23 宇都宮大学国際学部研究論集 2010, 第29号, 23−34 Intercultural communication and the essence of humour Reimann Andrew Introduction A major focus of studies in intercultural communication deals with developing awareness large amount of insight into understanding a particular language group’s culture, communication styles, strategies and perspectives. of how language and culture function and interact In cross cultural communication, humour is within mutually recognizable, global or universal often used as a powerful speech act for breaking the environments. Cross cultural communication requires ice, building relationships or diffusing a difficult awareness of the assumptions one makes as well as situation. However, universal, the idea of “funny” is awareness of the assumptions and expectations of rarely translatable and is highly personal, context and others. Generally, the basic assumptions essential to culture specific. As a result many jokes, sarcastic or normal communication are underlying and hidden. ironical remarks which may be deeply tied to culture Gudykunst and Kim (2003) note that as a result of are often unperceived, misunderstood or offensive. the covert nature of cultural assumptions they are This presentation will explore examples of humour taken for granted and therefore difficult to explain. in communication and describe ways in which they An observable example of cultural assumptions in may be applied to the EFL classroom, used to bridge practice involves speech acts or communicative cultures and work as a mutual communication strategy. situations and rituals. Speech acts are words or The first section will provide background on various phrases that accompany social situations to facilitate methods, mediums and styles of humour as well as communication. Culturally universal speech acts such several case studies and examples from Japan, U.K. as greeting or thanking, are for the most part easy to Canada and America. The final section will report on a translate and understand, as these exist in most cultures survey of Japanese University student’s perceptions of and languages. However there are many cases in which “funny” by analysing and ranking several examples communicative situations are unique or culture specific of humour from a selection of international film and and are difficult to interpret or transfer. These acts do television media. In conclusion the factors effecting not cross over as there may not be a corresponding the success or failure of humour in interpersonal and situation in the target language or culture. High intercultural settings will be clarified. context languages such as Japanese, which rely heavily on shared knowledge and relationships for Introduction and Relevance to Intercultural communication and negotiation of meaning have Communication many such conventions (Hall 1976). Some examples Although laughter is a universal neurological include the expressions宜しくyoroshiku, お疲れ様 event, what makes us laugh is dependent on many otsukaresama or the procedures of 告白kokuhaku. variables and therefore problematic to quantify, These are strong relationship building devices which generalise or explain in relations to humour. Humour are virtually impossible to translate into English as the may produce other reactions besides laughter and equivalent social situations do not exist. Decoding the laughter may not always be a sign of amusement. The dynamics behind various speech acts can provide a challenge of understanding this has been the endless 24 Reimann Andrew quest of many including jesters, bards, entertainers, can become embarrassing, nonsensical, inappropriate, writers, comedians, sociologists, psychologists, offensive or plain meaningless. As stated by Immanuel philosophers and linguists. There are three basic Kant "Laughter is an effect that arises if a tense theories of humour which highlight the social, expectation is transformed into nothing." Whether psychological and philosophical influences. These or not expectations can be managed or light hearted include; Incongruity theory, Superiority theory and and playful intended utterances are indeed perceived Relief theory. as such, is the key to instilling laughter. Of crucial ・Incongruity theory sees humour as a response importance is the understanding and awareness of or reaction to ambiguity, logical impossibility, context. This affects all other variables and essentially irrelevance, or inappropriateness. Such determines the fate of any humorous endeavours. cognitive shift humour is most associated