Systematicity beyond obligatoriness in the history of the

Systematicity beyond obligatoriness
in the history of the English progressive
Berit Johannsen
[email protected]
Susanne Flach
[email protected]
bit.ly/s!ach
ICAME 36, Universität Trier, 28 May 2015
History of the English progressive
A. General rise in frequency
Figure 1: Frequency increase of the progressive 1710-1920
Data: CLMET
B. Usage in Early and Late
Modern English:
!
‣ " unsettled
‣ " unsystematic
‣ " not obligatory
“deviation from present-day usage”
“it occurs in stative contexts where one
would not use the progressive in PDE”
“it is not until the middle of the 19th
century that the [progressive] becomes obligatory”
“we can classify the pre-1600 period as one of unsystematic use, and the
post-1700 period as one of systematic or grammatically-required use”
Strang (1982), Nehls (1988,) Elsness (1994), Arnaud (1998), Rissanen (2000), Núñez-Pertejo (2004), Kranich (2010)
Problems of the notion
Present Day English (PDE)!
vs. !
Earlier English
systematic"
obligatory"
grammatically-required"
"
"
"
unsystematic
nonobligatory
free variation
Problems:
‣ " "dichotomy
‣ " "“present-day bias”
‣ " "operationalization
Hypotheses
I. " If UNSETTLEDNESS holds, expect more variation in
verbs, types, ranks between constructions.
II. " If UNSYSTEMATICITY holds, patterns should
signi$cantly di#er from PDE data.
III." If OBLIGATORINESS holds, expect a decrease in
the verbs that can occur in both constructions.
Data source
Instances of present constructions:
PERIOD
PCEEC
PCEME*
PCMBE
PCHE-TOTAL
CLMET-3.0
E1: 1500–1569
335,006
608,971
—
943,977
—
E2: 1570–1639
1,001,613
674,960
—
1,676,573
—
E3: 1640–1710
611,757
596,053
10,103
1,207,810
—
E4: 1711–1779
—
—
424,211
424,211
12,155,135
E5: 1780–1849
—
—
352,308
352,308
13,268,542
E6: 1850–1920
—
—
314,007
314,007
14,834,182
2,371,920
1,879,984
1,100,629
5,342,430
40,257,859
TOTAL
* Overlap material with PCEEC removed
PCEEC:" Corpus of Early English Correspondence
PCEME: " Corpus of Early Modern English
PCMBE: " Corpus of Modern British English
CLMET: " Corpus of Late Modern English Texts
Taylor et al (2006), Kroch et al (2004, 2010), De Smet (2005), Diller et al (2011)
Measuring lexis-structure interaction
ICE-GB
BNC
PROG
SIMPLE
PROG
SIMPLE
Collostructional analysis
talk
go
try
look
work
sit
wait
do
use
come
run
move
live
deal
walk
watch
wear
write
listen
seel
$ght
be
know
think
see
have
want
mean
need
seem
believe
call
put
remember
$nd
include
agree
base
set
sound
concern
imagine
go
talk
try
look
work
get
happen
plan
play
begin
wait
deal
move
seek
$ght
use
come
take
become
ask
pay
be
have
know
mean
want
seem
see
need
include
do
believe
like
remain
thank
suppose
require
remember
contain
think
appear
wish
‣ Observed vs. expected
‣ Association measure (here: LogL)
Distinctive collexemes
‣ Progressive: motion, action
or process-compatible verbs
‣ Simple: statives, perception
Methodological principles
‣ Beyond raw frequency
‣ Beyond single token/type
‣ General cxn patterns
Stefanowitsch & Gries (2003), Gries & Stefanowitsch (2004)
Progressive: lexis-cxn patterns (PCHE)
1570-1640 (E2)
1641-1710 (E3)
1711-1780 (E4)
PROG
SIMPLE
PROG
SIMPLE
PROG
SIMPLE
go
will
come
make
stir
want
owe
prepare
concern
please
enter
live
remain
provide
accord
$t
send
attend
return
set
write
be
have
do
know
say
pray
think
hope
desire
see
hear
thank
beseech
$nd
doubt
seem
give
mean
wish
trust
speak
go
will
come
want
make
prepare
concern
send
write
endeavour
draw
use
live
please
accord
return
haste
grow
entertain
labour
put
be
have
hope
do
think
pray
desire
believe
seem
know
say
see
hear
thank
suppose
wish
intend
doubt
rest
remember
lie
go
owe
miss
speak
come
write
starve
charm
boil
quarrel
issue
!y
surprise
entertain
invite
pass
die
revive
resent
burst
praise
be
have
do
know
believe
see
pray
hope
seem
give
say
suppose
love
tell
call
think
follow
look
stand
remember
desire
N = 307
N = 68,272
N = 396
N = 52,563
N = 176
UNSYSTEMATIC?
