Gen. Physiol. 45:487. Downloaded from on June 14, 2017 EVANS, D. R., and D. MELLON, JR. 1969 b. Stimulation of a primary taste receptor by salts. ,7. Gen. Physiol. 45:651. GXLLARY,H. L. 1966. Stimulation of the salt receptor of the blowfly. I. NaC1. J. Gen. Physiol. 50:337. HASSETT, C. C., V. G. DETHmR, and J. GANS. 1950. A comparison of nutritive values and taste thresholds of carbohydrates for thc blowfly. BioL Bull. 99:446. Ln~EWEAVER, H., and D. Btrm~. 1934. The determination of enzyme dissociation constants. J. Am. Chem. Soc. 56-'658. MONOD, J., J. WYMAN, and J. P. CI~NOEtrx. 1965. On the natures of allosteric transition: a plausible model. J. MoL BioL 12:88. MOmTA, H. 1959. Initiation of spike potentials in contact chemoseusory hairs of insects. III. D. C. stimulation and generator potential of labellar chemoreceptor of Calliphora. J. Cellular Comp. Physiol. 54:189. MOmTA, H., T. I-tmAKA, and A. SmRAmm. 1966. Excitatory and inhibitory effects of salts on the sugar receptor of the fleshfly. Mem. Fac. SK. Kyushu Univ. Ser. E. 4:123. MOmTA, H., and S. YAMASmTA. 1966. Further studies on the receptor potential of chemoreceptors of the blowfly. Mem. Fac. Sd. Kyushu Univ. Ser. E. 4:83. STEmHARDT, R. A., H. Mom"rA, and E. S. HODOSON. 1966. Mode of action of straight chain hydrocarbons on primary ehcmoreceptors of the blowfly, Phormia regina. J. Cellular Physiol. 67:53. TATEDA, H., and I. HmAKA. 1966. Taste responses to sweet substances in rat. Mere. Fac. Sd. Kyushu Univ. Ser. E. 4:137. T Y ~ , I., R. E. B~Esc~, and R. BES~mCH. 1966. The preparation and properties of the isolatcd e~- and ~-subunits of haemoglobin A. Biochemistry. 5:2957. WYMAN,J. 1963. Allosteric effects in hemoglobin. Cold Spring Harbor Symp. Quant. Biol. 28:483.
© Copyright 2025 Paperzz