Awareness of context includes knowing your with philosophers Immanuel Kant and audience, being sensitive to both cultural and personal Søren Kierkegaard. This type of humour is taboos, values and perspectives as well as a basic characteristic of the typical joke formula sense of appropriateness. Timing is a factor of this and which establishes a belief, provides confirming requires understanding of language, degree of shared evidence and then breaks from that train of background knowledge, sensitivity to environment thought by proving that the original premise was and advanced discourse awareness. Intent is also indeed false. crucial as it is often veiled and may well be part of the ・Superiority theory, initially developed by humour. How intent is interpreted will determine the Plato and Aristotle, emphasizes the aggressive reception of humour. A harmless joke, a witty remark, feelings that fuel humour and states that humour an interesting anecdote, a sarcastic comment or a sharp arises from a thrill or euphoria created when insult can originate from the same utterance however feeling supremacy over others. This type of the consequences will vary greatly depending on the humour would likely involve belittling a peer, audience’s reception and interpretation. If one is not in satire, sarcasm empathizing with a fool or jester. tune with the audience or the context, a well intended ・Relief theory, which is most associated with ice breaker could backfire as a searing rebuke. In this Freud, views humour as a basic method regard, especially in cases of cross cultural discourse, for releasing negative energy or feelings of careful consideration of time, place and occasion repression. This would include any type of light conventions is essential. Within these communication hearted and frivolous humour such as sight styles and politeness strategies, social distance, degree gags, clown antics or simple word play. of formality and basic etiquette all play important Trying to determine the essence behind humour has roles. “Men show their character in nothing more vast implications for understanding language and clearly than by what they think laughable.” Johann communication strategies, psychology and cognitive Wolfgang von Goethe. processes as well as social, personal or cultural For humour to be successfully understood and values, beliefs, attitudes and perspectives. The key enjoyed across the cultural divide; not only must to understanding these processes and conventions the purveyor be aware of the previously mentioned on either a personal or social level requires a deeper social conventions, but also adept at communication, consideration of the structure and method of humour in selecting content, sensing an opportunity and spinning a particular context, timing and intent. Most attempts at a good yarn (story). This skill is typically referred to humour fail because one or more of these elements are as a “sense of humour”. A sense of humour also either; misinterpreted, ignored or too ambiguous. As requires being able to forgo logical conclusions and a result an otherwise amusing quip, joke or anecdote abandon your belief system or reliance on common Intercultural communication and the essence of humour 25 sense. Most humour depends on shared knowledge and variety shows, scatological referencing, irony, sarcasm background, however, in intercultural situations this is or the ubiquitous and formulaic logic bending joke. usually the one element that is missing and the cause The following will highlight some relevant examples of most cultural conundrums and misunderstandings. of humour from Japan, Britain, America and Canada. In an analysis of various types of humour, Lee (1994) In Japan humour is governed by strong concluded that humour is the most challenging social conventions especially hierarchy, harmony, form of intercultural communication. Lee stated formality, context, membership and insider/shared that humour is a communicative situation in which perspectives. The protocols created by these social we must make new conventional rules that involve forces characterised as uchi内 (private) /soto外 explicitly discussing the cultural knowledge that we (public) and tatemae建前 (overt,formal)/hone本音 take for granted in the humorous message. However (covert, hidden) make it almost impossible for humour in most instances, this information is not stated as it is to be spontaneous or unexpected in a natural setting. often an essential part of the humour itself. Types of Although Japanese are very