‣ process/
dynamics
‣ stative/
perception
UNSETTLED?
‣ More by rank
‣ Less by verb
types
‣ data sparseness
N = 13,334
grouped by association (G
PCHE, Simple distinctive collexeme analysis (Gries & Stefanowitsch 2004, Gries 2007)
Progressive: lexis-cxn patterns (CLMET)
unchanged changed (cxn preference relative to BNC)
1710-1780 (E4)
1781-1850 (E5)
1851-1920 (E6)
PROG
SIMPLE
PROG
SIMPLE
PROG
SIMPLE
go
owe
come
prepare
write
talk
want
set
wait
endeavour
tri!e
speak
die
hasten
accord
concern
act
return
wander
oblige
try
N = 4,658
be
have
know
seem
hope
believe
do
think
say
suppose
see
mean
fear
appear
$nd
require
need
arise
love
observe
wish
N = 382,238
go
come
get
wait
talk
write
speak
grow
die
owe
advance
make
prepare
pass
approach
begin
act
try
live
accord
gather
N = 7,259
be
have
know
seem
see
believe
hope
do
mean
appear
fear
like
remember
need
love
require
dare
wish
say
wonder
consist
N = 428,019
go
come
wait
talk
get
try
begin
die
write
live
speak
$ght
look
stay
work
move
play
watch
grow
su#er
owe
N = 12,350
be
have
know
seem
see
like
do
believe
mean
appear
suppose
wish
remember
need
love
$nd
remain
dare
belong
consist
round
N = 497,801
UNSETTLED?
‣ More by rank
‣ Less by verb
types
Association scores:
E4~E5: r = .95
E4~E5: r = .99
Rank di"erences:
(Wilcoxon signed-rank)
E4~E5: p = 0.55, n.s.
E5~E6: p = 0.41, n.s.
CLMET-3-1, Simple distinctive collexeme analysis (Gries & Stefanowitsch 2004, Gries 2007)
Progressive: lexis-cxn patterns (CLMET)
unchanged changed (cxn preference relative to BNC)
1710-1780 (E4)
1781-1850 (E5)
1851-1920 (E6)
PROG
SIMPLE
PROG
SIMPLE
PROG
SIMPLE
go
owe
come
prepare
write
talk
want
set
wait
endeavour
tri!e
speak
die
hasten
accord
concern
act
return
wander
oblige
try
N = 4,658
be
have
know
seem
hope
believe
do
think
say
suppose
see
mean
fear
appear
$nd
require
need
arise
love
observe
wish
N = 382,238
go
come
get
wait
talk
write
speak
grow
die
owe
advance
make
prepare
pass
approach
begin
act
try
live
accord
gather
N = 7,259
be
have
know
seem
see
believe
hope
do
mean
appear
fear
like
remember
need
love
require
dare
wish
say
wonder
consist
N = 428,019
go
come
wait
talk
get
try
begin
die
write
live
speak
$ght
look
stay
work
move
play
watch
grow
su#er
owe
N = 12,350
be
have
know
seem
see
like
do
believe
mean
appear
suppose
wish
remember
need
love
$nd
remain
dare
belong
consist
round
N = 497,801
shared types = 32.1 %
shared types = 37.5 %
UNSETTLED?
‣ More by rank
‣ Less by verb
types
OBLIGATORY?
‣ Shared types
increase
‣ More lexical
overlap
shared types = 41.2 %
CLMET-3-1, Simple distinctive collexeme analysis (Gries & Stefanowitsch 2004, Gries 2007)
Subtle changes in stable patterns
PCHE
1500
8.4 %
11.5 %
1570
18.5 %
1640
CLMET
Major #ndings:
‣ General aspectual pattern
from 1500s
‣ Evolving the cxn since 1500
‣ Idiosyncracies, e.g.
• Meaning change
• Word frequency change
• Phraseological change
BNC (100m): 37 %
BNC-BABY (4m): 39 %
BASE (1.7m): 53 %
BROWN (1m): 32 %
VNC: Gries & Hilpert (2008, 2012)
15.0 %
1710
17.3 %
1780
32.1 %
18.2 %
1850
37.5 %
1920
41.2 %
Typical & untypical uses revisited
(1) a."
And why does he sit in her house waiting for her?
■ b." O, wherefore sitt'st thou there?
(2) a."