humorous, fun loving humour which are deeply rooted in culture, linguistic people and not significantly different from any other knowledge or “insider” perspectives usually fail group in regard to enjoying laughter; the time, place to cross over the cultural abyss. Most universally and acceptability for uncontrolled outbursts is carefully appreciated examples of humour are those that are observed or limited to warai no ba 笑いの場 (laughter simple, highly visual and in tune with the basics places). In Japan humour rarely crosses hierarchical of innate or more primal levels of human common borders nor appears in formal contexts. In many sense. Explaining or translating a spontaneous gag cultures however the opposite is true and humour is or witty remark will rarely result in the desired effect often employed as a politeness strategy, equalizing of laughter. According to E.B. White, "Humour can power distances, reducing social anxiety and generally be dissected as a frog can, but… …the thing dies in assisting communication in tense situations. This the process and the innards are discouraging to any would not work in Japan and would likely have the but the pure scientific mind." With this in mind it opposite effect. In a survey of humorous utterances is possible to explore examples of humour usage in in daily conversations in both English and Japanese, various cultures for the purpose of understanding Takekuro (1997) found that on average there were the conventions, rules, techniques, expectations, four times as many examples of humour in English methods and taboos. This will not necessarily result in conversation than in Japanese. In English 30% of success as a multicultural comedian but will heighten these occurred in formal situations whereas there understanding of communication styles, cultural values were no instances in Japanese. Similarly in the case of and social conventions. Japanese, humour was limited to family and friends There are many types of humour which vary and there were no examples found between new according to media type, audience and social context. acquaintances or strangers. These are very interesting Cultural preferences also determine what methods and revealing results in that they highlight not only are most conducive to laughter and which are not important cultural values and communication styles appreciated or appropriate. According to Mark Twain, but also clearly outline the format and preferences “The humorous story is American, the comic story is for humour and jovial communication. As Japanese English, the witty story is French. The humorous story value harmony, humour being unpredictable, is often depends for its effect upon the manner of the telling; avoided as there is a good possibility that difficult the comic story and the witty story upon the matter.” feelings may arise as a result of a gag gone awry. Some common humour genres include satire, Traditionally, although this is changing, many felt wordplay, sight gags, slapstick, stand up comedy or that public laughter was shameful. Women tended 26 Reimann Andrew to cover their mouths when laughing and men tried Japan is a type of word play called洒落 (sha-re) this to avoid displaying such emotion all together. In the is a form of punning which utilizes pivot words掛 Chrysanthemum and the Sword, Ruth Benedict (1946) 詞 (kakekotoba) or homonyms to make amusing or outlines a hierarchy of emotions in Japan which ranks ambiguous utterances. In a study of media broadcasts, laughter below anger and grief, stipulating that any Nagashima (1999) found that more than ten percent such display is best controlled. This lends credence to of all advertisements and announcements employed a the proverb顔で笑って心で泣いて“kao de waratte form of Sha-re to emphasise their message. There are kokoro de naite.” Similarly, Lafcadio Hearn (1904) linguistic, religious and social reasons why this form in his “Glimpses of Unfamiliar Japan” called this of humour is most widely used. The Japanese language nonverbal expression the “Japanese Smile” and consists of only about 21 phonemes (compared to suggested that smiling was the preferred strategy 41 in English) resulting in an unusual number of for dealing with shame, grief or anger and diffusing homonyms, creating a natural tendency for word play difficult situations and was rarely seen to signify or double entendre. From Shinto, many Japanese actual laughter or amusement. Considering these believe that words possess spirit and soul, this is cultural parameters, raises the question as to the exact reflected in literary styles as俳句haiku and短歌tanka consistency and nature of Japanese humour. and allows for shar-re style humour to be respected For the most part Japanese humour is extremely in the same vein. In social situations, sha-re follows complex, multi dimensional and carefully planned, the conventions of maintaining harmony in that the making it more of an art form than a spontaneous audience always has a way out. Rather than being vehicle for diversion. In a series of articles in the direct and obnoxious like many western jokes, sha- Japan Times, Pulvers (2009) describes Japanese re offers an amusing or intellectual observation which humour as “harmless fun” seldom at the expense is purposefully ambiguous and can either be taken at of others, refraining from exploiting or exposing face value, ignoring the pun, or laughed at depending taboos, the sacred or the profane. Dirty jokes or on the context or feeling of the audience. Considering 下ネタ (shimoneta) are usually avoided and most Japanese social conventions this makes sha-re a highly preferences involve a “comedy of manners” appropriate medium for humour. This also helps to or social indiscretions traditionally involving an clarify why many types of foreign comedy or humour unassuming country yokel おのぼりさん・尾登 fail in Japan or are met with a forced giggle and the さん (onoborisan) adapting to the sophistication of muttering of “Ah American Joke!” the city. This is typical of漫才 (manzai), a form of In comparison American humour tends to be more stand up comedy and ワイドwaido or variety shows individualistic and places emphasis on performance, which employ the standard routine of ぼけboke (the overstating, exaggerating through hyperbole or silly fool) and tsukomi つっこみ (the sharp man). Such antics (Rourke, 1931). American’s are not usually formulas are characteristic of popular Manzaiコン embarrassed by their emotions and as a result do ビ (combinations) such asダウンタウン Downtown, not mind laughing loudly or drawing attention to Sanma Akashiya 明石家さんま, Beat Takeshi ビー themselves. Major influences on this type of humour トたけし or Tamori タモリ. This type of お笑い come from Jewish communities and culture. Including (owarai) humour is highly context and culture specific the comedy styles of The Three Stooges, The Marx and any outsider, unfamiliar with the subtleties of Brothers, Rodney Dangerfield, Jackie Mason, Woody Japanese society or common sense, would be totally Allen, Mel Brooks, Jerry Seinfeld, Jon Stewart, Lewis oblivious to the finer meanings and nuances which Black and Larry David. This type of humour tends make it interesting or laughable. In public or less to be more open and accessible as it often ignores scripted venues, the most popular form of humour in hierarchy, relationships and formality, however Intercultural communication and the essence of humour 27 satirizing such sacred cows as the social system, a hybrid of British and American styles. The default patriotism or national security through sarcasm and humour or communication strategy for most Canadians irony, is less typical (Pegg, 2007). An exception is satire. Whether it is the vast geographic distances, to this is Saturday Night Live, one of the longest linguistic diversity or multicultural differences, as running comedy shows in America (1975-present). a shared identity, Canadians like to make fun of The show is infamous for pushing the taboo envelope everything as a coping mechanism, and nothing and and has been the starting point for some of comedies no one is off limits. This often causes problems in biggest names including: Eddie Murphy, Chevy the international sphere as Canada has been rebuked Chase, Chris Rock, Ben Stiller, Billy Crystal, Mike in the past for ridiculing dignitaries and heads of Myers and Dan Aykroyd. In general less high brow, state. George W. Bush refused to be interviewed observational techniques like those used by stand up by the CBC for the duration of his presidency after comedians are preferred as is slapstick or physical, being duped into calling the Prime Minister of vaudevillian, comedy. If an American were to make Canada “Jean Poutine” by a comedian posing as a a sarcastic or ironic remark and however obvious the reporter in 2000. Prime Minister Pierre Trudeau was erroneous intention, it would usually be followed by also criticised for dancing a pirouette and making the qualifying remark; “Just kidding!” questionable gestures while walking behind an British humour in contrast tends to be more oblivious Queen Elizabeth