‘I'll probably go faster with you on my back;
you walk too slow. Come on.’
sitPROG
walkPROG
■ b." ‘Give me your hand,’ said he, ‘my good girl,
you walk too fast.’
(3) a."
‘You jest, of course?’ he said drily.
jestPROG
■ b." You jest, Lydia!
(4) a."
This table of di#erences now represents the
di#erences that there are still remaining.
remainSIMPLE
■ b." Some of it is yet remaining in my hands, for uses:
“it is easy to $nd simple verb forms in contexts in
which Present-Day English would use the
progressive.” (Rissanen 2000:216)
Nehls (1988), Elsness (1994), Rissanen (2000)
“it occurs in stative contexts where
one would not use the progressive
in PDE“ (Strang 1982:429)
Concluding remarks
A. Method & assumption: obligatoriness notion
A.I. "
A.II. "
A.III."
Di%cult to de$ne
Di%cult to measure
Present-day bias:
• "Point of reference (PDE? vs. periods?)
• "Comparing (non-)typicalness (across periods)
B. Phenomenon: Progressive
B.I. "
B.II. "
Constructional perspective: distant-reading
• Stability/evolution (general)
• Idiosyncrasies (instances/types)
Obligatoriness vs. constructionalization
References
Arnaud, René. 1998. The development of the progressive in
19th century English: A quantitative survey. Language
Variation and Change 10(2). 123-152.
Diller, Hans-Jürgen, Hendrik De Smet & Jukka Tyrkkö. 2011.
A European database of descriptors of English
electronic texts. The European English Messenger 19.
21-35.
Elsness, Johan. 1994. On the progression of the
progressive in early Modern English. ICAME Journal 18.
5-25.
Hilpert, Martin. 2006. Distinctive collexeme analysis and
diachrony. Corpus Linguistics and Linguistic Theory 2(2).
doi:10.1515/CLLT.2006.012.
Gries, Stefan Th. 2007. Coll.analysis 3.2. A program for R.
Gries, Stefan Th. & Martin Hilpert. 2008. The identi$cation
of stages in diachronic data: Variability-based
Neighbour Clustering. Corpora 3(1). 59–81.
Gries, Stefan Th. & Martin Hilpert. 2012. Variability-based
neighbour clustering: A bottom-up approach to
periodization in historical linguistics. In Terttu
Nevalainen & Elizabeth Closs Traugott (eds.), The Oxford
handbook on the history of English, 134–144. OUP.
Gries, Stefan Th. & Anatol Stefanowitsch. 2004. Extending
collostructional analysis: A corpus-based perspective on
“alternations.” International Journal of Corpus
Linguistics 9(1). 97-29.
Kranich, Svenja. 2010. The progressive in modern English: A
corpus-based study of grammaticalization and related
changes..Rodopi.
Kroch, Anthony, Beatrice Santorini & Lauren Delfs. 2004.
The Penn-Helsinki Parsed Corpus of Early Modern
English (PCEME). University of Pennsylvania.
Kroch, Anthony, Beatrice Santorini & Lauren Delfs. 2010.
The Penn-Helsinki Parsed Corpus of Modern British
English (PCMBE). University of Pennsylvania.
Nehls, Dietrich. 1988. On the development of the
grammatical category of verbal aspect in English. In
Josef Klegraf & Dietrich Nehls (eds.), Essays on the
English language and applied linguistics on the occasion
of Gerhard Nickel!s 60th birthday, 173-198. Groos.
Núñez-Pertejo, Paloma. 2004. The progressive in the
history of English: with special reference to the Early
Modern English period: A corpus-based study. Lincom.
Rissanen, Matti. 2000. Syntax. In Roger Lass (ed.), The
Cambridge History of the English Language, vol. 3:
1476-1776, 187-331. Cambridge: Cambridge
University Press. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/
CHOL9780521264761.005.
Stefanowitsch, Anatol & Stefan Th. Gries. 2003.
Collostructions: Investigating the interaction of words
and constructions. International Journal of Corpus
Linguistics 8(2). 209-243.
Smet, Hendrik De. 2005. A corpus of Late Modern English
texts. ICAME Journal 29. 69-82.
Strang, Barbara. 1982. Some aspects of the history of the
BE+ING construction. In John M. Anderson (ed.),
Language form and linguistic variation: Papers
dedicated to Angus McIntosh, 427-474. Benjamins.
Taylor, Anne, Arja Nurmi, Anthony Warner, Susan Pintzuk
& Terttu Nevalainen. 2006. Parsed Corpus of Early
English Correspondence, tagged version. (PCEEC).
Compiled by the CEEC project team. York/Helsinki:
University of York/University of Helsinki.