II in Buckingham Palace. deeply ingrained in language, culture and society and Canadian variety or comedy shows similarly base is highly ironic and satirical in nature. The stereotype their routine around tricking people, catching someone of dry British wit, characteristic of Oscar Wilde, off guard or ridiculing the latest trend or headline. Monty Python, Benny Hill, The Two Ronnies or Ricky Examples of this include the hidden camera style Just Gervais is often difficult to comprehend as it is deeply for Laughs in which unsuspecting people are filmed in veiled in rhetoric and innuendo. Contrary to the stiff embarrassing encounters or Rick Mercer’s “Talking upper lip image portrayed by British popular comedy, to Americans”, where the stereotype of American Britain’s do indeed enjoy slapstick and silliness. ignorance is tested through random interviews about Both Benny Hill and John Cleese are notorious for fake headlines and nonsensical Canadian trivia. their zaniness and wild escapades. Mr. Bean one of Following the British models, farcical shows such the most successful and internationally distributed as Wayne and Shuster (1940-1990) have been quite examples of British comedy represents a balance of successful. The duo who appeared on the Ed Sullivan simple visual gags and antics with deeper British wit show a record 67 times were even ranked as the best and sarcasm. According to Rowan Atkinson (Mr. comedy team in America in the 1960’s. This type Bean), an object or a person can become funny in of humour was very sophisticated yet silly and light three different ways. By behaving in an unusual way, hearted. They were dubbed “the harbingers of literate by being in an unusual place or by being the wrong slapstick” and appealed to a wide range of Americans size. This is a basic and universally popular formula becoming hugely popular on both sides of the border. for typical sight gags or slap stick humour. This type Canadian humour can also be very dark and cynical to of humour is highly adaptable across cultures provided the extent that it is often difficult to distinguish serious that the objects, artefacts and instruments in question comments from sarcastic wit. This can be a difficult are familiar enough to be considered part of common cultural obstacle as such utterances are often perceived sense or logical in their representation, appearance and as rude, crass or insincere. Self deprecation is also application. a standard Canadian form of humour, again being Canadian humour, perhaps as a result of its the cold and barren neighbour of the most powerful colonial history and geographic location, represents country in the world, Canada’s only defence is to 28 Reimann Andrew laugh at itself. As a model for humour in intercultural imagine Mr. Bean acting out a similar scenario, and communication, this Canadian tradition may well be most likely this visual quality of the joke boosts its most appropriate, as the best laugh is always at your international appeal. Subsequent results from this own expense. study also revealed several strong cultural differences In 2000 a survey was conducted in Britain to determine what serves as a model for intercultural in humour. Some of the most notable findings are as follows: humour. As the joke is the most concrete and formulaic ・Many European countries, such as France, of the many types and varieties of humour, jokes were Denmark and Belgium, displayed a penchant collected from around the world and recorded into the for off-beat surreal humour as well as jokes largest such database ever complied. Over 350,000 that involved making light of topics that make people from 70 countries logged onto a special people feel anxious, such as death, illness and website, and provided data that was used to explore marriage. various aspects of the psychology of humour. People ・People from the Republic of Ireland, the UK, from all nationalities were invited to judge jokes as Australia and New Zealand most enjoyed jokes well as contribute their own. This research, carried involving word plays. out by psychologist Dr. Richard Wiseman, from the ・Americans and Canadians preferred jokes where University of Hertfordshire, through a “LaughLab” there was a strong sense of superiority -- either website, attracted more than 40,000 jokes and almost because a character looks stupid or is made to two million ratings. These were then ranked and look stupid by someone else. categorised in order to determine the funniest and most Wiseman concluded that: universal joke. "These results are really interesting. It The final result for the most successful and universally suggests that people from different parts funny joke is as follows: of the world have fundamentally different Two hunters are out in the woods when senses of humour. Humour is vital to one of them collapses. He doesn't seem to communication and the more we understand be breathing and his eyes are glazed. The about how people's culture and background other guy takes out his phone and calls the affect their sense of humour, the more we emergency services. will be able to communicate effectively. He gasps: "My friend is dead! What can I Also, we find jokes funny for lots of do?" The operator says: "Calm down, I can different reasons. They sometimes make us help. First, let's make sure he's dead." There feel superior to others, reduce the emotional is a silence, then a gunshot is heard. Back on impact of anxiety-provoking situations the phone, the guy says: "OK, now what?" or surprise us because of some kind of Whether or not this example works as a model for incongruity. The hunters joke contained all intercultural humour, the hunter joke was most highly three elements." rated by a variety of international participants. This may be due to the fact that the situation and subject Wiseman, R. (2002) Survey matter depicted are more or less universal and not culture specific. The pattern, rhythm and set up are Considering the information and cultural variations very straight forward and typical of this type of in humour described in this report, the subsequent formula joke. The break or gap in logic also does investigation will propose to answer the following not depend on shared knowledge nor is it language, questions by surveying groups of university students context or culture specific. One could perhaps easily regarding their humour preferences while having 29 Intercultural communication and the essence of humour them view and rank several examples of international however males indicated that they enjoyed foreign humour. humour more than females. Scores for general humour Questions to be considered in analysis include: preferences were not significantly different with all 1. What is humour? groups indicating that they enjoyed humour of one ・Are there any common or universal elements form or another. present in something funny? Procedure and Method ・Are there any essential elements required for Subjects viewed 12 examples of humorous media something to be considered funny? and completed a questionnaire indicating the degree ・Can the elements which constitute funniness be of funniness, their understanding, and any significant separated or isolated? reasons. Each example ranged in length from 30 2. What psychological or cognitive factors are seconds to 4 minutes and consisted of content from involved in finding something funny? US, UK, Japan and Canada. Some examples which 3. How is humour related to laughter? What other included a Japanese theme were used in order to help verbal or non verbal reactions are stimulated? with context and understanding (Jack Ass, Austin 4. What elements allow humour to transcend cross Powers, Budweiser “Wasabi”). cultural boundaries? Results and Discussion Subjects From analysis of the survey data, several trends In order to analyse individual and cultural become clear (Table 2 and 3). ・Japanese Language media had the highest differences in humour appreciation and comprehension, several groups of university students scores. were shown international samples of comedy and ・Women tended to prefer more verbal and humorous media. Four intact groups were surveyed multidimensional humour such as Monty and data was collected from a total of 110 subjects Python. ・Men preferred visual or non verbal humour (Mr. (Table 1.). The groups consisted of English, Economics, Science and Education majors. Age ranges Bean or Jack Ass) ・There were significant variations within groups were 18-25 and gender distribution was as follows; 72 male and 38 female. Most subjects were second indicating strong individual differences. year university students and approximately 25% The lowest ranked examples contained either had some form of experience abroad ranging from 1 culture specific themes and content or confusing and week to 1 year. Female subjects tended to have had unfamiliar language. There appears also to be a direct more experience travelling abroad than male subjects correlation between comprehension and ability to Table 1. Subject Demographics Group Gender M Economics Foreign Experience F Mean Enjoy Humour Std. D. Mean Std. D. Enjoy Foreign Humour Mean Std. D. 5 4 2.04 1.035 4.72 0.894 3.41 1.87 English 11 16 1.33 1.109 4.22 0.891 3.22 1.577 Education 26 15 1.37 0.538 4.41 0.728 3.49 1.143 Science 30 4 1.7 1 3.59 1.499 2.65 1.323 Male 72 n/a 1.43 0.852 4.11 1.205 3.2917 1.398 Female n/a 38 1.64 0.986 4.07 0.983 2.84 1.288 1.45 0.871 4.11 1.118 3.1589 3.158 Total Avg. 110 30 Reimann Andrew understand humour and score. This is not surprising Japanese language media was most highly ranked however further analysis would be necessary to supports this however the degree to which language determine whether this is in fact a trend or if the affects humour was not directly measured. subjects are responding to expectations. The fact that Table 2. Group Ranking of Humour (0 not funny-5 very funny) Group Economics English Education Science Male Female Total Group Borat 0.82 2.11 1.41 1.44 1.48 1.71 1.58 Tampopo Economics English Education Science Male Female Total 2.61 2.62 2.43 2.38 2.54 2.38 2.49 Jack Ass 3.41 2.55 3.9 3.3 3.52 2.89 3.36 U.S. CM 1.56 1.74 2.24 1.85 2.04 1.87 2.03 Japan CM 2.36 3.48 4.24 2.7 3.43 3.58 3.54 Mr. Bean 3.04 3.92 3.9 3.29 3.84 3.33 3.71 Sushi 3.33 4.33 3.95 4.23 4.01 4.38 4.15 Austin Powers 1.73 2.25 2.1 2.03 2.18 1.97 2.11 Wayne & Shuster 2.23 n/a 2.34 1.91 2.13 2.33 2.17 Shimura Ken Monty Python 2.02 n/a 4 2.56 3.21 3.71 3.36 Simpsons 1.55 n/a 4.22 2.5 3.31 3.57 3.37 1.38 n/a 2.44 1.94 2.19 2.33 2.22 Table 3. Humour Comprehension Could you understand the humour? Group Borat Jack Ass Yes No Yes No Economics 6.0% 94.0% 96.0% 4.0% English 55.6% 44.4% 81.5% 18.5% Education 18.9% 81.1% 97.3% 2.7% Science 41.2% 58.8% 82.4% 17.6% Male 31.9% 68.1% 87.5% 12.5% Female 37.1% 62.9% 82.9% 17.1% Total 33.6% 66.4% 86.0% 14.0% Could you understand the humour? Group Tampopo Austin Powers Economics English Education Science Male Female Total Yes 89% 96.3% 75.7% 85.3% 83.3% 88.6% 85.0% No 11% 3.7% 24.3% 14.7% 16.7% 11.4% 15.0% Yes 17% 77.8% 62.2% 88.2% 79.2% 65.7% 74.8% No 93% 22.2% 37.8% 11.8% 20.8% 34.3% 25.2% U.S. CM Japan CM Mr. Bean Yes No Yes No Yes No 48.0% 52.0% 64.0% 36.0% 93.0% 7.0% 44.4% 55.6% 88.9% 11.1% 100.0% 0.0% 59.5% 40.5% 100.0% 0.0% 89.2% 10.8% 73.5% 26.5% 91.2% 8.8% 100.0% 0.0% 62.5% 37.5% 93.1% 6.9% 100.0% 0.0% 51.4% 48.6% 94.3% 5.7% 88.6% 11.4% 58.9% 41.1% 93.5% 6.5% 96.3% 3.7% Wayne & Shuster Yes No 94% 6% n/a n/a 78.4% 21.6% 76.5% 23.5% 80.3% 19.7% 73.7% 26.3% 78.8% 21.3% Shimura Ken Monty Python Yes 62% n/a 94.6% 88.2% 86.9% 94.7% 88.8% Yes 69% n/a 97.3% 82.4% 88.5% 94.7% 90.0% No 38% n/a 5.4% 11.8% 13.1% 5.3% 11.3% No 31% n/a 2.7% 17.6% 11.5% 5.3% 11.0% Sushi Yes No 100.0% 0.0% 100.0% 0.0% 100.0% 0.0% 97.1% 2.9% 100.0% 0.0% 97.1% 2.9% 99.1% 0.9% Simpsons Yes 34% n/a 54.1% 35.3% 47.5% 36.8% 45.0% No 66% n/a 45.9% 64.7% 52.5% 23.2% 55.0% 31 Intercultural communication and the essence of humour Table 4. Ranking by Gender Rank 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 Total Sushi Mr. Bean Japan CM Shimura Ken Jack Ass Monty Python Tampopo Simpsons Wayne & Shuster Austin Powers U.S. CM Borat Male Sushi Mr. Bean Jack Ass Japan CM Shimura Ken Monty Python Tampopo Simpsons Austin Powers Wayne & Shuster U.S. CM Borat Conclusion Humour appreciation varies greatly by personality, gender, experience and culture. Gender Female Sushi Monty Python Japan CM Shimura Ken Mr. Bean Jack Ass Tampopo Wayne & Shuster Simpsons Austin Powers U.S. CM Borat and developing an ability to engage successfully in intercultural communication as well as foster higher levels of cultural awareness both locally and globally. differences suggest that women tend to have a heightened sensitivity and preference for more References multilevel and complex humour (Table 4.). This is Benedict, R. (94). The Chrysanthemum and the further supported by neurological research which has found that women access the prefrontal cortex, the section of the brain associated with language and memory, which is most affected while experiencing humour or laughter more often than men (Reiss 2005). Cultural differences, language and lack of shared Sword. Tokyo: Charles E. Tuttle. Cortazzi, H. (200). A Japanese sense of humor? The Japan Times. Tuesday, March 2, 200. Davis, J.M. (200) Understanding Humour in Japan. Wayne state University Press. Dillon, T. (2000). When intercultural humor is no knowledge are, not surprisingly, the greatest barriers joke. The Japan Times. Wednesday, February to comprehension and enjoyment. Types of humour th, 2000. which do not require specialised language, background Gudykunst,W. B., Kim, Y. Y. (2003). Communicating knowledge or culture specific themes and artefacts with strangers: An approach to intercultural are more successful over a diverse demographic. This communication (th ed.). New York: McGraw explains why Borat’s antics or the Simpson’s ironies Hill. are lost on many international audiences. Nonverbal Hall, E. (9). Hidden Differences, Doing Business and context general examples like Mr. Bean or Jack with the Japanese. New York, Anchor Press/ Ass are also as a result more successful with a wider Double Day and Company. range of audiences. From this investigation it becomes clear that humour varies greatly not only across cultures but on all levels of individual and social Hearn, L. (94). Glimpses of Unfamiliar Japan. Kessinger Publishing. Lee, W. S. (994). Communication about humor difference. As there is no salient reasoning for this, the as procedural competence in intercultural only conclusion possible in considering this research encounters. In L. A. Samovar & R. E. Porter would suggest that a higher level of awareness and (Eds.), Intercultural communication: A reader (th sensitivity to the causes, effects and processes involved ed., pp. 33-32). Belmont, CA: Wadsworth. in humour are essential for understanding differences Nagashima (999). Sha-re, Dajare-gaku Koto Hajime. 32 Reimann Andrew Waraigaku Kenkyu, , Osaka: JSLHS, (999). Reiss, A. etal. (2005). Sex differences in brain Pegg, S. (200). What are you laughing at? The activation elicited by humor. Proc Natl Acad Sci Guardian, Saturday, February 0, 200. U S A.; 02 (45): 49-50 Pulvers, R. (2009). Humour maybe universal, but Rourke, C. (93). American Humor: A Study of the Japan’s is largely smut free. Counterpoint, The National Character. Japan Times. Sunday, August 9th, 2009. Takekuro, M. (99). Conversational Jokes as a Pulvers, R. (2009). Comparing and Contrasting politeness strategy: Observations from English to plumb the heights of Japanese humor. and Japanese. Japan Women’s University, Counterpoint, The Japan Times. Sunday, August Tokyo, Journal of the Graduate School of 2nd, 2009. Humanities 4 (99). Pulvers, R. (2009). Do the Japanese have a sense of Wiseman, R. (2002). Laughlab: The Scientific Search humor? Counterpoint, The Japan Times. Sunday, for the World's Funniest Joke. Random House: November 5 , 2000. London, UK. th Appendix Humour Analysis Survey 性別 男 女 外国の経験 なし 1週間(+) 1か月(+) 1年(+) ユーモアとお笑いは好きですか? 0 1 2 3 4 5 外国のユーモアは好きですか? 0 1 2 3 4 5 1) Borat - Guide to America (8:08) (UK) 全く面白くない 0 普通 1 このユーモアを理解できますか? 2) はい いいえ とても面白い 5 4 とても面白い 5 4 とても面白い 5 4 とても面白い 5 4 とても面白い 5 いいえの場合はなぜ: 2 はい いいえ 3 いいえの場合はなぜ: Bud “Wasabi!” Commercial (0:30) (U.S.) 全く面白くない 0 普通 1 このユーモアを理解できますか? 2 はい いいえ 3 いいえの場合はなぜ: Japanese CM “Lost Voice” (1:37) (Japan) 全く面白くない 0 普通 1 このユーモアを理解できますか? 5) 4 普通 1 このユーモアを理解できますか? 4) 3 Jack Ass – Japan (2:58) (U.S.) 全く面白くない 0 3) 2 2 はい いいえ 3 いいえの場合はなぜ: Mr Bean – Merry Christmas (25:43) (18:37- 21:54) (UK) 全く面白くない 0 このユーモアを理解できますか? 普通 1 2 はい いいえ 3 いいえの場合はなぜ: 33 Intercultural communication and the essence of humour 6) Foreigners Guide to Sushi (8:08) (Japan) 全く面白くない 0 普通 1 このユーモアを理解できますか? 7) はい いいえ 4 とても面白い 5 4 とても面白い 5 4 とても面白い 5 4 とても面白い 5 4 とても面白い 5 4 とても面白い 5 4 とても面白い 5 いいえの場合はなぜ: 普通 1 このユーモアを理解できますか? 2 はい いいえ 3 いいえの場合はなぜ: Austin Powers – Sumo (16:07) (0:00 – 4:14) (U.S.) 全く面白くない 0 普通 1 このユーモアを理解できますか? 9) 3 Tampopo-Spaghetti Scene (6:32) (Japan) 全く面白くない 0 8) 2 2 はい いいえ 3 いいえの場合はなぜ: Wayne & Shuster – Economy Class vs. First Class (6:47) (Canada) 全く面白くない 0 普通 1 このユーモアを理解できますか? 2 はい いいえ 3 いいえの場合はなぜ: 10) Shimura Ken 1985 (1:23) (Japan) 全く面白くない 0 普通 1 このユーモアを理解できますか? 2 はい いいえ 3 いいえの場合はなぜ: 11) Monty Python “Spam” (3:23) (UK) 全く面白くない 0 普通 1 このユーモアを理解できますか? 2 はい いいえ 3 いいえの場合はなぜ: 12) Simpsons Canada (2:19) (U.S.) 全く面白くない 0 このユーモアを理解できますか? 普通 1 2 はい いいえ 3 いいえの場合はなぜ: 34 Reimann Andrew 異文化コミュニケーションにおけるユーモアの役割 ライマン アンドリュー 要約 異文化コミュニケーションでは、ユーモアが緊張をほぐしたり、気まずい状況を打開したりするため の有力な発話行為として用いられる。しかし、 「おかしい」という概念は決して翻訳できるものではなく、 非常に個人的で、文脈や文化に規定される。その結果、様々なジョークや、皮肉・反語をこめたコメン トなどは文化と深い関係性を持っている可能性がある。ゆえに、気づかれずにたびたび誤解されたり侮 辱的になったりする。本論文では、コミュニケーションに現れるユーモアの例を調べ、EFL の授業に利 用し、文化間の橋渡しを通して相互コミュニケーションの方策として使用できるような様々な方法を説 明する。まず、様々なユーモアの方法・媒体・スタイルの背景知識を述べ、日本・イギリス・カナダ・ アメリカのケーススタディやユーモアの例を紹介する。その後で、日本の大学生の「おかしい」につい ての認識調査を報告する。この調査は、数々の国際映画とテレビ・メディアの中からユーモアの例を分 析して序列をつける方法で行った。結びとして、個人間及び異文化間コミュニケーションの場において ユーモアがうまく働くか否かを決定付ける要素を明らかにする。なお,ユーモアの基礎理論には社会的・ 心理学・哲学的な影響が見られ、カントなどの不調和理論、プラトンなどの優越理論、フロイトの解放 理論などがある。 (2009 年 11 月 4 日受理)
© Copyright 2024 Paperzz